Synaptic Encoding of Time in Working Memory

  1. School of Natural Sciences, Institute for Advanced Study, Princeton, United States
  2. Sorbonne Université, INSERM, CNRS, Institut de la Vision, Paris, France
  3. Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique, Paris, France
  4. Department of Brain Sciences, Weizmann Institute of Science, Rehovot, Israel

Peer review process

Not revised: This Reviewed Preprint includes the authors’ original preprint (without revision), an eLife assessment, public reviews, and a provisional response from the authors.

Read more about eLife’s peer review process.

Editors

  • Reviewing Editor
    Tatyana Sharpee
    Salk Institute for Biological Studies, La Jolla, United States of America
  • Senior Editor
    Panayiota Poirazi
    FORTH Institute of Molecular Biology and Biotechnology, Heraklion, Greece

Reviewer #1 (Public review):

Summary:

The issue of how the brain can maintain the serial order of presented items in working memory is a major unsolved question in cognitive neuroscience. It has been proposed that this serial order maintenance could be achieved thanks to periodic reactivations of different presented items at different phases of an oscillation, but the mechanisms by which this could be achieved by brain networks, as well as the mechanisms of read-out, are still unclear. In an influential 2008 paper, the authors have proposed a mechanism by which a recurrent network of neurons could maintain multiple items in working memory, thanks to `population spikes' of populations of neurons encoding for the different items, occurring at alternating times. These population spikes occur in a specific regime of the network and are a result of synaptic facilitation, an experimentally observed type of synaptic short-term dynamics with time scales of order hundreds of ms.

In the present manuscript, the authors extend their model to include another type of experimentally observed short-term synaptic plasticity termed synaptic augmentation, which operates on longer time scales on the order of 10s. They show that while a network without augmentation loses information about serial order, augmentation provides a mechanism by which this order can be maintained in memory thanks to a temporal gradient of synaptic efficacies. The order can then be read out using a read-out network whose synapses are also endowed with synaptic augmentation. Interestingly, the read-out speed can be regulated using background inputs.

Strengths:

This is an elegant solution to the problem of serial order maintenance that only relies on experimentally observed features of synapses. The model is consistent with a number of experimental observations in humans and monkeys. The paper will be of interest to a broad readership, and I believe it will have a strong impact on the field.

Weaknesses:

(1) The network they propose is extremely simple. This simplicity has pros and cons: on the one hand, it is nice to see the basic phenomenon exposed in the simplest possible setting. On the other hand, it would also be reassuring to check that the mechanism is robust when implemented in a more realistic setting, using, for instance, a network of spiking neurons similar to the one they used in the 2008 paper. The more noisy and heterogeneous the setting, the better.

(2) One major issue with the population spike scenario is that (to my knowledge) there is no evidence that these highly synchronized events occur in delay periods of working memory experiments. It seems that highly synchronized population spikes would imply (a) a strong regularity of spike trains of neurons, at odds with what is typically observed in vivo (b) high synchronization of neurons encoding for the same item (and also of different items in situations where multiple items have to be held in working memory), also at odds with in vivo recordings that typically indicate weak synchronization at best. It would be nice if the authors at least mention this issue, and speculate on what could possibly bridge the gap between their highly regular and synchronized network, and brain networks that seem to lie at the opposite extreme (highly irregular and weakly synchronized). Of course, if they can demonstrate using a spiking network simulation that they can bridge the gap, even better.

Reviewer #2 (Public review):

In this manuscript, the authors present a model to explain how working memory (WM) encodes both existence and timing simultaneously using transient synaptic augmentation. A simple yet intriguing idea.

The model presented here has the potential to explain what previous theories like 'active maintenance via attractors' and 'liquid state machine' do not, and describe how novel sequences are immediately stored in WM. Altogether, the topic is of great interest to those studying higher cognitive processes, and the conclusions the authors draw are certainly thought-provoking from an experimental perspective. However, several questions remain that need to be addressed.

The study relates to the well-known computational theory for working memory, which suggests short-term synaptic facilitation is required to maintain working memory, but doesn't rely on persistent spiking. This previous theory appears similar to the proposed theory, except for the change from facilitation to augmentation. A more detailed explanation of why the authors use augmentation instead of facilitation in this paper is warranted: is the facilitation too short to explain the whole process of WM? Can the theory with synaptic facilitation also explain the immediate storage of novel sequences in WM?

In Figure 1, the authors mention that synaptic augmentation leads to an increased firing rate even after stimulus presentation. It would be good to determine, perhaps, what the lowest threshold is to see the encoding of a WM task, and whether that is biologically plausible.

In the middle panel of Figure 4, after 15-16 sec, when the neuronal population prioritizes with the second retro-cue, although the second retro-cue item's synaptic spike dominates, why is the augmentation for the first retro-cue item higher than the second-cue augmentation until the 20 sec?

Author response:

Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

(1) The network they propose is extremely simple. This simplicity has pros and cons: on the one hand, it is nice to see the basic phenomenon exposed in the simplest possible setting. On the other hand, it would also be reassuring to check that the mechanism is robust when implemented in a more realistic setting, using, for instance, a network of spiking neurons similar to the one they used in the 2008 paper. The more noisy and heterogeneous the setting, the better.

The choice of a minimal model to illustrate our hypothesis is deliberate. Our main goal was to suggest a physiologically-grounded mechanism to rapidly encode temporally-structured information (i.e., sequences of stimuli) in Working Memory, where none was available before. Indeed, as discussed in the manuscript, previous proposals were unsatisfactory in several respects. In view of our main goal, we believe that a spiking implementation is beyond the scope of the present work.

