Author response:
We thank the reviewers and editors for their careful and constructive assessment of our manuscript. We have provided a provisional response to the eLife assessment and the reviewer’s public comments below, addressing their main concerns and outlining our planned revisions that we believe will substantially strengthen our paper.
eLife Assessment
This study presents a valuable finding on the representational structure of task encoding in the prefrontal cortex. The evidence supporting the claims of the authors is solid, representing an impressive data collection effort and best-practice fMRI analyses. However, at least including visual regions as a control and controlling for behavioral differences in the task in representation analyses would have strengthened the study. The work will be of interest to cognitive neuroscientists interested in the neural basis of cognitive control.
We plan to address both specific methodological weaknesses mentioned in the assessment in our forthcoming revision. First, the revision will include analyses of an early visual cortex ROI as an additional control region, allowing us to test whether the primary auditory cortex findings generalize to the sensory cortex across input modalities. Preliminary results indicate that the early visual cortex ROI exhibits a similar pattern of results, with evidence for coding both task-relevant and task-irrelevant visual dimensions across both tasks, as well as the context dimension specifically in the hierarchy task. Second, we will include behavioral performance as a covariate for the relevant statistical comparison across tasks to mitigate concerns over performance-related confounds. In addition, we will include a set of control analyses that demonstrate that equating the amount of data for pattern analyses across the two tasks by subsampling from the hierarchy task, while reducing our overall power, does not appreciably alter our results. We note that our analyses of representational geometries relied only on neural data from correct trials and, in the first-level modelling of the fMRI data, already controlled for differences in trial-by-trial response times. Therefore, our analyses of decoding and representation similarity are not directly affected by differences in performance across the two tasks. Finally, we have provided clarifications regarding Reviewer 2’s questions about the size and construction of the regions of interest employed in the study, as well as about the language employed to discuss null results.
Public Reviews:
Reviewer #1 (Public review):
Summary:
Bhandari and colleagues present tour-de-force analyses that compare the representational geometry in the lateral prefrontal cortex and primary auditory cortex between two complex cognitive control tasks, with one having a "flat" structure where subjects are asked to form rote memory of all the stimulus-action mappings in the task and one having a "hierarchical" task structure that allows clustering of task conditions and that renders certain stimulus dimensions irrelevant for choices. They discovered that the lPFC geometry is high-dimensional in nature in that it allows above-chance separation between different dichotomies of task conditions. The separability is significantly higher for task-relevant features than task-irrelevant ones. They also found task features that are represented in an "abstract" format (e.g., audio features), i.e., the neural representation generalizes across specific task conditions that share this variable. The neural patterns in lPFC are highly relevant for behaviors as they are correlated with subjects' reaction times and choices.
Strengths:
Typically, geometry in coding patterns is reflected in single-unit firings; this manuscript demonstrates that such geometry can be recovered using fMRI BOLD signals, which is both surprising and important. The tasks are well designed and powerful in revealing the differences in neural geometry, and analyses are all done in a rigorous way. I am thus very enthusiastic about this paper and identify no major issues.
I am curious about the consequence of dimensionality collapse in lPFC. The authors propose a very interesting idea that separability is critical for cognitive control; indeed, separability is high for task-relevant information. What happens when task-relevant separation is low or task-irrelevant separation is high, and will this lead to behavioral errors? Maybe a difference score between the separability of task-relevant and taskirrelevant features is a signature of the strength of cognitive control?
We appreciate the reviewers’ positive evaluation of our paper.
Weaknesses:
The authors show a difference between flat and hierarchical tasks, but the two tasks are different in accuracy, with the flat task having more errors. Will this difference in task difficulty/errors contribute to the task differences in results reported?
To address the Reviewer’s concern about the difference in behavioural performance between the two tasks influencing our results, we will take several approaches. First, we will include behavioral performance as a covariate for the relevant statistical comparison across tasks. This should ensure that any differences we observe across tasks are over and above those that can be explained by the difference in behavioral performance. Second, we will include a set of decoding analyses that control for differences in performance across the tasks. We note that all our analyses of representational geometries relied on neural data from correct trials only. In addition, the first-level modelling of the fMRI data already controlled for trial-by-trial variability in response times. Therefore, our decoding and representation similarity analyses should not directly be affected by differences in performance across the two tasks. However, one possible issue with this approach is that the larger number of errors in the flat task means that less data was available for estimating multivoxel patterns in the flat task compared to the hierarchy task, resulting in differential power to detect decoding effects across the two tasks. We note that the on average, this difference was not substantial: on average, 21.7 runs were available per participant for the flat task, while 23.8 runs per participant were available for the hierarchy task. Moreover, rerunning our analyses with the number of runs equated for each participant does not meaningfully alter the pattern of results. These additional analyses will be included in the supplement in the forthcoming revised manuscript.
