Abstract
Guilt and shame are key moral emotions that influence mental health and regulate social behavior. Although prior research has examined the psychological and neural correlates of these emotions, the cognitive antecedents trigger them, as well as their transformation into social behavior, remain insufficiently understood. In this study, we developed a novel task to investigate how two crucial cognitive antecedents, harm and responsibility, elicit guilt and shame, and how these emotions subsequently drive compensatory behavior, by combining functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) with computational modeling. Behaviorally, we found that harm had a stronger impact on guilt than on shame, whereas responsibility had a stronger impact on shame than guilt, which support the functionalist theory of emotion. Moreover, compared to shame, guilt exerted a greater effect on compensation. Computational modeling results indicated that individuals integrate harm and responsibility in the form of a quotient, aligning with the phenomenon of responsibility diffusion. The fMRI results revealed that brain regions associated with inequity represenation (posterior insula) and value computation (striatum) encode this integrated measure. Furthermore, individual differences in responsibility-driven shame sensitivity were associated with activity in theory-of-mind regions (temporoparietal junction and superior temporal sulcus). Guilt- and shame-driven compensatory behavior recruited distinct neural substrates, with shame-driven compensatory sensitivity being more strongly linked to activity in the lateral prefrontal cortex, a region implicated in cognitive control. Our findings provide computational, algorithmic, and neural accounts of guilt and shame.
Introduction
Guilt and shame, two prominent moral emotions, underpin a wide array of social behaviors and phenomena, including norm compliance, cooperation, face-saving strategies, suicides, and even large-scale conflicts like wars (Haidt, 2003; Sznycer et al., 2021). Although guilt and shame often coexist after moral violation and serve to inhibit further transgressions (Eisenberg, 2000; Lewis, 1971), they differ in their associations with psychological and behavioral issues (Tangney & Dearing, 2003). For instance, shame is commonly associated with higher levels of anxiety, depression, stress, eating disorders, and aggression, whereas guilt is typically unrelated or negatively related to these issues (Blythin et al., 2020; Caldwell et al., 2021; Cândea & Szentagotai-Tătar, 2018; Elison et al., 2014; Kim et al., 2011; Schuster et al., 2021).
Recognizing their distinctions, a growing body of research in psychology and neuroscience has sought to elucidate the psychological and neural mechanisms differentiating guilt and shame, providing insights into how these emotions are processed and regulated. Psychological studies have revealed that guilt involves more concerns on one’s actions on others (i.e., negative behavioral impacts), whereas shame involves more concerns on one’s self-image (i.e., negative self-evaluations) (Lewis, 1971; Tangney et al., 2007; Tangney & Dearing, 2003). Echoing and complementing these findings, neuroscience research showed that neural activity in brain regions related to other-oriented theory-of-mind processing (temporoparietal junction (TPJ) and dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (DMPFC)), self-oriented self-referential processing (anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and DMPFC), and cognitive control (lateral prefrontal cortex (LPFC)) can distinguish guilt and shame (Piretti et al., 2023; Zhu, Feng, et al., 2019).
Although the psychological processes and neural activities related to guilt and shame experience are relatively well-documented, the cognitive antecedents of these emotions and their neural representation remain insufficiently understood. Existing research has identified harm (i.e., severity of harm) and responsibility (i.e., responsibility for harm) as cognitive antecedents influencing guilt (Abrams & Doosje, 2011; Berndsen et al., 2004; Čehajić-Clancy et al., 2011; Gao et al., 2021; Iyer et al., 2007; Koban et al., 2013; Li et al., 2020; Tangney, 1991; Yu et al., 2014) and shame (Iyer et al., 2007; Koban et al., 2013; Tangney, 1991). However, it remains unclear whether these factors differ in the strength of their influence on guilt and shame. Functionalist theories propose that guilt functions to minimize and repair undue harm on valued others, thereby addressing the adaptive problem of insufficiently valuing others—a behavior that can indirectly harms oneself (e.g., harming one’s partner can disrupt cooperation, ultimately damaging one’s own interests) (Sznycer, 2019). In contrast, shame functions to prevent and mitigate the threat of being devalued by others, thus addressing the adaptive problem of reputational damage to the self (e.g., being perceived as deficient in competence or morality by others) (Landers, Sznycer, & Al-Shawaf, 2024; Sznycer, 2019; Sznycer et al., 2021). If the functionalist theories are correct, the intensity of guilt and shame should more closely track the information that reflects the adaptive problems they are meant to address (e.g., Sznycer et al., 2016). Accordingly, severity of harm—a factor reflecting inadequate valuation of others—is expected to have a stronger impact on guilt than on shame, whereas responsibility for harm—a factor tied to devaluation by others, particularly in open and transparent social contexts—is anticipated to exert a greater influence on shame than guilt (see the Supplementary Information for illustrative examples).
Previous neuroscience studies have examined the neural response to harm or responsibility. Greater harm inflicted on others is associated with stronger activation in the DMPFC, TPJ, and insula (Crockett et al., 2017; Koban et al., 2013). Greater responsibility for harm is linked to stronger activation in the anterior middle cingulate cortex (aMCC) (Li et al., 2020; Yu et al., 2014). Despite these findings, it remains elusive how human brain integrate harm and responsibility when both factors are simultaneously present and dynamically vary. One possibility is that distinct brain regions separately encode harm and responsibility, with their signals converging in other regions responsible for integrating these two factors (e.g., Hu et al., 2017; Yu et al., 2018). Alternatively, some brain regions may directly encode an integrated representation of harm and responsibility, potentially through computational processes such as their product or quotient (e.g., Gray et al., 2002).
According to appraisal theory, emotions are not directly elicited by cognitive antecedents (e.g., harm and responsibility) but arise from the appraisal processes applied to these antecedents (Lazarus & Smith, 1988; Moors et al., 2013). Since appraisal processes vary across individuals, the same stimuli can evoke emotions of distinct intensities and even distinct types across different individuals (Ellsworth, 2013; Moors et al., 2013). Individual differences in emotional experiences, such as guilt and shame, may originate from the variations in neural responses to their cognitive antecedents (Hamann & Canli, 2004; Morawetz & Basten, 2024). However, the neural substrates that determine the extent to which harm and responsibility are transformed into guilt and shame remain largely unknown.
Shifting the focus from the origins of guilt and shame to their consequences, existing evidence on the distinct associations between these emotions and behavior remains incomplete. Guilt is widely recognized as a powerful motivator of altruistic behaviors, such as offering apologies and making amends (Graton & Ric, 2017; Howell et al., 2012; Ketelaar & Tung Au, 2003; Zhu et al., 2017). In comparison, shame appears to have a weaker promotive effect on altruistic behaviors compared to guilt (de Hooge et al., 2007, 2008; Declerck et al., 2014) and is commonly associated with non-cooperative and antisocial behaviors, including hiding, evasion, self-improvement, externalizing blame, and aggression (de Hooge et al., 2010; Gausel & Leach, 2011; Landers, Sznycer, & Al-Shawaf, 2024; Tangney et al., 1996; Zhu, Xu, et al., 2019). Most studies examined only guilt or only shame, restricting direct comparisons of their behavioral effects. The few studies that have directly compared these emotions relied on participants’ recollections of past personal events (de Hooge et al., 2007) or imagined scenarios (Ghorbani et al., 2013) to elicit guilt and shame. Variations in recollections and imagined contexts may introduce potential confounds, making it difficult to discern whether the observed behavioral differences are caused by guilt and shame or by irrelevant contextual factors.
Neurally, whether the transformation of guilt and shame into behavior depends on distinct neural bases remains an open question. Yu et al. (2014) found that the aMCC and midbrain nuclei are involved in linking guilt to compensatory behavior. To the best of our knowledge, the neural bases underlying the conversion of shame into behavior have yet to be explored. Naturally, this also means that no studies have directly compared the neural correlates of guilt-driven and shame-driven behavior.
To address the research gaps outlined above and provide computational, algorithmic, and neural accounts of guilt and shame (see Yu et al., 2024), we combined computational modelling and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) with a novel interpersonal game. In this game, we independently manipulated the harm inflicted on a victim and the responsibility of participants. Throughout the experiment, participants engaged in compensatory decision-making while undergoing fMRI scanning, followed by post-experimental self-reports of their guilt and shame feelings. This paradigm allowed us to explore the associations among harm, responsibility, guilt, shame, and compensation, as well as to uncover relevant neural substrates. Based on the functionalist theories (Baumeister et al., 1994; Gilbert, 1997; Landers, Sznycer, & Durkee, 2024; Sznycer, 2019; Sznycer et al., 2016), we predicted that harm would have a greater impact on guilt than shame, whereas responsibility has a greater impact on shame than guilt. Based on previous research on the relationships between guilt, shame, and altruistic behavior (de Hooge et al., 2007), we predicted that guilt would exert stronger influence on compensation than shame. Drawing from existing knowledge of the neural correlates of guilt and shame (Bastin et al., 2016; Li et al., 2020; Yu et al., 2020; Zhu, Feng, et al., 2019), we predicted that brain regions involved in emotional processing (e.g., insula), self-referential processing (e.g., DMPFC), theory-of-mind processing (e.g., TPJ), and cognitive control (e.g., LPFC) would play important roles in the neural representation of harm and responsibility, and the emergence of guilt-driven and shame-driven compensation.
Methods
Participants
We recruited 49 college students. All participants had normal or corrected-to-normal vision and reported no history of psychological or neurological disorders. They provided written consent and gained monetary payment for their participation. The experimental protocol (protocol number: ICBIR_A_0071_010) was approved by the local research ethics committee at the State Key Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and Learning, Beijing Normal University and was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.
Data from four participants weren’t correctly recorded due to a machine malfunction. One participant dropped out the experiment for personal reasons, one missed more than half of the experimental trials, and one fell asleep in the fMRI scanner. These seven participants were removed from all analyses. In addition, one participant who didn’t compensate the receiver in any trial (i.e., all compensation decisions were 0) was excluded from the computational modelling because the model parameters couldn’t be reliably estimated under such a condition (Zhong et al., 2016). Thus, analyses unrelated to computational modelling were conducted with 42 participants (18 females, 24 males; age: M ± SD = 21.71 ± 2.30 years), while analyses related to computational modelling were conducted with 41 participants (18 females, 23 males; age: M ± SD = 21.71 ± 2.33 years).
Experimental procedures
The experiment comprised two phases (Figure 1A). In the first phase, participants completed personality questionnaires online, including the Guilt and Shame Proneness scale (GASP; Cohen et al., 2011), Gratitude Questionnaire–6 scale (GQ-6; McCullough et al., 2002), and Social Value Orientation Slider Measure (SVO Slide Measure; Murphy et al., 2011). The 16-item GASP measures trait guilt and trait shame. Participants read various scenarios and indicated the likelihood of specific responses on a 7-point Likert scale (1 = very unlikely, 7 = very likely). The scale includes two guilt subscales—negative behavior evaluations (e.g., “What is the likelihood that you would feel terrible about the lies you told?”) and repair action tendencies (e.g., “What is the likelihood that you would try to act more considerately toward your friends?”)—and two shame subscales—negative self-evaluations (e.g., “What is the likelihood that this would make you would feel like a bad person?”) and withdrawal action tendencies (e.g., “What is the likelihood that you would avoid the guests until they leave?”). The six-item GQ-6 is a widely used measure of trait gratitude. Each item (e.g., “I am grateful to a wide variety of people”) is answered on a 7-point Likert scale (1 = strongly disagree, 7 = strongly agree). The six-item SVO Slide Measure assesses social value orientation (SVO), defined as the tendency to prioritize and balance outcomes for oneself and others in interdependent situations (Murphy & Ackermann, 2014). Participants chose among several self-other payoff combinations. Based on their choices, an SVO score was calculated. Larger SVO scores indicate more prosocial orientation. We measured trait gratitude and SVO for exploratory purposes. The participants filled out the questionnaires online at least one day before the second phase.

