Abstract
Hunger is a motivational drive that promotes feeding, and it can be generated by the physiological need to consume nutrients as well as the hedonic properties of food. Brain circuits and mechanisms that regulate feeding have been described, but which of these contribute to the generation of motive forces that drive feeding is unclear. Here, we describe our first efforts at behaviorally and neuronally distinguishing hedonic from homeostatic hunger states in Drosophila melanogaster and propose that this system can be used as a model to dissect the molecular mechanisms that underlie feeding motivation. We visually identify and quantify behaviors exhibited by hungry flies and find that increased feeding duration is a behavioral signature of hedonic feeding motivation. Using a genetically-encoded marker of neuronal activity, we find that the mushroom body (MB) lobes are activated by hedonic food environments, and we use optogenetic inhibition to implicate a dopaminergic neuron cluster (PAM) to α’/β’ MB circuit in hedonic feeding motivation. The identification of discrete hunger states in flies and the development of behavioral assays to measure them offers a framework to begin dissecting the molecular and circuit mechanisms that generate motivational states in the brain.
Introduction
Hedonist philosophers believed that pleasure is the supreme goal of life and that behavior is determined by the pursuit of it (Aristotle et al., 2004). Hunger for tasty delicacies, for example, is a particularly potent motivator of behavior, as it can be promoted not only by a physiological need to consume nutrients, but also by the hedonic properties of food. Due to its dichotomous nature, defining behavioral and neural correlates of hunger is a challenge that has perplexed scientists for over a century. Early physiologists wondered how to explain hunger and where it might originate, and they eventually described hunger as a dull ache or gnawing sensation arising from the stomach or abdominal region (Cannon & Washburn, 1993). This historical description has aided the discovery of hormones like ghrelin, insulin, and others that communicate information from the periphery to the brain and have roles in controlling feeding in response to energetic or nutritional needs, a state we now refer to as homeostatic hunger. In the decades since these discoveries, it has become clear that peripheral signals and others are integrated in the central nervous system to promote changes in behavior and reward signaling, which we interpret as indicators of hunger, but the nature of the motivational brain systems that drive these outputs remains less straight forward to identify and quantify.
Homeostatic feeding, or “need-based” intake, is defined as food intake that is necessary to ensure caloric and nutritional needs are met, and it has been quantified by measuring bulk food intake. Such measures may be of total caloric intake or consumption of specific nutrients, but it is unclear whether these measures are representative of hunger that is driven by energetic need, nutrient deficiency, or some other motivation, which has led to variation in the way measures of homeostatic hunger are reported and interpreted (for review of mammalian brain circuits that modulate feeding, see (Alcantara et al., 2022; Andermann & Lowell, 2017; Saper et al., 2002)). However, the observation that most animals, including humans, regulate food intake around a protein target and the identification of discrete neuronal populations that regulate protein-specific feeding in model systems suggest that levels of homeostatic hunger may be primarily determined by the amount of protein an animal requires and thus, in recent years, protein intake has become a common method for measuring need-based hunger (Gosby et al., 2011; Liu et al., 2017; Ro et al., 2016; Simpson et al., 2003; Weaver et al., 2022).
Our understanding of hunger has continued to evolve, due in part to the observation that the pleasurable effects of food also stimulate feeding independently of physiological need, a state referred to as hedonic hunger (Lowe & Butryn, 2007). Hedonic, or “need-free”, intake is thought to be evoked by the incentive salience or tastiness of food, and thus, the sweetness of the food environment is predicted to play a larger role (Lowe & Butryn, 2007). Indeed, hedonic hunger is likely the predominant driver of feeding in modern human environments where food is readily available and energy deprivation is seldom experienced. Hedonic responses to various food or environmental manipulations have been described as either “liking” responses (relating to the pleasure of a rewarding experience) or “wanting” responses (relating to the motivation to seek a reward), which arise from different neuronal substrates (reviewed in (Berridge, 2009)). Accordingly, “liking” is measured in rats by tracking facial responses in response to sweet tastants, while “wanting” is measured by tracking latency to search for and consume a tasty food treat or by using the novelty suppressed feeding assay that attempts to measure an animal’s ability to overcome an aversive environment to consume a food reward (Arcego et al., 2020; Pecina et al., 2006). While observations like these have greatly aided our ability to identify hedonic hunger states in mammals, much less is known about their neuronal and mechanistic origins relative to homeostatic hunger.