Reviewer #2 (Public review):
Summary:
The authors study the influence of tasks on the representational geometry of the lPFC and auditory cortex (AC). In particular, they use two context-dependent tasks: a task with a hierarchical structure and a task with a flat structure, in which each context/stimulus maps to a specific response. Their primary finding is that the representational geometry in the lPFC, in contrast to AC, aligns with the optimal organization of the task. They conclude that the geometry of representations adapts, or is tailored, to the task in the lPFC, therefore supporting control processes.
Strengths:
(1) Dataset:
The dataset is impressive and well-sampled. Having data from both tasks collected in the same subjects is a great property. If it is publicly available, it will be a significant contribution to the community.
(2) Choice of methods:
The choice of analyses are largely well-suited towards the questions at hand - crosscondition generalization, RSA + regression, in combination with ANOVAs, are well-suited to characterizing task representations.
(3) I found some of their results, in particular, those presented in Figures 4 and 5, to be particularly compelling.
(4) The correlation analysis with behavior is also a nice result.
We thank the reviewer for noting the strengths of the paper. We respond to the weaknesses noted below.
Weaknesses:
(1) Choice of ROIs:
A strength of fMRI is its spatial coverage of the whole brain. In this study, however, the authors focus on only two ROIs: the lPFC and auditory cortex. Though I understand the justification for choosing lPFC from decades of research, the choice of AC as a control feels somewhat arbitrary - AC is known to have worse SNR in fMRI data, and limiting a 'control' to a single region seems arbitrary. For example, why not also include visual regions, given that the task also involves two visual features?
We agree with the reviewer that the whole-brain fMRI data certainly provide ample opportunities to explore the nature of these representations across the brain. Our focus in this paper is squarely on the principles of coding and flexibility in the lPFC. We believe that a whole-brain exploration addresses a separate question that would be out of the scope of this study. To clarify, we are not arguing that the lPFC is the only region in the brain that employs the coding principles that our study brings to light. Our contention is only that lPFC employs these principles, and it differs at least from the primary sensory cortex. The questions of whether these principles generalize beyond lPFC (quite likely) and, if so, how broadly, are distinct from the ones addressed in the manuscript. We intend to follow up with another manuscript that addresses these questions.
Nevertheless, given the focus of this paper, we agree that a second control region, which allows one to test if the primary auditory cortex findings generalize to the sensory cortex more broadly, would strengthen our claims. We will include an early visual cortex ROI in our forthcoming revision. Preliminary results indicate that the early visual cortex ROI shows a similar set of findings – with evidence for coding of task-relevant and taskirrelevant visual dimensions across both tasks, but also specifically the context dimension in the hierarchy task. These results will be detailed in the forthcoming revision
(2) Construction of ROIs:
The choice and construction of the ROIs feel a bit arbitrary, as the lPFC region was constructed out of 10 parcels from Schaefer, while the AC was constructed from a different methodology (neurosynth). Did both parcels have the same number of voxels/vertices? It would be helpful to include a visualization of these masks as a figure.
We defined the lPFC ROIs by selecting Schaefer parcels in the frontal lobe that were previously mapped onto the Control A resting state network identified by Yeo et al. (2011). This network aligns with the multiple-demand network, which has also been identified in the macaque, where it includes the lPFC regions that abut the principal sulcus. Prior results from these regions in the monkey brain provide the scientific premise for our hypotheses. The two lPFC ROIs in each hemisphere were constructed out of 5 Schaefer parcels in each hemisphere. These parcels cluster into the same functional network and tend to behave similarly in univariate analyses. Given that our hypotheses do not distinguish between the different parcels, we elected to improve power by merging them into left and right dlPFC ROIs.
On the other hand, the same approach could not be used to identify the primary auditory cortex. As Yeo et al. noted in their paper, the 17 resting state networks they identify did not adequately parcellate somatomotor and auditory cortices into distinct networks, likely due to their proximity (see Fig 14 and related text in Yeo et al. (2011)). We therefore relied on a different approach to define the primary auditory cortex, using an association test in Neurosynth to obtain a map of regions associated with the term “primary auditory”. In the revised manuscript, we will also include a primary auditory cortex ROI, defined again using a term-based association test in Neurosynth.