Experimental procedure.
(A) Timeline for the whole experiment. (B) Timeline for the interpersonal game. In this example, the participant, who was one of the four deciders, made an incorrect estimation, as did one of the other deciders. ISI, inter-stimulus interval.

Behavioral results.
(A) Harm had a stronger effect on guilt than on shame. (B) Responsibility revealed a stronger effect on shame than on guilt. (A, B) We created regression equations for guilt and shame ratings using the coefficient estimates from the linear mixed-effect regression analyses. To illustrate the impacts of harm and responsibility, we visualized the regression lines based on these equations. (C) The coefficient estimates from the linear mixed-effect regression model showed that, compared with shame, guilt exerted a larger effect on compensation. (D) Participants’ average guilt and shame feelings showed no significant difference. (C, D) Data are shown as the mean ± standard error with overlaid dot plots. *P < 0.05, ***P < 0.001; NS, not significant.
In the second phase, each participant arrived at our laboratory individually. Upon arrival, the participant was introduced in person to three co-players. These co-players, however, were confederates, and they were purported to interact with the participant later via an internal network. At least one confederate shared the participant’s gender, and at least one was of a different gender. The participant was then led to a separated room and completed a series of tasks.
Pain calibration
First, the participant underwent an individual pain calibration procedure with an SXC-4A multichannel electrical stimulator. After introducing the calibration process and precautions, we cleaned the participant’s left forearm and placed two disposable electrodes on the back of their left wrist. The initial electric shock was set as 12 repeated square waveform electrical stimulation pulses (current intensity of each pulse: 0.2 mA; duration of each pulse: 0.5 ms; interval between pulses: 10 ms) (Yu et al., 2018). We gradually increased or decreased the current intensity of each pulse in small increments (0.5 mA) with a 3:1 ratio (Crockett et al., 2014). The participant rated their subjective experience after each shock on an 11-point Likert scale (0 = no sensation, 10 = maximum tolerable pain). The calibration procedure continued until the participant reported a rating of 8. The current intensity value corresponding to this self-reported pain rating of 8 was recorded and used in the following steps. We then adjusted the number of pulses and applied electric shocks involving 2, 4, 8 and 12 repeated pulses to the participant (Yu et al., 2018). The participant rated the subjective pain intensity for each of the four shock levels. They were informed that these four pain levels would be used in the upcoming task. Providing the participant with the opportunity to experience the electric shocks beforehand enhanced the experiment’s authenticity.
Interpersonal game (with fMRI scanning)
After the pain calibration, the participant played a novel interpersonal game while their blood-oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) signals were measured using fMRI. The development of this game was inspired by several previous studies on guilt and shame (Li et al., 2020; Yu et al., 2014; Zhu, Feng, et al., 2019; Zhu, Wu, et al., 2019). In the game, there were five players: four deciders and one receiver. At the beginning of each trail, a picture of dots was presented on each decider’s computer screen (dots period, 1.5 s) (Figure 1B). Each decider indicated whether the number of dots was more than or less than 20 based on their own estimation by pressing a corresponding button (dots estimation period, < 2.5 s). Unbeknownst to the participant, the picture always contained 20 dots and the positions of the dots varied across trials. The four deciders wouldn’t receive any electric shock regardless of the correctness of their estimations. However, the receiver would receive an electric shock, if any decider made an incorrect estimation. The electric shock had four levels corresponding to the pain levels in the pain calibration procedure (pain levels 1 to 4). It was emphasized that all electric shocks were within safe limits and we had measured the pain sensitivity of all players to ensure that the deciders could experience the receiver’s pain sensation caused by different levels of electric shocks. The pain level of each trial was randomly determined. After making their estimations, the deciders saw the pain level of the current trial (cue period, 1 to 3 s), which was indicated by the number of white squares in the four white frames next to the electric shock symbol (e.g., three white squares indicating pain level 3). Afterwards, the deciders viewed the outcome (outcome evaluation period, 4.5 s). The first row displayed whether the receiver was subjected to an electric shock and its pain level. If the receiver wouldn’t receive any electric shock (i.e., all deciders made correct estimations), the white squares in the white frames disappeared. If the receiver would receive an electric shock, the white squares turned into black and filled the white frames. The second row showed whether the current decider made a correct estimation. A tick appeared in the black frame for a correct estimation, while a cross appeared for an incorrect estimation. The third row revealed how many other deciders, excluding the current decider, made incorrect estimations. It was represented by the number of black squares in the three white frames next to the small cross symbol. At the end of each trial, each decider was endowed 10 tokens (1 token = 0.5 Chinese yuan) and could freely distribute the tokens between themselves and the receiver (minimum unit: 1 token) by moving a black block using a response box (decision making period, < 6 s). The initial position of the block was randomized across trials. When the decider finalized their decision by pressing a button, the black block turned red as feedback for their response (decision feedback period, 6 s - response time).
Each decider made their distribution independently and didn’t know how many tokens the other deciders gave to the receiver. The receiver could receive a maximum of 40 tokens in a trail. Five trials would be randomly chosen and actualized (i.e., implementing the electric shocks and monetary rewards) at the end of the interpersonal game.
The participant and three confederates played as the deciders. We told the participant a cover story that the receiver was played by another college student who were not present in the laboratory at the time. They had visited the laboratory, undergone pain calibration, spent time understanding the game rules, and agreed to participate as the receiver. The receiver would visit the laboratory again to see the behaviors of all the deciders and face the consequences, including electric shocks and monetary rewards.
The outcomes of the interpersonal game were predetermined. The experimental trials were those in which the participant as a decider was informed that they made an incorrect estimation (i.e., the self-error condition). We had a 4 (harm: levels 1 to 4) × 4 (responsibility: levels 1 to 4) within-subjects design. To create different levels harm and responsibility, we manipulated the pain levels of the electric shock [harm level 1 (pain level 1), harm level 2 (pain level 2), harm level 3 (pain level 3), and harm level 4 (pain level 4)] and the number of other deciders who also made incorrect estimates [responsibility level 1 (3 deciders), responsibility level 2 (2 deciders), responsibility level 3 (1 deciders), and responsibility level 4 (0 decider), respectively. There were 16 possible combinations of harm levels and responsibility levels. We repeated these combinations 4 times and thus built 64 experimental trials. Unless otherwise specified, only data from these experimental trials were included in our analyses. To enhance the authenticity of the experiment, we also involved 48 filler trials, in which the participant was informed that they made a correct estimation (see Supplementary Table 1). Thus, the interpersonal game contained a total of 112 trials. We divided the trials into 4 sessions. Each session involved 16 experimental trials and 12 filler trials.
The trials of a specific session were presented in a pseudo-random order. The 4 sessions were assigned to 4 fMRI scanning runs in a balanced manner across participants.
Post-game survey (guilt and shame feelings recalling)
After the interpersonal game, the outcomes of the experimental trials were presented again. All the participants were required to recall and rate their feelings of guilt and shame (emotion ratings) when they saw those outcomes during the game (0 = not at all, 10 = very strong; 11-point Likert scale). They gave a rating of guilt and a rating of shame for each harm-responsibility combination (i.e., each participant provided 16 guilt ratings and 16 shame ratings).
Post-game survey (responsibility manipulation check)
The participants were also asked to rate their perceived responsibility for causing the receiver’s harm (1) when they were the only decider who made an incorrect estimation; (2) when they were one of the two deciders who made incorrect estimations; (3) when they were one of the three deciders who made incorrect estimations; (4) when they were one of the four deciders who made incorrect estimations; and (5) when they made a correct estimation, but the other deciders made wrong estimations (1 = no responsibility, 9 = high responsibility, 9-point Likert scale).
Post-game survey (additional measures and procedures)
Participants rated the perceived difficulty of the dot estimation task (1 = not at all, 4 = very much, 4-point Likert scale; M = 3.07, SD = 0.46) (Yu et al., 2014) and the extent of variation in task difficulty across trials (1 = not at all, 4 = very much, 4-point Likert scale; M = 2.43, SD = 0.59). Additionally, they reported to what extent they wanted to make correct estimations in the dot estimation task (1 = not at all, 9 = very much, 9-point Likert scale; M = 7.45, SD = 1.56) and to what extent they wanted to help the receiver to avoid electric shocks (1 = not at all, 9 = very much, 9-point Likert scale; M = 7.40, SD = 1.99). The results suggest that the participants found the task moderately challenging with some variation in difficulty across trials, while remaining highly motivated to perform accurately and positively impact the receiver, reflecting their active engagement in the task.
To examine whether the participants had any suspicion of the experiment, we conducted an interview at the very end of the experiment. No participant expressed doubts about the authenticity of the experiment.
Magnetic resonance imaging data acquisition
Whole-brain imaging data were acquired using a 3-Tesla Prisma magnetic resonance scanner at the State Key Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and Learning & IDG/McGovern Institute for Brain Research, Beijing Normal University. During the interpersonal game, T2-weighted echo-planar functional images were acquired (TR/TE: 2000/30 ms; FoV: [224 mm (RL) 224 mm (AP) 143 mm (FH)]; flip angle: 90°; voxel size: 2 × 2 × 2 mm; number of slices: 62; slice thickness: 2 mm; slice gap: 0.3 mm). Additionally, a T1-weighted 3-dimensional magnetization-prepared rapid acquisition gradient echo structural image was collected for image registration (TR/TE: 2530/2.98 ms; FoV: [192 mm (RL) 224 mm (AP) 256 mm (FH)]; flip angle: 7°; voxel size: 0.5 × 0.5 × 1 mm; number of slices: 192; slice thickness: 1 mm; slice gap: 0.5 mm) and gradient-echo field map images were acquired for correcting deformations (TR/TE1/TE2: 620/4.92/7.38 ms; FoV: [224 mm (RL) 224 mm (AP) 143 mm (FH)]; flip angle: 60°; voxel size: 2 × 2 × 2 mm; number of slices: 62; slice thickness: 2 mm; slice gap: 0.3 mm).
Behavioral analyses
Harm and responsibility manipulation checks
Painful feelings caused by electric shocks can be regarded as a form of harm (e.g., Crockett et al., 2017). To examine whether the electric shocks induced distinguishable perceptions of pain, we compared participants’ pain ratings pairwise across four levels of harm by conducting repeated measures analyses of variance (ANOVA). To examine whether the number of other deciders who made incorrect estimates with the participant caused distinguishable perceptions of responsibility, we compared participants’ responsibility ratings pairwise across four levels of responsibility by conducting repeated measures ANOVA. Additionally, to confirm that participants’ perceptions of responsibility were influenced by their correctness of estimates, we compared participants’ responsibility ratings between conditions where they made incorrect and correct estimates using repeated measures ANOVA.