Although we now understand that hunger stratifies into two distinct states, one driven by need and the other by pleasure, we have a limited understanding of their behavioral and neuronal distinctions and connections. In mammals, brain circuits that modulate homeostatic or hedonic appetites have been identified. The well-described AGRP neurons in the arcuate nucleus of the hypothalamus, for example, are critical components of a circuit for homeostatic feeding (for review, see (Sternson & Eiselt, 2017)). They express receptors for peripherally released hormones that signal energetic state, are more active upon food deprivation, and transmit a negative valence signal that drives animals to find and consume food when the need arises (Betley et al., 2015; Denis et al., 2015; Willesen et al., 1999; Zigman et al., 2006). In addition to systems that seem to motivate homeostatic feeding, distinct circuits have also been associated with hedonic feeding including the monoaminergic populations that express serotonin and/or dopamine, and the mesolimbic dopamine pathway (Ahn et al., 2022; Altherr et al., 2021; Denis et al., 2015; Ghiglieri et al., 1997; Rossi & Stuber, 2018). While there is evidence to suggest that both branches of hunger are modulated by homeostatic state and hedonic feedback (e.g., (Denis et al., 2015; Krashes et al., 2011; Zigman et al., 2006)), the mechanistic origins that determine how various hunger drives work together and independently to generate feeding responses have yet to be fully described.
Simpler model systems, such as the fruit fly, Drosophila melanogaster, have proven valuable for insights into the neural circuitry that modulates feeding, but the nature of their hunger drives is unknown. Whether flies experience hedonic hunger, for example, is unclear, although it seems likely that they do; hungry flies learn to associate odors with a sucrose reward and when fed a high-sugar diet they overeat and become obese (Huetteroth et al., 2015; May et al., 2019). These behaviors are all thought to occur through mechanisms involving dopaminergic neurons in the fly brain which, when artificially activated, promote foraging and food intake (Tsao et al., 2018). Honeybees have also exhibited signatures of food wanting, a component of hedonic hunger, which manifests through activation of dopaminergic populations in the brain that are reflective of reward expectation (Huang et al., 2022).
To better understand the different motive forces that drive feeding, it seems clear that there is a need to move beyond measures of food intake or foraging behavior, which have been useful for identifying external and internal factors that change the way an animal eats, in order to dissect when and to what degree animals may be experiencing different types of hunger. Here we describe our first efforts to address this need by generating clear behavioral definitions of hunger states in Drosophila with the hope of providing an attractive system for defining the molecular features of their independent roles and the relationships between them. We characterize a repertoire of feeding micro-behaviors in the fly and use them to argue that Drosophila experience both homeostatic and hedonic hunger. We identify neural correlates of hedonic hunger in specific lobes of the Drosophila mushroom body and show that they are required for hedonic feeding. Finally, we develop a framework to examine the relationships between hedonic and homeostatic hunger states and present evidence for the existence of a system that acts as a potentiator of the relationship between them. Translating findings about the nature of feeding drives and the relationships among them could have far reaching implications for obesity and energy balance research and other behaviors influenced by motivational drives, including drug addiction and eating disorders.