We would like to note that the mechanism originally proposed in Mongillo et al. (2008), has been repeatedly implemented, by many different groups, in various spiking network models with different levels of biological realism (see, e.g., Lundquivst et al. (2016), for an especially ‘detailed’ implementation) and, in all cases, the relevant dynamics has been observed. We take this as an indication of ‘robustness’; the relevant network dynamics doesn’t critically depend on many implementation details and, importantly, this dynamics is qualitatively captured by a simple rate model (see, e.g., Mi et al. (2017)).

In the present work, we make a relatively ‘minor’ (from a dynamical point of view) extension of the original model, i.e., we just add augmentation. Accordingly, we are fairly confident that a set of parameters for the augmentation dynamics can be found such that the spiking network behaves, qualitatively, as the rate model. A meaningful study, in our opinion, then would require extensively testing the (large) parameters’ space (different models of augmentation?) to see how the network behavior compares with the relevant experimental observations (which ones? behavioral? physiological?). As said above, we believe that this is beyond the scope of the present work.

This being said, we definitely agree with the reviewer that not presenting a spiking implementation is a limitation of the present work. We will clearly acknowledge, and discuss, this limitation in the revised version.

(2) One major issue with the population spike scenario is that (to my knowledge) there is no evidence that these highly synchronized events occur in delay periods of working memory experiments. It seems that highly synchronized population spikes would imply (a) a strong regularity of spike trains of neurons, at odds with what is typically observed in vivo (b) high synchronization of neurons encoding for the same item (and also of different items in situations where multiple items have to be held in working memory), also at odds with in vivo recordings that typically indicate weak synchronization at best. It would be nice if the authors at least mention this issue, and speculate on what could possibly bridge the gap between their highly regular and synchronized network, and brain networks that seem to lie at the opposite extreme (highly irregular and weakly synchronized). Of course, if they can demonstrate using a spiking network simulation that they can bridge the gap, even better.

Direct experimental evidence (in monkeys) in support of the existence of highly synchronized events -- to be identified with the ‘population spikes’ of our model -- during the delay period of a memory task is available in the literature and we have cited it, i.e., Panichello et al. (2024). In the revised version, we will provide an explicit discussion of the results of Panichello et al. (2024) and how these results directly relate to our model. After submission, we became aware of another experimental study (in humans) specifically dealing with sequence memory, i.e., Liebe et al. (2025). Their results, again, are fully consistent with our model. We will also provide an explicit discussion of these results in the revised version.

We note that there is no fundamental contradiction between highly synchronized events in ‘small’ neural populations (e.g., a cell assembly) on one hand, and temporally irregular (i.e., Poisson-like) spiking at the single-neuron level and weakly synchronized activity at the network level, on the other hand. This was already illustrated in our original publication, i.e., Mongillo et al. (2008) (see, in particular, Fig. S2).

We further note that the mechanism we propose to encode temporal order -- a temporal gradient in the synaptic efficacies brought about by synaptic augmentation -- would also work if the memory of the items is maintained by ‘tonic’ persistent activity (i.e., without highly synchronized events), provided this activity occurs at suitably low rates such as to prevent the saturation of the synaptic augmentation.

We will include a detailed discussion of these points in the revised version.

Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

The study relates to the well-known computational theory for working memory, which suggests short-term synaptic facilitation is required to maintain working memory, but doesn't rely on persistent spiking. This previous theory appears similar to the proposed theory, except for the change from facilitation to augmentation. A more detailed explanation of why the authors use augmentation instead of facilitation in this paper is warranted: is the facilitation too short to explain the whole process of WM? Can the theory with synaptic facilitation also explain the immediate storage of novel sequences in WM?

In the model, synaptic dynamics displays both short-term facilitation and augmentation (and shortterm depression). Indeed, synaptic facilitation, alone, would be too short-lived to encode novel sequences. This is illustrated in Fig. 1B. We will provide a more detailed discussion of this point in the revised version.

In Figure 1, the authors mention that synaptic augmentation leads to an increased firing rate even after stimulus presentation. It would be good to determine, perhaps, what the lowest threshold is to see the encoding of a WM task, and whether that is biologically plausible.

We believe that this comment is related to the above point. The reviewer is correct; augmentation alone would require fairly long stimulus presentations to encode an item in WM. ‘Fast’ encoding, indeed, is guaranteed by the presence of short-term facilitation. We will emphasize this important point in the revised version.

In the middle panel of Figure 4, after 15-16 sec, when the neuronal population prioritizes with the second retro-cue, although the second retro-cue item's synaptic spike dominates, why is the augmentation for the first retro-cue item higher than the second-cue augmentation until the 20 sec?

This is because of the slow build-up and slow decay of the augmentation. When the second item is prioritized, and the corresponding neuronal population re-activates, its augmentation level starts to increase. At the same time, as the first item is now de-prioritized and the corresponding neuronal population is now silent, its augmentation level starts to decrease. Because of the ‘slowness’ of both processes (i.e., augmentation build-up and decay), it takes about 5 seconds for the augmentation level of the second item to overcome the augmentation level of the first item.

We note that the slow time scales of the augmentation dynamics, consistently with experimental observations, are necessary for our mechanism to work.

  1. Howard Hughes Medical Institute
  2. Wellcome Trust
  3. Max-Planck-Gesellschaft
  4. Knut and Alice Wallenberg Foundation