The effects of harm and responsibility on guilt and shame
To test the influence of harm and responsibility on guilt and shame, we performed two separate linear mixed-effect regressions. In the first regression, we regressed participants’ emotion ratings onto harm levels, emotion types (guilt vs. shame), and their interaction. In the second regression, we regressed participants’ emotion ratings onto responsibility levels, emotion types (guilt vs. shame), and their interaction. The significance of the interaction effects in these two models allowed us to evaluate whether harm and responsibility had distinct influence on guilt and shame, which is a key question we are concerned with. We also conducted simple slope analyses for each emotion type to test whether harm and responsibility had significant effects on guilt and shame, respectively. As a complementary analysis, we conducted another linear mixed-effect regression, in which participants’ emotion ratings were regressed onto harm levels, responsibility levels, emotion types (guilt vs. shame), and all possible interaction. We used it to test whether the interaction between harm and emotion type, as well as the interaction between responsibility and emotion type, remained significant when controlling for the effects of other regressors. For all the regressions above, participant-specific random constants and random slopes for each fixed effect were involved as random effects.
The effects of guilt and shame on compensation
To examine whether and how guilt and shame affect compensation, we built four linear mixed-effect regression models.
In all models, the dependent variable remained the same: the amount of compensation (i.e., the number of tokens that participants distributed to the receiver), while the fixed-effect regressors varied. Model I included guilt rating as the sole regressor. Model II included shame rating as the sole regressor. Model III included both guilt and shame ratings. Model IV included guilt rating, shame rating, and their interaction. In all the regression models, participant-specific random constants and random slopes for each fixed effect were involved as random effects. The model with the lowest Bayesian information criterion (BIC) value was selected as the best model. Model III won, which suggest guilt and shame each uniquely contribute to compensatory behavior. To further understand the effects of guilt and shame, we used Wald chi-square tests to directly compared their influence (i.e., regression coefficients) on compensation based on the best model (i.e., Model III). Besides, to test whether the difference in the effects of guilt and shame on compensation was due the difference in their intensity, we compare participants’ average guilt and shame ratings across trials using repeated measures ANOVA.
Correlation analyses
Although the current study focused on state guilt and shame, we were aware of prior findings suggesting that trait guilt is associated with moral behavior, whereas trait shame seems not (Cohen et al., 2011; Tangney & Dearing, 2003). To replicate these findings, we calculated Pearson correlation coefficients to examine the relationship between the amount of compensation and trait guilt and trait shame. Additionally, we conducted exploratory analyses by calculating Pearson correlation coefficients between the amount of compensation and trait gratitude and social value orientation (SVO).
Computational modelling
Model development
After validating the associations among harm, responsibility, guilt, shame, and compensation through separate linear mixed-effect regressions, we sought to advance mechanistic understanding of guilt-driven and shame-driven compensatory decision-making through computational modelling. Drawing from the phenomenon of responsibility diffusion (Darley & Latané, 1968), we hypothesized that individuals perceive responsibility as being diluted when multiple wrongdoers are involved. This implies a cognitive mechanism wherein individuals mentally distribute harm across all wrongdoers, thereby reducing their altruistic behavior (compensation). Based on this Diffusion Hypothesis, we constructed Model family 1, where harm and the number of wrongdoers are integrated in the form of a quotient. All models within this family share this psychological mechanism, differing only in whether self-interest was accounted for (e.g., Wu et al., 2024) and whether a compensatory baseline was incorporated (e.g., Feng et al., 2023), both of which are common psychological components involved in social decision-making.
The models of Model family 1 are described as below: Model family 1 (Diffusion Hypothesis): Model 1.1 (self-interest considered and compensatory baseline exists):
where 𝑈(𝐷) is the utility for a compensatory decision. D is the number of tokens distributed to the receiver by participants and 10 − D is the number of tokens left for participants themselves (i.e., self-interest). H and W are the level of harm and the number of wrongdoers, respectively. W is the complement transformation of the level of responsibility (W = 5 – the level of responsibility).
Model 1.2 (self-interest considered and compensatory baseline doesn’t exist):
Model 1.3 (self-interest not considered and compensatory baseline exists):
Model 1.4 (self-interest not considered and compensatory baseline doesn’t exist):
It is also plausible that individuals might perceive their responsibility as intensifying the impact of harm (Moore, 2015). Under this mechanism, harm and responsibility interact in a multiplicative manner, resulting in an amplified sense of moral obligation and increased compensatory behaviors. Based on this Amplification Hypothesis, we built Model family 2. The models in the Model family 2 was the same as the Model family 1, expect that the quotient was replaced by the product of harm and responsibility. The models of Model family 2 are described as below:
Model family 2 (Amplification Hypothesis):
Model 2.1 (self-interest considered and compensatory baseline exists):
where R is the level of responsibility (R = 5 - W). Model 2.1 is the full model in model family 2, whereas the following Model 2.2, Model 2.3, and Model 2.4 are simplified versions.
Model 2.2 (self-interest considered and compensatory baseline doesn’t exist):
Model 2.3 (self-interest not considered and compensatory baseline exists):
Model 2.4 (self-icy baseline doesn’t exist):
We note that Model Families 1 and 2 reflect distinct psychological mechanisms and differ fundamentally in their mathematical formulations
For all the models, we transformed the trial-by-trial utility into choice probabilities (P) using the softmax function:
where 𝐷𝑗 represents a possible number of tokens that distributed to the receiver by participants (from 0 to 10 tokens, in increments of 1). J is the full set of j. λ is the inverse temperature parameter indicating the level of stochasticity in the compensatory decision.
Parameter estimation
For each model, we used the fmincon function of the Matlab to estimate the parameters for each participant. To decrease the possibility of the model converging on a local minimum, we constructed a coarse grid in the parameter space to choose the best start location. Maximum likelihood estimation was performed by maximizing the log likelihood function over each participant i and trial t:
Model comparison
We compared the models adopting the Bayesian information criteria, which considers both goodness of fit and model complexity. Model 1.3 was selected as the winning model. It assumes that individuals neglect their self-interest, have a compensatory baseline, and integrate harm and responsibility in the form of a quotient.
Model validation
We performed a series of analyses to validate the winning model (Model 1.3). To evaluate predictive accuracy, we used the estimated parameters of the winning model to simulated each participant’s compensatory decisions 50 times, generating 50 sets of simulated data. Predictive accuracy for each participant was calculated as the proportion of simulated decisions that matched their real decisions. We tested whether the predictive accuracies of our participants exceeded the chance level (9%) using a one-sample T test. We also visualized the effects of harm level and responsibility level on both simulated and real decisions, checking whether the behavioral patterns of the simulated decision aligned with those of the real decisions (Figures 3A and 3B).

Computational modeling results.
(A, B) Model simulations reproduced the behavioral patterns of compensatory decisions as influenced by harm (A) and responsibility (B). Data are shown as the mean ± standard error with overlaid dot plots. (C) The compensatory sensitivity (κ) and compensatory baseline (η) had no significant correlation. The line represents the least squares fit with shading showing the 95% confidence interval.
To demonstrate that the parameters of the winning model are identifiable, we conducted parameter recovery analyses. We refitted the model using the 50 sets of simulated data. We then evaluated the degree to which the parameter could be recovered by calculating the Pearson correlation coefficients between the parameters estimated from the real data and the parameters estimated from the simulated data.
To explore th e relationships among the parameters of the winning model, we calculated the Pearson correlation coefficients among the parameters estimated from the real data.
fMRI analyses
fMRI data preprocessing
Imaging data were preprocessed using the Statistical Parametric Mapping 12 (SPM 12, https://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/software/spm12/). Functional images were realigned to the first volume for head-motion correction, corrected for geometric distortions using field maps, slice-time corrected to the acquisition time of the middle slice. The mean functional image was co-registered with each participant’ T1-weighted image using parameters from the segmentation performed on both type of images. The functional images were then normalized into the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space, resampled to 3-mm isotropic voxels, and smoothed with an isotropic 8-mm full width at half-maximum Gaussian kernel.
Activation analysis (neural representation of cognitive antecedents)
We built several separate general linear models (GLMs) to explore how the cognitive antecedents of the guilt and shame are represented in the brain during outcome evaluation. They were designed to test different possibilities while addressing the problems of multicollinearity among regressors.
GLM 1 aimed to identify brain areas responding parametrically to the level of harm and level of responsibility. At the first level, we modeled the periods of dots (duration: 1.5 s), dots estimation (duration: 2.5 s), cue (duration: 1 to 3 s), outcome evaluation (duration: 4.5 s), decision making (duration: response time), decision feedback (duration: 6 s - response time), and missing trials (if existed) (Figure 1B). The period of outcome evaluation was modeled using two event regressors: one for experimental trials where participants made incorrect dot estimations (i.e., the self-error condition), and another for filler trials where participants made correct dot estimations. All other periods were modeled using a single regressor that encompassed all trials. Our analysis focused on the period of outcome evaluation in the self-error condition. For this corresponding event regressor, we included two parametric modulators: the level of harm (H) and the number of wrongdoers (W) (number of wrongdoers = 5 – responsibility level). Notably, in our experimental design, we ensured that the values of these two modulators were orthogonal, effectively avoiding multicollinearity issues. No additional orthogonalization adjustment were applied during the fMRI analysis. The regressors were convolved with a canonical hemodynamic response function. To control a motion effect of no interest, six head movement parameters from the realignment procedure were involved in the GLM as nuisance regressors. The inter-stimulus interval (i.e., jitter), which was not modeled in the GLM, served as an implicit baseline. We defined two contrasts at the first level, corresponding to each parametric modulator: (1) the level of harm, (2) the number of wrongdoers, each compared to the implicit baseline.
As complement analyses, we build another two GLMs to test the extent to which including the level of harm and the number of wrongdoers in the separate GLMs affected the neural findings compared to modelling them in same GLM. The new GLMs (GLM 2 and GLM 3) were identical to GLM 1, expect that each of them had only one parametric modulator (GLM 2: the level of harm; GLM 3: the number of wrongdoers) instead of two (GLM 1: the level of harm and the number of wrongdoers). For each of GLMs 2 and 3, we defined one contrast at the first level, with the parametric modulator compared to the implicit baseline.
GLM 4 targeted to identify brain areas responding to the average harm for which the participant is responsible (i.e., the quotient of the level of harm divided by the number of wrongdoers). It is same as the GLM 3, except that the parametric modulator was replaced with the quotient
GLM 5 set out to identify brain areas responding to the product of the level of harm and the level of responsibility (R). It is same as the GLM 4, except that the parametric modulator was replaced with the product (H×R). For GLM 5, we defined one contrast at the first level, with the parametric modulator compared to the implicit baseline.