Results
We set out to complement and enrich traditional measures of feeding by identifying new behavioral metrics that might be useful for classifying different hunger states. Fly feeding behavior is typically described as changes in volumetric food intake, which is difficult to accurately quantify at the individual level due to the small amount of food ingested (Deshpande et al., 2014; Shell et al., 2021; Shell et al., 2018), or by using automated systems that detect physical interactions with food sources (Itskov et al., 2014; Ro et al., 2014). These automated devices have revealed rhythmic patterns of feeding behavior in flies that mimic feeding strategies in other animals, suggesting that the behaviors are highly organized and centrally regulated. They are, however, unable to distinguish different stereotypical actions that a freely-moving fly might exhibit when interacting with a food source. Detailed behavioral characterizations of this sort have proven to be useful read-outs of neural states in other contexts and have the potential to inform our understanding of the motivational states that drive feeding (Chen et al., 2002; Duistermars et al., 2018; Ning et al., 2022). Much like human infants, for example, facial expressions and tongue movements in rats in response to appetitive tastants have been used to identify hedonic reactions and the brain systems that mediate them (Berridge, 1996).
To identify nuances in individual fly feeding, we developed video systems for manual and high-throughput annotation of what flies do when they eat. In all cases, we recorded freely-moving flies placed in feeding chambers of the Fly Liquid-Food Interaction Counter (FLIC) system, which is designed to continuously record individual flies’ interactions with food sources (Ro et al., 2014). We began by simply recording flies in the FLIC and manually curating each video in its entirety (Fig. 1A). With the hope of observing the greatest variety of behaviors, we collected observations from flies ranging from 5-20 days old, from mated female and male sated and starved flies, from flies housed singly or in groups of up to three, and from flies interacting with 5% or 10% sucrose food. We observed 30 flies and nearly 300 “events”, which were demarcated by intervals greater than one second during which the fly was not in physical contact with the food. Events were easy to identify, and different behavioral characteristics associated with events were easy to distinguish from each other (Slooten, 1994). Rare events (observed in fewer than 5% of flies) were not considered further, and events during which sight of the fly was out of focus or obscured (typically 1-2 events per fly) were ignored. Based on the remaining events, we created a library of user-defined behaviors that captured common, distinct, and definable interactions of flies with their food (Table 1).
We classified the behaviors we observed into five main categories: two non-feeding behaviors (“other” and “interaction” events) and three feeding behaviors (“fast” feeding events, “long” feeding events, and “long/quick” feeding events) (Table 1). The first feeding event type comprised fast feeding events (termed “F” or “FI”) during which we observed contact of the proboscis with the food for less than four seconds. These types of events were common (occurring in ∼95% of flies, Fig. 1B, 1C) and were sometimes simultaneous with the interaction of a fly’s legs with the food. Longer feeding events (termed “L”, “LI”, or “LQ”) were also identified, during which time we observed contact of the proboscis with the food for more than four seconds. Long events were less common than F-type events and could also include simultaneous interaction of legs with food. Long feeding events were either a continuous and sustained interaction of the proboscis with the food (“L” and “LI”, occurring in ∼85% of flies, Fig. 1B, 1C) or, less commonly, a continuous interaction during which the proboscis moved quickly in and out of the food (“LQ”, occurring in ∼60% of flies, Fig. 1B, 1C). The first type of non-feeding behavior was termed an interaction event (“IF” or “IH”) during which the fly interacted with the food only using its legs; no proboscis contact was observed. These putative tasting interactions often occurred in a “pitter-patter” motion, as if the fly were “playing” with their food, and they occurred in greater than 90% of flies (Fig. 1B, 1C). Lastly, we occasionally observed other non-feeding events (“G” or “UC”) that, nevertheless, often occurred just before or after feeding (occurring in ∼60% of flies, Fig. 1B, 1C). These tended to be various grooming behaviors, including using the forelegs to rub the head after interacting with food.