Our lPFC ROIs and pAC ROIs are of similar size. In the left hemisphere, the lPFC ROI (constructed from merging Schaefer parcels 128-thru-132) has, on average, 624.55 voxels. The left pAC ROI (defined with Neurosynth) has, on average, 628 voxels. In the right hemisphere, the lPFC ROI (constructed from merging Schaefer parcels 330-thru334), has 470.8 voxels on average. The right pAC ROI has, on average, 568 voxels. A table reporting the size of our parcels and ROIs was included in the supplement. In our forthcoming revision, we will additionally include a supplementary figure visualizing the ROI masks.
(3) Task dimensionality:
In some ways, the main findings - that representation dimensionality is tailored to the task - seem to obviously follow from the choice of two tasks, particularly from a normative modeling perspective. For example, the flat task is effectively a memorization task, and is incompressible in the sense that there are no heuristics to solve it. In contrast, the hierarchical task can have several strategies, an uncompressed (memorized) strategy, and a compressed strategy. This is analogous to other studies evaluating representations during 'rich' vs. 'lazy'/kernel learning in ANNs. However, it seems unlikely (if not impossible) to form a 'rich' representation in the flat task. Posed another way, the flat task will always necessarily have a higher dimensionality than the hierarchical task. Thus, is their hypothesis - that representational geometry is tailored to the task - actually falsifiable? I understand the authors posit alternative hypotheses, e.g., "a fully compressed global axis with no separation among individual stimulus inputs could support responding [in the flat task]" (p. 36). But is this a realistic outcome, for example, in the space of all possible computational models performing this task? I understand that directly addressing this comment is challenging (without additional data collection or modeling work), but perhaps some additional discussion around this would be helpful.
We thank the reviewer for this comment, which gives us a chance to clarify our argument.
As noted by the reviewer, whether a network takes advantage of the compressibility of a task depends on its learning regime (i.e. rich vs lazy). One way to frame our question regarding the lPFC’s coding strategy, then, is to ask whether it operates in a rich or a lazy learning regime (which would predict, respectively, task-tailored vs task-agnostic representations). The reviewer’s concern is that the two task structures we employed are differentially compressible, and therefore, it is inevitable that we observe tailored representations and therefore, our hypotheses are not falsifiable.
First, it is important to clarify the theoretical premise behind our design and how it relates logically to our hypotheses. Under a lazy learning regime, a network would encode highdimensional representations of both tasks, regardless of their compressibility. On the other hand, under a rich learning regime, representational dimensionality will likely be shaped by the tasks’ structure. If the two tasks differ in their compressibility, only in the rich learning regime would the network learn representations of different dimensionality. Therefore, observing representations with dimensionality tailored to the task structure rules out the possibility that the lPFC is operating in a lazy regime. Therefore, the hypotheses are certainly testable.
The second point of clarification is that, contrary to the reviewer’s assertion, the flat task is, in fact, compressible – the task can be solved with a categorical representation of the response categories, with no sensitivity to the different specific stimuli within each category. Indeed, it is possible to train a simple, three-layer feedforward artificial neural network to perform the flat task perfectly with only 2 units in the hidden layer, demonstrating this compressibility. While we agree with the reviewer that in the space of all possible architectures one might consider the two tasks may differ in compressibility, particularly at the local levels, as we noted above, this does not imply that our hypotheses are not testable.
Finally, as a third point of clarification, our focus in this paper is on understanding the nature of coding in the lPFC in particular. Arguments based on a normative modelling perspective properly apply to the representations learned by an agent (such as an ANN or a human) as a whole. In a minimal feedforward ANN with a single hidden layer trained in a regime which encourages compression (i.e. a rich learning regime), it would indeed be the case that the representational dimensionality in that hidden layer would be higher for less compressible tasks. However, when applied to humans, such an argument applies to the brain as a whole rather than to an individual region of the brain like the lPFC. As such, it is less straightforward to predict how a single region might represent a task without additional information about the region’s inputs, outputs and broader position in a network. Even for a highly compressible task, a particular brain region may nevertheless be sensitive to all task dimensions. Conversely, even when a task is not compressible, a particular population within the brain may be invariant to some task features. For example, the primary auditory cortex is expected to be invariant to visual task dimensions.