At the second level, we submitted the first-level contrast images from all the GLMs above to one-sample T tests. The statistical threshold was set as P < 0.05 [family-wise error correction (FWE) at the cluster level] with an initial cluster-defining voxel-level threshold of P < 0.001 for the whole-brain analysis. This statistical threshold was applied to all fMRI data analyses unless stated otherwise.
Activation analysis (neural basis of emotion sensitivity)
The harm-driven guilt sensitivity, responsibility-driven guilt sensitivity, harm-driven shame sensitivity, and responsibility-driven shame sensitivity refer to the degree to which harm and responsibility elicit guilt and shame for an individual. We quantified them by the individual-specific coefficient estimates of harm and responsibility on guilt and shame from the linear mixed-effect regressions (Supplementary Tables 4 and 5). To examine whether these emotion sensitivities are correlated with neural responses to the quotient of harm divided by the number of wrongdoers, we submitted the first-level contrast images from GLM 4 to one-sample T tests and involved the four types of emotion sensitivity as covariates at the second level.
The results showed that the neural responses in the temporoparietal junction/superior temporal sulcus and precentral cortex/postcentral cortex/supplementary motor area were negatively correlated with the responsibility-driven shame sensitivity. To test whether these brain regions were more involved in the responsibility-driven shame sensitivity than the other types of emotion sensitivity, we used the identified brain regions as masks and extracted mean estimates (i.e., neural responses values) from them. We then compared the Pearson correlations between the extracted mean estimates and the four types of emotion sensitivities using Z test (Pearson and Filon’s Z).
Activation analysis (neural basis of compensatory sensitivity)
The guilt-driven compensatory sensitivity and shame-driven compensatory sensitivity refer to the extent to which guilt and shame are converted into compensation for an individual. They can be indicated by the individual-specific coefficient estimates of guilt and shame on compensation from the linear mixed-effect regression (Supplementary Table 8). To identify whether these two types of compensatory sensitivity are associated with brain activity during outcome evaluation, an additional contrast was defined at the first level based on GLM 4. This contrast examined average brain activity during outcome evaluation (effect of the event regressor: viewing the outcome in the self-error condition) (Losin et al., 2020). At the second level, we submitted the first-level contrast images from GLM 4 to one-sample T tests and involved the two types of compensatory sensitivity as covariates.
We found that neural activity in the left temporal pole, bilateral inferior parietal lobe, and left lateral prefrontal cortex was only correlated with guilt-driven or shame-driven compensatory sensitivity (Figures 5A and 5B). To directly test whether these regions were more involved in one of the two types of compensatory sensitivity, we used the identified brain regions as masks and extracted mean estimates (i.e., neural activation values) from them. We then compared the Pearson correlations between the extracted mean estimates and the two types of sensitivities using Z test (Pearson and Filon’s Z).

Neural representation of cognitive antecedents and neural basis of emotion sensitivity.
(A) The quotient of harm divided by the number of wrongdoers (i.e., average harm per person) is represented by the striatum and posterior insula (pINS). (B) The neural responses to average harm per person in the two clusters containing temporoparietal junction (TPJ)/ superior temporal sulcus (STS) and precentral cortex (PRC)/postcentral cortex (POC)/ supplementary motor area (SMA) were negatively correlated with responsibility-driven shame sensitivity. (A, B) Negative T values indicate negative correlation. Whole-brain FWE-cluster correction at P < 0.05 after cluster-forming-threshold at P < 0.001. (C) The TPJ/STS and PRC/POC/SMA clusters showed a significantly stronger negative correlation with shame-driven sensitivity than with guilt-driven sensitivity. Each line represents the least squares fit with shading showing the 95% confidence interval.

Neural basis of compensatory sensitivity.
(A) The dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (DMPFC), supplementary motor area (SMA), and temporal pole (TP) showed significant activity associated with guilt-driven compensatory sensitivity. A red circle marked the region showed significant activity associated with guilt-driven compensatory sensitivity but not with shame-driven compensatory sensitivity. (B) The DMPFC, SMA, right and left inferior parietal lobe (IPL), and left lateral prefrontal cortex (LPFC) showed significant activity associated with shame-driven compensatory sensitivity. Blue circles marked the region showed significant activity associated with shame-driven compensatory sensitivity but not with guilt-driven compensatory sensitivity. (C) The left LPFC showed a significantly stronger positive correlation with shame-driven sensitivity than with guilt-driven sensitivity. Each line represents the least squares fit with shading showing the 95% confidence interval. (A, B, D) The neural correlates of the parameter κ largely overlapped with the regions linked to guilt-driven and shame-driven compensatory sensitivities. The parameter κ is additionally associated with activation in the bilateral anterior insula (aINS). r, right; l, left; Whole-brain FWE-cluster correction at P < 0.05 after cluster-forming-threshold at P < 0.001.
Compensation may be driven by emotion or motivation other than guilt and shame (e.g., indebtedness) (Gao et al., 2024). The parameter κ from our winning computational model (i.e., Model 1.3) is supposed to capture the combined influence of various psychological processes on compensation, including guilt and shame. To confirm the relationships between κ and guilt-driven and shame-driven compensatory sensitivities, we calculated the Pearson correlations between them. Besides, to more comprehensively explore the neural basis of compensatory sensitivity, we submitted the first-level contrast images from GLM 4 to one-sample T tests, incorporating the values of κ as covariates.
Activation analysis and mediation analysis (neural correlates of trait guilt and compensation)
As we observed significant correlations between trait guilt scores and compensatory behavior, we further explored brain activity that mediates their relationships. At the first step, we attempted to identify the neural correlates of the two types of trait guilt by submitted the first-level contrast images from GLM 4 to one-sample T tests and involved the negative behavior-evaluations and repair action tendencies scores as covariates at the second level. The aMCC and midbrain nuclei are defined as the regions of interest (ROIs), given that they have been found to be associated with guilt and compensation (Yu et al., 2014). The ROIs for aMCC and midbrain nuclei were respectively defined as a 6-mm-radius sphere centered at the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) coordinates [0, 34, 16] and [−2, −20, −20] (Yu et al., 2014). Brain activities that revealed significant correlations with trait guilt scores were considered as candidate mediators (e.g., Losin et al., 2020). The statistical threshold was set as P < 0.05 [family-wise error correction (FWE) at the cluster level] with an initial cluster-defining voxel-level threshold of P < 0.001 for the small-volume correction analysis or the whole-brain analysis.
At the second step, we examined whether the candidate mediators mediated the relationship between trait guilt and compensation using the PROCESS macro based on the SPSS software (http://www.processmacro.org/index.html). We tested the mediation effect, with participants’ trait guilt scores as the predictor variable, compensation as the outcome variable, and mean estimates extracted from the clusters we identified in the first stage or the ROIs (Supplementary Table 19) as the mediator variable (one candidate mediator per mediation analysis). We used a bootstrap procedure (randomly sampling 5000 observations with replacement) to obtain 95% confidence intervals (CI) of path coefficients for significance testing. If the CI of the path coefficient did not cover zero, the effect was considered as significant.
For completeness, we explored the neural correlates of other trait by submitted the first-level contrast images from GLM 4 to one-sample T tests and involved the trait shame, trait gratitude, or SVO scores as covariates at the second level.
Results
Manipulate checks
Participants’ pain ratings differed significantly across the four harm levels (harm level 1 vs. harm level 2: F(1,41) = 324.63, P < 0.001, partial 𝜂2 = 0.888; harm level 2 vs. harm level 3: F(1,41) = 181.26, P < 0.001, partial 𝜂2 = 0.816; harm level 3 vs. harm level 4 F(1,41) = 247.41, P < 0.001, partial 𝜂2 = 0.858). Their responsibility ratings differed significantly across the four responsibility levels (responsibility level 1 vs. responsibility level 2: F(1,41) = 36.41, P < 0.001, partial 𝜂2 = 0.470; responsibility level 2 vs. responsibility level 3: F(1,41) = 46.47, P < 0.001, partial 𝜂2 = 0.531; responsibility level 3 vs. responsibility level 4: F(1,41) = 139.24, P < 0.001, partial 𝜂2 = 0.773) (Supplementary Figure 1; Supplementary Tables 2 and 3). In addition, participants reported higher responsibility ratings when they made incorrect estimates compared to correct estimates (making right estimates vs. responsibility level 1: F(1,41) = 56.63, P < 0.001, partial 𝜂2 = 0.580; making right estimates vs. responsibility level 2: F(1,41) = 148.25, P < 0.001, partial 𝜂2 = 0.783; making right estimates vs. responsibility level 3: F(1,41) = 237.52, P < 0.001, partial 𝜂2 = 0.853; making right estimates vs. responsibility level 4: F(1,41) = 495.73, P < 0.001, partial 𝜂2 = 0.924). These results suggest our manipulations of harm and responsibility were successful.
Distinct impacts of harm and responsibility on guilt and shame
To confirm and compare the effects of harm and responsibility on guilt and shame, we ran two linear mixed-effect regression analyses. In the first analysis, participants’ emotion ratings were regressed onto harm levels, emotion types (guilt vs. shame), and their interaction. In the second analysis, participants’ emotion ratings were regressed onto responsibility levels, emotion types (guilt vs. shame), and their interaction. To determine whether harm and responsibility had significant effects on guilt and shame respectively, we performed simple slope analyses for each emotion type. We found that harm significantly increased participants’ guilt ratings (βguilt = 0.74, T(67) = 8.92, P < 0.001) and shame ratings (βshame = 0.23, T(67) = 2.77, P = 0.007). Similarly, responsibility significantly increased participants’ guilt ratings (βguilt = 0.57, T(61) = 5.27, P < 0.001) and shames ratings (βshame = 0.93, T(61) = 8.54, P < 0.001). Of importance, harm had a stronger effect on guilt than on shame, as indicated by the interaction effect between harm and emotion type (βguilt - βshame = 0.51, T(41) = 5.96, P < 0.001) (Figure 2A; Supplementary Table 4). Conversely, responsibility exerted a greater influence on shame than on guilt, as shown by the interaction effect between responsibility and emotion type (βguilt - βshame) = −0.35, T(41) = 3.60, P < 0.001) (Figure 2B; Supplementary Tables 5). To control for the potential influence of other interaction effects, we conducted an additional linear mixed-effect regression. We regressed participants’ emotion ratings onto harm levels, responsibility levels, emotion types (guilt vs. shame), and all their interactions. The results showed that the interaction between harm and emotion type, as well as the interaction between responsibility and emotion type, remained significant (Supplementary Table 6). Together, these findings demonstrate the differential impacts of harm and responsibility on guilt and shame.
Guilt and shame differed in their effects on compensatory behavior
To investigate whether and how guilt and shame contributed to compensation, we tested 4 linear mixed-effect regression models. The models regressed the number of tokens that participants distributed to the receiver on different fixed-effect regressors (see Methods). The results of the model comparison showed that the best model (Model III, Supplementary Table 7) included guilt and shame ratings (but not their interaction) as the regressors. The statistical results of the best model revealed that both guilt (β = 0.22, T(41) = 5.16, P < 0.001) and shame (β = 0.10, T(36) = 3.69, P < 0.001) were significantly associated with compensation (Supplementary Table 8). Furthermore, a direct comparison between the coefficient estimates indicated that guilt had a stronger effect on compensation than shame (χ2(1) = 6.48, P = 0.011) (Figure 2C). This difference in the effects of guilt and shame on compensation could not be attributed to differences in the intensity of guilt and shame, as participants’ average guilt and shame ratings across trials showed no significant difference (F(1,41) = 1.11, P = 0.297, partial 𝜂2 = 0.026) (Figure 2D). Our results provide solid evidence supporting that guilt, compared to shame, is more effectively converted into compensation.