We next asked whether our behavioral classification could be used to distinguish hungry from sated flies. Recognizing a need to significantly increase the number of observed feeding events to provide sufficient statistical power to detect differences in behavioral tendencies, we designed a direct communication link between our custom DTrack video software and the FLIC system to trigger video capture only when there was physical contact between a fly and the food source, together with 5s windows before and after the contact. This allowed us to create video compilations of feeding behaviors for fully fed or starved (24hr) flies over several hours. Over 500 distinct events in these compilations were then manually scored using our classification of feeding behaviors, and comparisons were made between fully-fed and starved flies for each behavioral category in Table 1. We observed that starved flies exhibited a greater frequency of feeding behaviors overall, as expected (sated event frequency = 0.606 events/minute compared to starved event frequency = 0.788 events/minute, p = 0.061, data not shown), and that the relative frequency with which L-type feeding events occurred was increased (Fig. 1D, quantified at right). The observation that starved flies exhibit more frequent long-duration events is consistent with recent reports that starved flies increase “sips per burst” – a measure that is similar to our definition of L and LQ events (Itskov et al., 2014).
Qualitative aspects of the relationships between individual behaviors - that is, the architecture that results from sequences of behaviors - can reveal complexities that may also distinguish between different neural states (Casarrubea et al., 2019; Casarrubea et al., 2015; Flavell et al., 2022). Thus, we asked whether certain sequential patterns in fly feeding behavior appear repeatedly and whether they are affected by starvation. To do this, we used classic sequence analysis to map the probability that one event follows another in a sequence more often than what is expected to occur by chance alone - an approach that has been useful for identifying stereotyped patterns or structures of behaviors that tend to occur together (Ning et al., 2022; Slooten, 1994). We observed that hungry flies, whom we had starved for 24 hours, but not sated flies, followed F feeding events with additional F feeding events (transitional probability = 0.6 in starved flies with p-value <0.001, compared to a non-significant transitional probability = 0.55 in fully-fed flies) and L-type feeding events with additional L-type feeding events (transitional probability = 0.439 in starved flies with p-value <0.001 compared to non-significant transitional probability = 0.1538 in fully-fed flies) more often than would be expected by chance (Fig. 1E). In contrast, fully-fed flies were likely to engage in F-type events after grooming or interaction events and were more likely to groom following most other events, which may be reflective of a less goal-oriented behavioral state compared to that of starved flies (Fig. 1E).
Having established that long-duration events occur more frequently and in a predictable sequence in starved flies, we sought to replicate this finding in flies during their normal circadian “meal-time”, when they are presumably physiologically hungry but not yet starved. When monitored over several days on the FLIC, flies exhibited circadian meal patterns that corresponded to the light cycle, with one peak in the morning when lights turn on and a second peak in the evening when lights turn off (e.g. Fig. 1 – Fig. Supp. 1). We obtained 24hr video compilations of nearly 700 feeding events in the FLIC and used the flies’ endogenous feeding rhythm to designate “meal” and “non-meal” times of day. We manually scored the videos during each time for feeding events (fast, long, and long/quick events). We found that during mealtimes flies exhibited a significantly greater frequency of LQ events, while the frequencies of F and L events were statistically indistinguishable between times of day (Fig 1F). Thus, both starved flies and flies during their circadian meal-time exhibit an increase in the total number of feeding events together with an increase in the relative frequency of long feeding events.
The FLIC generates several quantitative signal measures when flies interact with food, and we asked whether any of these numerical metrics might substitute for direct visual observation and distinguish among fast feeding events, long feeding events, and interactions. To determine how signal measures change in accordance with a fly’s behavior, we scored 15 video compilations (resulting in 332 independent events) of flies interacting on the FLIC for F-type, L-type, or I-type events, and we matched each visually identified behavior to its corresponding FLIC signal data (e.g., Figs. 2A-2C). We evaluated differences in the characteristics of signal intensity for each event (i.e., its variance, minimum, maximum, average, and total) as well as the duration of the signal from each event. We found that only signal duration effectively distinguished long events from both fast events and interactions (Fig. 2D-2I). Thus, we established that longer signal durations from the FLIC are representative of L-type feeding events. We also noted that maximum signal intensity and the variance of the signal can distinguish interactions from both types of feeding events, which may be broadly useful to distinguish non-feeding interactions (i.e., tasting events) from feeding events (Ro et al., 2014).