Therefore, how a task is represented in the lPFC in particular (as opposed to the whole brain) depends on its computational function and coding principles, which remain debated. For instance, as some accounts (such as the guided activation theory) posit, if the primary function of the lPFC is to encode ‘context’ and shape downstream processing based on context, we might only expect to see the abstract coding of the auditory context in the hierarchy task (and, perhaps, the response categories across both tasks as they encode the ’context’ for the lower-level response decision), while being invariant to lowerlevel features of the input. In our paper, we specifically contrast two accounts of lPFC coding that have emerged in the literature – one positing that the lPFC learns a representation tailored to the structure of the task, and another that the lPFC encodes a high-dimensional representation that privileges sensitivity to many task features and their non-linear mixture at the cost of generalization. Regardless of the compressibility of the tasks in question, how the lPFC encodes the two tasks is an empirical question.
In our forthcoming revision, we will clarify these points in the discussion. We will also include the results of neural network simulations alluded to above.
(4) Related to the above:
The authors have a section on p. 27: "Local structure of lPFC representational geometry of the flat task shows high separability with no evidence for abstraction" - I understand a generalization analysis can be done in the feature space, but in practice, the fact that the flat task doubles as a memorization task implies that there are no useful abstractions, so it seems to trivially follow that there would be no abstract representations. In fact, the use of task abstractions in the stimulus space would be detrimental to task performance here. I could understand the use of this analysis as a control, but the phrasing of this section seems to indicate that this is a surprising result.
As explained above, there is no need for high local separability in the flat task. The lPFC could have completely abstracted over the individual trial-types that contributed to each response category, encoding only the response categories. Indeed, as also noted above, it is possible to train a simple, three-layer feedforward artificial neural network to perform the flat task perfectly with only 2 units in the hidden layer. The two hidden layer units code for each of the two response categories.
(5) Statistical inferences:
Throughout the manuscript, the authors appear to conflate failure to reject the null with acceptance of the null. For example, p. 24: "However, unlike left lPFC, paired t-tests showed no reliable difference in the separability of the task-relevant features vs the orthogonal, task-irrelevant features... Therefore, the overall separability of pAC representations is not shaped by either task-relevance of task structure."
We thank the reviewer for pointing these out. These sentences will be corrected in the revision. For instance, the sentence above will be modified to “Therefore, we find no evidence that the overall separability of pAC representations is shaped by either taskrelevance or task structure.”
Reviewer #3 (Public review):
Summary:
In this paper, Bhandari, Keglovits, et al. explore the representational structure of task encoding in the lateral prefrontal cortex. Through an impressive fMRI data-collection effort, they compare and contrast neural representations across tasks with different highlevel stimulus-response structures. They find that the lateral prefrontal cortex shows enhanced encoding of task-relevant information, but that most of these representations do not generalize across conditions (i.e., have low abstraction). This appears to be driven in part by the representation of task conditions being clustered by the higher-order task properties ('global' representations), with poor generalization across these clusters ('local' representations). Overall, this paper provides an interesting account of how task representations are encoded in the PFC.
Strengths:
(1) Impressive dataset, which may provide further opportunities for investigating prefrontal representations.
(2) Clever task design, allowing the authors to confound several features within a complex paradigm.
(3) Best-practice analysis for decoding, similarity analyses, and assessments of representational geometry.
(4) Extensive analyses to quantify the structure of PFC task representations.
Weaknesses:
(1) The paper would benefit from improved presentational clarity: more scaffolding of design and analysis decisions, clearer grounding to understand the high-level interpretations of the analyses (e.g., context, cluster, abstraction), and better visualizations of the key findings.
(2) The paper would benefit from stronger theoretical motivation for the experimental design, as well as a refined discussion on the implications of these findings for theories of cognitive control.
We thank the reviewer for highlighting the strengths of our paper and their feedback on the writing. We have reviewed these helpful suggestions with an eye to which we may implement in our revision to improve clarity. Our forthcoming revision will 1) provide clearer scaffolding to aid the reader in understanding our design, analyses and our interpretation of the results 2) incorporate the MDS-based visualization of the representational geometries, which is currently presented in the Supplement, as a figure panel in the main text, 3) provide a justification for the particular task structures we picked in the introduction and 4) incorporate a new paragraph in the Discussion section to highlight the implications of our findings for cognitive control.