Personality traits and compensatory behavior
Consistent with previous findings (e.g., Cohen et al., 2011), we found that two types of trait guilt scores were significantly correlated to participants’ compensatory behavior (negative behavior-evaluations: R = 0.39, P = 0.010; repair action tendencies: R = 0.33, P = 0.030), whereas two types of trait shame scores were not (negative self-evaluations: R = 0.20, P = 0.213; withdrawal action tendencies: R = 0.16, P = 0.315). These findings again demonstrate the distinct behavioral impacts of guilt and shame. The trait gratitude scores and SVO scores were not significantly correlated with compensatory behavior (trait gratitude: R = 0.18, P = 0.255; SVO: R = 0.24, P = 0.121).
Computational models of compensatory behavior
After confirming their associations through separate linear mixed-effect regression analyses, we used utility models to characterize how individuals integrate harm with responsibility during guilt-driven and shame-driven compensatory decision-making within a unified computational framework. We constructed 8 models across 2 model families, which aimed at capturing individuals’ distinct latent psychological processes. They varied in whether self-interest was considered, whether compensatory baseline existed, and how harm and responsibility were integrated (see Methods). Based on the Bayesian information criteria, Model 1.3 outperformed other alternative models (Supplementary Table 9). This model assumes that, during compensatory decision-making, individuals disregard their self-interest, adopt a compensatory baseline, and integrate harm and the number of wrongdoers in the form of a quotient (rather than integrating harm and responsibility in the form of a product). The utility (U) of a compensatory decision is described as follows (Model 1.3):
where H and W are the level of harm and the number of wrongdoers, respectively.
We validated the winning model (Model 1.3) through a variety of analyses.
Simulation tests indicated that the model could replicate the influence of harm level and responsibility level on compensation through simulation (see Figures 3A and 3B) and predict participants’ compensatory decisions with significantly higher accuracy than the chance level (mean accuracy: 31%, 95% confidence interval (CI): [27%, 36%], chance level: 9%). Parameter recovery tests revealed that the parameters were highly identifiable (Pearson correlation between real and recovered parameters: rκ = 0.95, CI = [0.95, 0.96]; rη = 0.99, CI = [0.99, 0.99]). No evidence support that the psychological processes represented by the parameters κ and η overlap, as the Pearson correlations between the two parameters were not significant (R = −0.17, P = 0.289) (Figure 3C).
Neural representation of cognitive antecedents
To investigate the neural representation of the cognitive antecedents of guilt and shame during outcome evaluation, we constructed general linear models (GLMs) to identify brain regions that respond parametrically to harm, the number of wrongdoers, the quotient of harm divided by the number of wrongdoers, and the product of harm and responsibility (responsibility = 5 – the number of wrongdoers) (see Methods). No brain region showed significant responses to harm, which implies the brain doesn’t represent harm in isolation. The precentral and postcentral cortices, known to be involved in movement preparation (Porro et al., 1996; Thoenissen et al., 2002), exhibited positive parametric responses to the number of wrongdoers, potentially reflecting motor preparation for subsequent button-press action related to compensation (Supplementary Table 10). The results of the neural representation of the level of harm and the number of wrongdoers remained consistent, regardless of whether these two parametric modulators were put in the same GLM or in separated GLMs (Supplementary Tables 10 and 11).
The quotient of harm divided by the number of wrongdoers correlated parametrically with the activation in brain regions involved in social cognition. Specifically, the striatum linked to value computation (Bartra et al., 2013; Clithero & Rangel, 2013) and the posterior insula (pINS) associated with inequity aversion (Gao et al., 2018; Hsu et al., 2008) exhibited negative parametric responses to the quotient (Figure 4A; Supplementary Table 12). In contrast, no brain region showed significant responses to the product of harm and responsibility. Together with the findings from computational modeling, these fMRI results suggest that individuals integrate harm and responsibility in the form of a quotient rather than a product.
Neural basis of emotion sensitivity
To examine the neural basis of the individual harm-driven guilt sensitivity, responsibility-driven guilt sensitivity, harm-driven shame sensitivity, and responsibility-driven shame sensitivity, we correlated the coefficient estimates of harm and responsibility on guilt and shame from the linear mixed-effect regressions (Supplementary Tables 4 and 5) with the whole-brain neural activity. The whole-brain analysis revealed that the parametric responses of the theory-of-mind areas (i.e., TPJ and superior temporal sulcus (STS)) to the quotient of harm divided by the number of wrongdoers were negatively correlated with responsibility-driven shame sensitivity (Figure 4B; Supplementary Table 13). Namely, participants who more intensively converted responsibility into shame had weaker neural responses to the average harm per person in these regions. To assess whether these brain regions were specifically involved in responsibility-driven shame sensitivity, we compared the Pearson correlations between their activity and all types of emotion sensitivities. The results demonstrated the domain specificity of these regions, by revealing that the TPJ/STS cluster had more negative parametric responses to responsibility-driven shame sensitivity than responsibility-driven guilt sensitivity (TPJ/STS: Z = 4.12, P < 0.001), harm-driven shame sensitivity (TPJ/STS: Z = 3.45, P < 0.001), and harm-driven guilt sensitivity (TPJ/STS: Z = 2.02, P = 0.044) (Figure 4C; Supplementary Table 14). In addition, the sensorimotor areas (i.e., precentral cortex (PRC), postcentral cortex (POC), and supplementary motor area (SMA)) exhibited the same activation pattern as the TPJ/STS (Figure 4B and 4C; Supplementary Tables 13 and 14).
No brain response showed significant correlation with responsibility-driven guilt sensitivity, harm-driven shame sensitivity, or responsibility-driven shame sensitivity.
Neural basis of compensatory sensitivity
To examine the neural basis of the individual guilt-driven and shame-driven compensatory sensitivities, we correlated the coefficient estimates of guilt and shame on compensation from the linear mixed-effect regression (Supplementary Table 8) with the whole-brain neural activity. The whole-brain analysis showed that the activation in the DMPFC/SMA was significantly correlated with both guilt-driven and shame-driven compensatory sensitivities (Supplementary Table 15). Besides, the activity in the left temporal pole (TP) was significantly correlated with guilt-driven compensatory sensitivity but not with shame-driven compensatory sensitivity (Figure 5A), whereas activity in the bilateral inferior parietal lobe (IPL), and left LPFC clusters was significantly correlated with shame-driven compensatory sensitivity but not with guilt-driven compensatory sensitivity (Figure 5B). To directly assess whether these brain regions were more involved in guilt-driven or shame-driven compensatory sensitivity, we compared the Pearson correlations between brain activity and the two types of compensatory sensitivities. The results revealed that the left LPFC was more engaged in shame-driven compensatory sensitivity (Z = 2.51, P = 0.012), as its activity showed a significantly stronger positive correlation with shame-driven sensitivity than with guilt-driven sensitivity (Figure 5C). No significant difference was found in the Pearson correlations between the activity of the left TP, right IPL, left IPL, and the two types of sensitivities (Supplementary Table 16).
In addition to guilt and shame, other emotion or motivation may also contribute to the transformation of harm and responsibility into compensatory behavior. The parameter κ from our winning computational model ought to capture the combined effects of various psychological processes on compensation, including guilt and shame. Confirming their associations, the parameter κ significantly correlated to both guilt-driven (β = 2.06, T(38) = 5.85, P < 0.001) and shame-driven (β = 2.43, T(38) = 2.82, P = 0.008) compensatory sensitivities (Supplementary Table 17). To gain a more comprehensive understanding of the neural basis of compensatory sensitivity, we correlated κ with the whole-brain neural activity. The neural correlates of the parameter κ largely overlapped with those associated with guilt-driven and/or shame-driven compensatory sensitivities, including the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (DMPFC), supplementary motor area (SMA), left temporal pole (TP), left LPFC (LPFC), bilateral inferior parietal lobe (IPL) (Figures 5A, 5B, and 5D; Supplementary Tables 14 and 18). Notably, κ is also associated with activation in the bilateral anterior insula (aINS).
Neural correlates of trait guilt and compensation
As we observed significant correlations between trait guilt scores and compensatory behavior, we further our investigation by examining their neural correlates. A small-volume correction analysis revealed that repair action tendencies (i.e., scores of a trait guilt subscale) were significantly associated with the aMCC’s responses to the quotient of harm divided by the number of wrongdoers (peak MNI coordinates: [-3, 30, 15]; cluster size: 12 voxels; PFWE = 0.003, small volume corrected; Figure 6A). The results of the aMCC, along with other clusters, retained significance after whole-brain correction (Figure 6B; Supplementary Table 19). Moreover, only the activity in the aMCC’s mediated the relationship between repair action tendencies and compensation (indirect effect: 𝛽 = 0.10, CI = [0.01, 0.21]) (Figure 6C).

Neural correlates of trait guilt and compensation.
(A) A small-volume correction analysis showed that participants with higher guilt trait scores (i.e., repair action tendencies) have more positive parametric responses to the quotient of harm divided by the number of wrongdoers in the anterior middle cingulate cortex (aMCC). The scatter plot is for presenting the positive correlation relationship between repair action tendencies and neural responses of the aMCC. The line represents the least squares fit with shading showing the 95% confidence interval. (B) The neural finding of the aMCC remained significant after whole-brain correction. Whole-brain FWE-cluster correction at P < 0.05 after cluster-forming-threshold at P < 0.001. (A, B) A circle of dots indicated the position of the aMCC mask. (C) The aMCC parametric responses mediated the relationship between repair action tendencies and compensation. β, path coefficient; CI, 95% confidence interval of β; bold font, significant indirect effect.
No neural activity showed a significant correlation with negative behavior evaluations (i.e., another trait guilt), trait shame, trait gratitude, or SVO.
Discussion
Guilt and shame have long been a focal point of research across various disciplines, including psychology, behavioral economics, neuroscience, and psychiatry (e.g., Chang et al., 2011; Gao et al., 2018; Landers, Sznycer, & Durkee, 2024; Mottershead et al., 2024; Schuster et al., 2021; Sznycer, 2019; Tangney et al., 2007). Extensive research has investigated the psychological and neural activities associated with the experience of guilt and shame to enhance emotion regulation and improve behavioral prediction (Bastin et al., 2016; Miceli & Castelfranchi, 2018; Michl et al., 2014; Piretti et al., 2023; Pulcu et al., 2014; Takahashi et al., 2004; Tangney & Dearing, 2003; Xu et al., 2022; Zhu, Feng, et al., 2019). We extended this research by shifting the focus from the mere states of guilt and shame to their associations with the cognitive antecedents and behavioral consequences. Our findings advance the understanding of the psychological and neural mechanisms that underlie both the formation of guilt and shame and their subsequent transformation into compensatory behavior.
Consistent with previous studies (Gao et al., 2021; Iyer et al., 2007; Koban et al., 2013; Li et al., 2020; Tangney, 1991; Yu et al., 2014), we observed that both harm and responsibility increase individuals’ feelings of guilt and shame. Of importance, for the first time, we discovered that harm exerts a stronger effect on guilt than on shame, whereas responsibility has a stronger effect on shame than on guilt. These findings provide empirical support for the proposition that guilt and shame serve distinct social functions. According to functionalist theories (e.g., Baumeister et al., 1994; Gilbert, 1997; Sznycer, 2019; Sznycer et al., 2021), guilt functions to curb the harm imposed on valued others, whereas shame functions to mitigate the cost of reputational damage to oneself. If these theories hold true, guilt should be more sensitive than shame to the information related to harm inflicted on valued others, where shame should be more sensitive than guilt to the information related to reputational damage to oneself (Sznycer et al., 2016). However, few studies have provided such direct evidence. An exception is the study by Landers et al. (2024), which found that information related to harm inflicted on valued others (e.g., liking of a victim) and concerns about reputational damage (e.g., fear of a victim) were respectively predictive of guilt and shame. Notably, Landers et al. (2024) employed a vignette-based paradigm and assessed participants’ guilt and shame using items that reflected the motivational tendencies characteristic of these emotions (e.g., guilt: “I would go to him and apologize for it”; shame: “I would feel like avoiding him for a while”). In contrast, our study utilized a laboratory-based paradigm with real-time decision-making, directly measured participants’ state guilt and shame experiences, and test new psychological factors (harm and responsibility). This approach offers greater ecological validity and provides novel evidence (Yu et al., 2024).
Existing findings suggest that guilt is more strongly linked to altruistic behavior than shame is (de Hooge et al., 2007, 2008; Declerck et al., 2014; Gausel & Leach, 2011; Graton & Ric, 2017; Ketelaar & Tung Au, 2003; Tangney et al., 2007; Tangney & Dearing, 2003). Nevertheless, because of methodological limitations, such as failing to compare guilt and shame directly or employing methods that may introduce confounding variables, conclusive evidence has been lacking. Overcoming these limitations, our study demonstrates that while both guilt and shame promote compensation, guilt is more effective in prompting compensatory behavior. The functionalist theories offer a framework for understanding why guilt exerts a stronger effect on compensation than shame does. Guilt corresponds to the adaptive problem of insufficiently valuing others (Sznycer, 2019). To address it, individuals in guilt must bring benefits to those they have harmed—typically through altruistic behavior—to correct the inequity caused by their wrongdoing. In contrast, shame is tied to reputational damage (Sznycer, 2019; Sznycer et al., 2021). Although altruistic behavior can also mitigate shame by demonstrating their social value, individuals may resort to other strategies—such as avoidance or aggression—to protect themselves from potential devaluation by others (de Hooge et al., 2018; Zhu, Xu, et al., 2019).
For a deeper understanding of social emotions, it is crucial to formally model their cognitive operations and investigate the neural underpinnings of these cognitive operations (Yu et al., 2024). Building on this research line, our computational modelling results reveal that individuals in guilt and shame disregard their self-interest, adopt a compensatory baseline, and mentally distribute harm across all wrongdoers. The findings not only offer a mechanistic explanation at the behavioral (algorithmic) level for guilt- and shame-driven compensatory decision-making, but also deepen the understanding of the phenomenon of responsibility diffusion by offering a formal mathematical formulation and linking it to compensatory behavior (Darley & Latané, 1968).
At the neural level, our findings demonstrate the involvement of the posterior insula and striatum in representing the cognitive antecedents of guilt and shame. Specifically, the activation in these brain regions decreased as the quotient of harm divided by the number of wrongdoers increased. Harm inflicted on the victim, particularly the portion for which a wrongdoer is responsible, creates a sense of inequity between them. Beyond its well-established role in interoceptive awareness (Craig, 2009; Craig, 2011), the pINS has been implicated in processing economic inequity in allocation tasks (Gao et al., 2018; Hsu et al., 2008). For instance, Hsu et al. (2008) reported that pINS activation negatively correlates with the degree of inequity, suggesting that greater inequity elicits lower pINS activation. Our results extend this role of the pINS beyond economic inequity to encompass harm inequity. Given that the striatum is implicated in value computation (Bartra et al., 2013; Crockett et al., 2017; Rilling et al., 2008), we believe that its activity reflects individuals’ perception of the loss (i.e., harm) inflicted on the victim. In contrast, no brain region had significant responses to the product of harm and responsibility. Thus, the fMRI and computational modelling findings offer convergent evidence indicating that individuals are more likely to integrate these cognitive antecedents in a form of quotient.
In addition, no brain region exhibited significant responses to harm. Only the sensorimotor areas showed significant responses to the number of wrongdoers (i.e., the complement of responsibility, 5 – responsibility level). Since no brain region associated social cognition showed significant responses to harm or responsibility, it appears that human brain encodes a unified measure integrating harm and responsibility (i.e., the quotient) rather than processing them as separate entities when both are relevant to subsequent emotional experience and decision-making.
As to emotion sensitivity, our findings show that individuals who tend to convert responsibility into shame exhibit reduced activation in brain regions associated with other-oriented theory-of-mind processing, specifically the TPJ and STS. The TPJ and STS have been implicated in inferring others’ mental states (Schurz et al., 2014). Lower activation in these regions indicates that individuals with higher responsibility-driven shame sensitivity may be less engaged in considering the victim’s experiences and thoughts. This aligns with existing research on shame, which, compared to guilt, is associated with less concerns on one’s actions on others (Tangney & Dearing, 2003) and weaker activation in the TPJ (Zhu, Feng, et al., 2019).
Regarding compensatory sensitivity, our results show that both individuals with higher guilt-driven and shame-driven compensatory sensitivity have stronger activation in the DMPFC. This region is central to both theory-of-mind processing (Schurz et al., 2014) and self-referential processing (Northoff et al., 2006), playing a crucial role in combining others’ thoughts and feelings with one’s own (D’Argembeau et al., 2007; Saxe et al., 2006). Zhu, Feng, et al. (2019) has identified the DMPFC’s involvement in the experience of both guilt and shame. Our findings here further highlight its role in translating these emotions into compensatory behavior.
We found that the TP’s activity is positively related to individuals’ guilt-driven compensatory sensitivity. This region is considered as a core part of the theory-of-mind network (Frith & Frith, 2003). Numerous studies suggest that the activation in this region reflect retrieval of both general conceptual knowledge (Lambon Ralph & Patterson, 2008) and social conceptual knowledge (e.g., social rules) (Ross & Olson, 2010; Sugiura et al., 2006; Tsukiura et al., 2010; Zahn et al., 2007). The retrieval of such information likely facilitates understanding others’ thoughts and empathizing with their suffering (Olson et al., 2007; Schurz et al., 2014). Empathy, in turn, has been widely established as a significant driver of altruistic behavior, including compensatory behavior (Ding & Lu, 2016; Eisenberg & Miller, 1987). Our findings confirm the involvement of the TP in translating guilt into compensation
The IPL’s activity has a positive correlation with individuals’ shame-driven compensatory sensitivity. This region is associated with various non-social and social cognitive functions, including number processing (Pinel et al., 2004; Sandrini et al., 2004), salience processing (Kahnt et al., 2014), and theory-of-mind processing (Igelström & Graziano, 2017; Tusche et al., 2016). Two recent studies provided direct evidence showing that the IPL plays a role in encoding others’ benefits during altruistic decision making (Hu et al., 2017, 2021). Our findings about IPL can be explained by its involvement in generating other-regarding motives (Hu et al., 2021) that facilitate the conversion from shame to compensation.
We did not find a significant difference in the correlations between TP activity and guilt-driven versus shame-driven compensatory sensitivities. Similarly, no significant difference was observed in the correlations between IPL activity and shame-driven versus guilt-driven compensatory sensitivities. These findings suggest that neither of these regions plays a domain-specific role in compensation driven by guilt or shame.
In contrast, LPFC activity exhibited a significantly stronger correlation with shame-driven compensatory sensitivity than with guilt-driven compensatory sensitivity, indicating a domain-specific role of the LPFC in shame-related compensation. The LPFC is implicated in cognitive control (Koechlin, 2003) and the optimization of social decision-making (Buckholtz & Marois, 2012; Feng et al., 2015). Some brain stimulation studies have demonstrated that disrupting LPFC activity impairs individuals’ ability to inhibit selfish or aggressive impulses, which can incur social devaluation and punishment from others (Knoch et al., 2006, 2009; Riva et al., 2014). Further research has extended these findings by emphasizing the LPFC’s role in strategic social behavior (Ruff et al., 2013; Strang et al., 2015). For instance, Ruff et al. (2013) found that when individuals face the possibility of being punished for selfish behavior, enhancing LPFC activity suppresses selfish impulses and promotes altruistic behavior. However, in the absence of punishment risk, enhancing LPFC activity instead reduces altruistic behavior and promote self-interest. Considering that guilt is typically alleviated through altruistic behavior (Tang et al., 2019), whereas coping strategies for shame are more varied—ranging from altruistic behavior to aggression and avoidance (Sznycer, 2019; Sznycer et al., 2016)—shame appears to be more closely linked to strategic thinking than guilt. This explains why LPFC activity, which is associated with strategic behavior, is more strongly related to shame-driven compensatory sensitivity than to guilt-driven compensatory sensitivity.
The neural correlates of the parameter κ largely overlapped with those linked to compensatory sensitivities driven by guilt and shame. Intriguingly, beyond that, κ also showed a strong association with aINS activity. Insula, known as a key node in the salience network (Uddin, 2015), engages in during experiencing various negative emotions, including sadness (Wagner et al., 2011), disgust (Craig, 2009), guilt (Yu et al., 2014), shame (Piretti et al., 2023; Zhu, Feng, et al., 2019), and indebtedness (Gao et al., 2024). Social neuroscience research highlights the critical role of the aINS in processing norm violations and guiding behavior accordingly (Bellucci et al., 2018; Zinchenko & Arsalidou, 2018). For example, Chang et al. (2011) found that aINS serves to mitigate anticipated negative feelings triggered by norm violations by facilitating individuals’ reciprocity toward their partners’ investments in a trust game, thereby maintaining adherence to the norm of reciprocity. Consistently, numerous studies on the ultimatum game reveal aINS’s involvement in rejecting unfair offers and upholding the norm of fairness (Feng et al., 2015; Gabay et al., 2014). In the same line, the findings on the involvement of aINS in social conformity also manifest its role in monitoring norm violations and reinforcing adherence to social norms (Berns et al., 2010; Klucharev et al., 2009). Given the involvement of the aINS in various social-affective processes, our findings suggest that the motivation to uphold social norms might directly shape individuals’ compensatory behavior or indirectly influence it through emotions beyond guilt and shame, with aINS activity playing a pivotal role in this process.
Interestingly, we found that the sensorimotor areas were associated with the representation of a shame-related cognitive antecedent (i.e., responsibility) and emotional sensitivity. Our findings align with the result from a fMRI meta-analysis, which identified the involvement of sensorimotor regions in processing shame (Piretti et al., 2023). It has been suggested that sensorimotor activation may reflect typical shame-related action tendencies, such as reduced social presence, speech, and movement (Piretti et al., 2023). However, in our study, participants were required to remain completely still throughout the experiment to maintain MRI data quality and were continuously monitored, eliminating the possibility of physical withdrawal. Therefore, the observed sensorimotor activation may reflect motor preparation for subsequent button-press responses associated with compensation rather than a general tendency toward shame-related avoidance. Future studies that permit participants to engage in actual avoidance behaviors could further clarify the role of sensorimotor areas in shame processing.
In line with previous research (Cohen et al., 2011), our findings reveal that both types of trait guilt were significantly associated with compensatory behavior, whereas neither type of trait shame exhibited such an association. Furthermore, we found neural responses in the aMCC mediated the relationship between trait guilt (i.e., repair action tendencies) and compensation. A substantial body of research has revealed that guilt processing consistently activates the aMCC (see a meta-analysis, Gifuni et al., 2017). It is suggested that, in the context of guilt, the aMCC plays a role in detecting the conflict between social norms and actual behavior, and signaling this conflict via generating negative emotions (Bastin et al., 2016; Gifuni et al., 2017). In addition, Yu et al. (2014) linked the aMCC activity with compensatory behavior. Accordingly, our fMRI findings suggest that individuals with stronger tendency to engage in compensation across various moral violation scenarios (indicated by their trait guilt scores) are more sensitive to the severity of the violation, experience stronger negative emotions, and therefore engage in greater compensatory behavior.
Our study makes several innovative contributions. First, we developed a novel paradigm that effectively elicits guilt and shame at comparable intensities, enabling researchers to systematically explore the associations among guilt, shame, their cognitive antecedents, and behavioral consequences. Future studies examining additional research questions related to guilt and shame can integrate this paradigm with other cognitive neuroscience techniques, such as electroencephalography (EEG) and magnetoencephalography (MEG). Second, our behavioral findings provide high-quality empirical evidence for functionalist theory, aligning with the contemporary trend of comprehending emotions through their adaptive functions (Landers, Sznycer, & Durkee, 2024; Sznycer et al., 2021). Third, our computational and neural findings offer a clear delineation of the neurocognitive mechanisms underlying guilt and shame. Building on knowledge that harm and responsibility are related to guilt and shame, our result further reveal that these cognitive antecedents are integrated in the form of a quotient. While previous studies have broadly identified brain regions associated with guilt and shame processing as a whole (Bastin et al., 2016; Gifuni et al., 2017; Piretti et al., 2023), our study advances this understanding by breaking down guilt and shame processing into distinct processes and precisely mapping the neural correlates of each process.
In conclusion, our findings support the functionalist theory by demonstrating distinct effects of harm and responsibility on guilt and shame, as well as differences in the efficiency with which guilt and shame translate into compensatory behaviors. Notably, harm and responsibility are integrated in the form of a quotient prior to influencing guilt-driven and shame-driven compensation. Furthermore, the distinct stages involved in guilt and shame processing correspond to activities in specific neural regions related to value computation, salience processing, theory-of-mind processing, self-referential processing, and cognitive control. By simultaneously providing computational, algorithmic, and neural accounts of guilt and shame (cf. Yu et al., 2024), our study advances the holistic understanding of these emotions, which provides insights into how guilt and shame can be regulated and informs the treatment of guilt- and shame-related mental disorders.
Data availability:
The behavioral data and a part of fMRI data are available on Open Science Framework (https://osf.io/sve7h/?view_only=10ee55af054d46a9907a827d3f042d8f). Raw fMRI data are available from the corresponding authors upon request due to privacy concern.
Code availability:
The codes used in the current study are available on Open Science Framework (https://osf.io/sve7h/?view_only=10ee55af054d46a9907a827d3f042d8f).
Author contributions:
Ruida Zhu and Chao Liu designed the experiment. Huanqing Wang implemented the study design and collected the data. Ruida Zhu carried out the analyses. Ruida Zhu and Chao Liu wrote the paper. All authors provided critical revisions and approved the final paper for submission.
Funding
This project has received funding from the Scientific and Technological Innovation (STI) 2030-Major Projects (2021ZD0200500 to Chao Liu), the National Natural Science Foundation of China (32200884 to Ruida Zhu; 32441109, 32271092, 32130045 to Chao Liu), the Open Research Fund of the State Key Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and Learning (CNLYB2404 to Ruida Zhu and Chao Liu), the Start-up Project for Support of Young Doctors (SL2023A04J00351 to Ruida Zhu), the Young Teachers’ Team Project of Basic Research Funds of Universities of Sun Yat-sen University (to Ruida Zhu), the Beijing Major Science and Technology Project under Contract No. Z241100001324005 (to Chao Liu), and the Opening Project of the State Key Laboratory of General Artificial Intelligence (SKLAGI20240P06 to Chao Liu).
References
- 1.The distinct roles of moral responsibility and group-based guilt in response to ingroup wrongdoingEuropean Journal of Social Psychology 41:825–839https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.826Google Scholar
- 2.The valuation system: A coordinate-based meta-analysis of BOLD fMRI experiments examining neural correlates of subjective valueNeuroimage 76:412–427https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.02.063Google Scholar
- 3.Feelings of shame, embarrassment and guilt and their neural correlates: A systematic reviewNeuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 71:455–471https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.09.019Google Scholar
- 4.Guilt: An interpersonal approachPsychological Bulletin 115:243–267https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.115.2.243Google Scholar
- 5.The role of the anterior insula in social norm compliance and enforcement: Evidence from coordinate-based and functional connectivity meta-analysesNeuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 92:378–389https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.06.024Google Scholar
- 6.Guilt and regret: The determining role of interpersonal and intrapersonal harmCognition and Emotion 18:55–70https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930244000435Google Scholar
- 7.Neural mechanisms of the influence of popularity on adolescent ratings of musicNeuroImage 49:2687–2696https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.10.070Google Scholar
- 8.Experiences of shame and guilt in anorexia and bulimia nervosa: A systematic reviewPsychology and Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice 93:134–159https://doi.org/10.1111/papt.12198Google Scholar
- 9.The roots of modern justice: Cognitive and neural foundations of social norms and their enforcementNature Neuroscience 15:655–661https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3087Google Scholar
- 10.Shame and guilt in the postnatal period: A systematic reviewJournal of Reproductive and Infant Psychology 39:67–85https://doi.org/10.1080/02646838.2020.1754372Google Scholar
- 11.Shame-proneness, guilt-proneness and anxiety symptoms: A meta-analysisJournal of Anxiety Disorders 58:78–106https://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2018.07.005Google Scholar
- 12.Affirmation, acknowledgment of in-group responsibility, group-based guilt, and support for reparative measuresJournal of Personality and Social Psychology 101:256–270https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023936Google Scholar
- 13.Triangulating the neural, psychological, and economic bases of guilt aversionNeuron 70:560–572https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2011.02.056Google Scholar
- 14.Informatic parcellation of the network involved in the computation of subjective valueSocial Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 9:1289–1302https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nst106Google Scholar
- 15.Introducing the GASP scale: a new measure of guilt and shame pronenessJournal of Personality and Social Psychology 100:947–966https://doi.org/10.1037/a0022641Google Scholar
- 16.How do you feel—now? The anterior insula and human awarenessNature Reviews Neuroscience 10:59–70https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2555Google Scholar
- 17.Significance of the insula for the evolution of human awareness of feelings from the bodyAnnals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1225:72–82https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.2011.05990.xGoogle Scholar
- 18.Moral transgressions corrupt neural representations of valueNature Neuroscience 20:879–885https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4557Google Scholar
- 19.Distinct regions of the medial prefrontal cortex are associated with self-referential processing and perspective takingJournal of Clinical Neuroscience 19:935–944https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2007.19.6.935Google Scholar
- 20.Bystander intervention in emergencies: Diffusion of responsibilityJournal of Personality and Social Psychology 8:377https://doi.org/10.1037/h0025589Google Scholar
- 21.The social side of shame: Approach versus withdrawalCognition and Emotion 32:1671–1677https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2017.1422696Google Scholar
- 22.Not so ugly after all: When shame acts as a commitment deviceJournal of Personality and Social Psychology 95:933–943https://doi.org/10.1037/a0011991Google Scholar
- 23.Moral sentiments and cooperation: Differential influences of shame and guiltCognition and Emotion 21:1025–1042https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930600980874Google Scholar
- 24.Restore and protect motivations following shameCognition and Emotion 24:111–127https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930802584466Google Scholar
- 25.No place to hide: When shame causes proselfs to cooperateJournal of Social Psychology 154:74–88https://doi.org/10.1080/00224545.2013.855158Google Scholar
- 26.Emotion, regulation, and moral developmentAnnual Reviews of Psychology 51:665–697https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.51.1.665Google Scholar
- 27.Shame and aggression: Theoretical considerationsAggression and Violent Behavior 19:447–453https://doi.org/10.1016/j.avb.2014.05.002Google Scholar
- 28.Appraisal theory: Old and new questionsEmotion Review 5:125–131https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073912463617Google Scholar
- 29.Neural signatures of fairness-related normative decision making in the ultimatum game: A coordinate-based meta-analysisHuman Brain Mapping 36:591–602https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.22649Google Scholar
- 30.Neurocomputational substrates underlying the effect of identifiability on third-party punishmentJournal of Neuroscience 43:8018–8031https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0460-23.2023Google Scholar
- 31.Development and neurophysiology of mentalizingPhilosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 358:459–473https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2002.1218Google Scholar
- 32.The Ultimatum Game and the brain: A meta-analysis of neuroimaging studiesNeuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 47:549–558https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.10.014Google Scholar
- 33.The psychological, computational, and neural foundations of indebtednessNature Communications 15:1–17https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-023-44286-9Google Scholar
- 34.The mutuality of social emotions: How the victim’s reactive attitude influences the transgressor’s emotional responsesNeuroImage 244:118631https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.118631Google Scholar
- 35.Distinguishing neural correlates of context-dependent advantageous-and disadvantageous-inequity aversionProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 115:E7680–E7689https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1802523115Google Scholar
- 36.Concern for self-image and social image in the management of moral failure: Rethinking shameEuropean Journal of Social Psychology 41:468–478https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.803Google Scholar
- 37.Guilt, Shame, and Reparative Behavior: The Effect of Psychological ProximityJournal of Business Ethics 114:311–323https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-012-1350-2Google Scholar
- 38.Neural mapping of guilt: A quantitative meta-analysis of functional imaging studiesBrain Imaging and Behavior 11:1164–1178https://doi.org/10.1007/s11682-016-9606-6Google Scholar
- 39.The evolution of social attractiveness and its role in shame, humiliation, guilt and therapyBritish Journal of Medical Psychology 70:113–147https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8341.1997.tb01893.xGoogle Scholar
- 40.How guilt leads to reparation? Exploring the processes underlying the effects of guiltMotivation and Emotion 41:343–352https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-017-9612-zGoogle Scholar
- 41.Integration of emotion and cognition in the lateral prefrontal cortexProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 99:4115–4120https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.062381899Google Scholar
- 42.The moral emotionsIn:
- Davidson R. J.
- Scherer K. R.
- Goldsmith H. H.
- 43.Individual differences in emotion processingCurrent Opinion in Neurobiology 14:233–238https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2004.03.010Google Scholar
- 44.Guilt, empathy, and apologyPersonality and Individual Differences 53:917–922https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2012.06.021Google Scholar
- 45.The right and the good: Distributive justice and neural encoding of equity and efficiencyScience 320:1092–1095https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1153651Google Scholar
- 46.Computational and neurobiological substrates of cost-benefit integration in altruistic helping decisionJournal of Neuroscience 41:3545–3561https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1939-20.2021Google Scholar
- 47.How do self-interest and other-need interact in the brain to determine altruistic behavior?NeuroImage 157:598–611https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.06.040Google Scholar
- 48.The inferior parietal lobule and temporoparietal junction: A network perspectiveNeuropsychologia 105:70–83https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.01.001Google Scholar
- 49.Why individuals protest the perceived transgressions of their country: The role of anger, shame, and guiltPersonality and Social Psychology Bulletin 33:572–587https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167206297402Google Scholar
- 50.Disentangling neural representations of value and salience in the human brainProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 111:5000–5005https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1320189111Google Scholar
- 51.The effects of feelings of guilt on the behaviour of uncooperative individuals in repeated social bargaining games: An affect-as-information interpretation of the role of emotion in social interactionCognition and Emotion 17:429–453https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930143000662Google Scholar
- 52.Shame, guilt, and depressive symptoms: A meta-analytic reviewPsychological Bulletin 137:68https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021466Google Scholar
- 53.Reinforcement learning signal predicts social conformityNeuron 61:140–151https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2008.11.027Google Scholar
- 54.Diminishing reciprocal fairness by disrupting the right prefrontal cortexScience 314:829–832https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1129156Google Scholar
- 55.Disrupting the prefrontal cortex diminishes the human ability to build a good reputationProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 106:20895–20899https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0911619106Google Scholar
- 56.Integration of error agency and representation of others’ pain in the anterior insulaJournal of Cognitive Neuroscience 25:258–272https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00324Google Scholar
- 57.The architecture of cognitive control in the human prefrontal cortexScience 302:1181–1185https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1088545Google Scholar
- 58.Generalization and differentiation in semantic memoryAnnals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1124:61–76https://doi.org/10.1196/annals.1440.006Google Scholar
- 59.ShameIn:
- Al-Shawaf L.
- Shackelford T. K.
- 60.Are self-conscious emotions about the self? Testing competing theories of shame and guilt across two disparate culturesEmotion 24:1–12https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0001321Google Scholar
- 61.Knowledge and appraisal in the cognition-emotion relationshipCognition and Emotion 2:281–300https://doi.org/10.1080/02699938808412701Google Scholar
- 62.Shame and guilt in neurosisInternational Universities Press Google Scholar
- 63.Guilty by association: How group-based (collective) guilt arises in the brainNeuroImage 209:116488https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116488Google Scholar
- 64.Neural and sociocultural mediators of ethnic differences in painNature Human Behaviour 4:517–530https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-020-0819-8Google Scholar
- 65.The grateful disposition: A conceptual and empirical topographyJournal of Personality and Social Psychology 82:112–127https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.82.1.112Google Scholar
- 66.Reconsidering the differences between shame and guiltEurope’s Journal of Psychology 14:710–733https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.v14i3.1564Google Scholar
- 67.Neurobiological underpinnings of shame and guilt: A pilot fMRI studySocial Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 9:150–157Google Scholar
- 68.Moral disengagementCurrent Opinion in Psychology 6:199–204https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.07.018Google Scholar
- 69.Appraisal theories of emotion: State of the art and future developmentEmotion Review 5:119–124https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073912468165Google Scholar
- 70.Neural underpinnings of individual differences in emotion regulation: A systematic reviewNeuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 162:105727https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2024.105727Google Scholar
- 71.A mixed-methods systematic review of offence-related shame and/or guilt in violent offendersAggression and Violent Behavior 78:1–12https://doi.org/10.1016/j.avb.2024.101989Google Scholar
- 72.Social value orientation: Theoretical and measurement issues in the study of social preferencesPersonality and Social Psychology Review 18:13–41https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868313501745Google Scholar
- 73.Measuring social value orientationJudgment and Decision Making 6:771–781https://doi.org/10.1017/S1930297500004204Google Scholar
- 74.Self-referential processing in our brain-A meta-analysis of imaging studies on the selfNeuroImage 31:440–457https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.12.002Google Scholar
- 75.The Enigmatic temporal pole: A review of findings on social and emotional processingBrain 130:1718–1731https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awm052Google Scholar
- 76.Distributed and overlapping cerebral representations of number, size, and luminance during comparative judgmentsNeuron 41:983–993https://doi.org/10.1016/S0896-6273(04)00107-2Google Scholar
- 77.The Neural signatures of shame, embarrassment, and guilt: A voxel-based meta-snalysis on gunctional neuroimaging dtudiesBrain Sciences 13:1–15https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci13040559Google Scholar
- 78.Primary motor and sensory cortex activation during motor performance and motor imagery: A functional magnetic resonance imaging studyJournal of Neuroscience 16:7688–7698https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.16-23-07688.1996Google Scholar
- 79.Increased amygdala response to shame in remitted major depressive disorderPLoS ONE 9:1–9https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0086900Google Scholar
- 80.The neurobiology of social decision-makingCurrent Opinion in Neurobiology 18:159–165https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2008.06.003Google Scholar
- 81.Reducing aggressive responses to social exclusion using transcranial direct current stimulationSocial Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 10:352–356https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsu053Google Scholar
- 82.Social cognition and the anterior temporal lobesNeuroImage 49:3452–3462https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.11.012Google Scholar
- 83.Changing social norm compliance with noninvasive brain stimulationScience 342:482–484https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1241399Google Scholar
- 84.The differential involvement of inferior parietal lobule in number comparison: A rTMS studyNeuropsychologia 42:1902–1909https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.05.005Google Scholar
- 85.Overlapping and non-overlapping brain regions for theory of mind and self reflection in individual subjectsSocial Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 1:229–234https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsl034Google Scholar
- 86.Fractionating theory of mind: A meta-analysis of functional brain imaging studiesNeuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 42:9–34https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.01.009Google Scholar
- 87.The role of shame and guilt in social anxiety disorderJournal of Affective Disorders Reports 6:100208https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadr.2021.100208Google Scholar
- 88.Be nice if you have to--the neurobiological roots of strategic fairnessSocial Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 10:790–796https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsu114Google Scholar
- 89.Cortical mechanisms of person representation: Recognition of famous and personally familiar namesNeuroImage 31:853–860https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.002Google Scholar
- 90.Forms and functions of the self-conscious emotionsTrends in Cognitive Sciences 23:143–157https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2018.11.007Google Scholar
- 91.Forms and functions of the social emotionsCurrent Directions in Psychological Science 30:292–299https://doi.org/10.1177/096372142110074Google Scholar
- 92.Shame closely tracks the threat of devaluation by others, even across culturesProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 113:2625–2630https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1514699113Google Scholar
- 93.Brain activation associated with evaluative processes of guilt and embarrassment: An fMRI studyNeuroImage 23:967–974https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.054Google Scholar
- 94.The association between guilt and prosocial behavior: A systematic review and meta-analysisAdvances in Psychological Science 27:773–788https://doi.org/10.3724/SP.J.1042.2019.00773Google Scholar
- 95.Situational determinan of shame and guilt in young adulthoodPersonality and Social Psychology Bulletin 18:199–206https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167292182011Google Scholar
- 96.Shame and guiltGuilford Press Google Scholar
- 97.Moral emotions and moral behaviorAnnual Review of Psychology 58:345–372https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.56.091103.070145Google Scholar
- 98.Relation of shame and guilt to constructive versus destructive responses to anger across the lifespanJournal of Personality and Social Psychology 70:797–809https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.70.4.797Google Scholar
- 99.Differential involvement of parietal and precentral regions in movement preparation and motor intentionJournal of Neuroscience 22:9024–9034https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-20-09024.2002Google Scholar
- 100.Dissociable roles of the anterior temporal regions in successful encoding of memory for person identity informationJournal of Cognitive Neuroscience 22:2226–2237https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21349Google Scholar
- 101.Decoding the charitable brain: Empathy, perspective taking, and attention shifts differentially predict altruistic givingJournal of Neuroscience 36:4719–4732https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3392-15.2016Google Scholar
- 102.Salience processing and insular cortical function and dysfunctionNature Reviews Neuroscience 16:55–61https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3857Google Scholar
- 103.Guilt-specific processing in the prefrontal cortexCerebral Cortex 21:2461–2470https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhr016Google Scholar
- 104.The motive cocktail in altruistic behaviorsNature Computational Science 4:659–676https://doi.org/10.1038/s43588-024-00685-6Google Scholar
- 105.Mortality salience enhances neural activities related to guilt and shame when recalling the pastCerebral Cortex 32:5145–5162https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhac004Google Scholar
- 106.A levels-of-analysis framework for studying social emotionsNature Reviews Psychology 3:198–213https://doi.org/10.1038/s44159-024-00285-1Google Scholar
- 107.Decomposing gratitude: Representation and integration of cognitive antecedents of gratitude in the brainJournal of Neuroscience 38:4886–4898https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2944-17.2018Google Scholar
- 108.The voice of conscience: Neural bases of interpersonal guilt and compensationSocial Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 9:1150–1158https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nst090Google Scholar
- 109.A generalizable multivariate brain pattern for interpersonal guiltCerebral Cortex 30:3558–3572https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhz326Google Scholar
- 110.Social concepts are represented in the superior anterior temporal cortexProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 104:6430–6435https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0607061104Google Scholar
- 111.Computational substrates of social norm enforcement by unaffected third partiesNeuroImage 129:95–104https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.01.040Google Scholar
- 112.Differentiating guilt and shame in an interpersonal context with univariate activation and multivariate pattern analysesNeuroImage 186:476–486https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.11.012Google Scholar
- 113.Relational utility affects self-punishment in direct and indirect reciprocity situationsSocial Psychology 48:19–27https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000291Google Scholar
- 114.Early distinction between shame and guilt processing in an interpersonal contextSocial Neuroscience 14:53–66https://doi.org/10.1080/17470919.2017.1391119Google Scholar
- 115.The effect of shame on anger at others: Awareness of the emotion-causing events mattersCognition and Emotion 33:696–708https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2018.1489782Google Scholar
- 116.Brain responses to social norms: Meta-analyses of fMRI studiesHuman Brain Mapping 39:955–970https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.23895Google Scholar
Article and author information
Author information
Version history
- Sent for peer review:
- Preprint posted:
- Reviewed Preprint version 1:
Cite all versions
You can cite all versions using the DOI https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.107223. This DOI represents all versions, and will always resolve to the latest one.
Copyright
© 2025, Zhu et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
- views
- 98
- downloads
- 0
- citations
- 0
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.