Development of an assay to identify homeostatic and hedonic feeding
Having established that hungry flies increase the duration of their feeding events as well as the total number of events (i.e., Figure 3A, top panel), we next asked if we might be able to identify distinct hunger drives in flies and, if so, whether our behavioral and quantitative metrics would distinguish between them. Recognizing that the original assays used for behavioral observation, in which flies were exposed to a single, homogenous food source, were likely insufficient to distinguish different types of feeding drives, we designed two types of food choice environments that we might expect would distinguish homeostatic from hedonic feeding. Homeostatic feeding drive in flies is thought to be measured by the extent to which flies choose to consume a yeast-containing food over one consisting of sucrose only, which we will refer to hereafter as a yeast preference (Liu et al., 2017; Ro et al., 2016). Therefore, for measures of homeostatic drive, we exposed flies to a choice of 2% sucrose vs. [2% yeast + 1% sucrose], and we focused our analysis on the flies’ relative interactions with the yeast containing food. To measure hedonic drive, on the other hand, we incorporated theorized requirements to experimentally measure hedonic hunger in mammals: animals must be energy replete and they must be tested on highly palatable food (Lowe & Butryn, 2007). We therefore exposed flies to a “hedonic choice” environment which mimicked the “homeostatic choice” environment except that the 2% sucrose food was replaced with sweeter 20% sucrose, and we focused our analysis on comparing interactions with the sucrose food between the food environments (e.g., Fig. 3A bottom panel). This design ensured flies remained sated by access to yeast while also allowing feeding on the highly palatable 20% sucrose solution.
We observed that the 2% sucrose environment elicited an increased yeast preference in starved compared to fully-fed female flies (Fig. 3B), reinforcing the notion that the total number of events with yeast vs. sucrose can be used as a relative measure of homeostatic hunger. We also observed an increase in yeast preference in fully-fed female compared to male flies (Fig. 3C), which may be reflective of a known difference in protein requirements, and therefore homeostatic hunger levels, between male and female flies. We next evaluated female and male fully-fed flies in the 20% sucrose food environment compared to the 2% sucrose environment, which elicited increased event durations with 20% sucrose compared to the 2% sucrose food (Fig. 3D) and also elicited decreased event durations with yeast food (Fig. 3E). The total number of events on sucrose or yeast wells was statistically indistinguishable between food environments (Fig. 3F, 3G), suggesting that modulation of event duration, but not total number of events, may be a feature of hedonic food environments. We also measured sucrose intake in the control vs. hedonic environments using a method that determines how much volume a group of flies consume in 24 hours (termed “Con-Ex” for Consumption-Excretion) (Shell et al., 2021; Shell et al., 2018), and found that flies consume twice as much sucrose volume in the hedonic environment (Fig. 3 – Fig. Supp. 1A). We concluded that the measurable increase in sucrose event durations in the hedonic food environment may reflect increases in a hedonic hunger drive while total number of events on yeast vs. sucrose food may reflect homeostatic state.
To challenge our measure of hedonic feeding, we reasoned that if increased event durations were indeed a result of hedonic hunger, then sweetness alone should be sufficient to increase event durations without modulating measures of homeostatic feeding. We tested two additional food choice environments where we altered either the sweetness or caloric content of the sugar choice. Sweetness was manipulated by using the non-metabolizable sweetener arabinose ([2% sucrose + 18% arabinose], which mimics the caloric and nutritive content of the 2% sucrose but contains the sweetness of the 20% sucrose choice. Caloric content was manipulated using the nutritive, but not sweet, sugar alcohol, sorbitol ([2% sucrose + 18% sorbitol], which mimics the caloric content of the 20% sucrose but contains the sweetness of the 2% sucrose (Burke & Waddell, 2011; Hassett, 1948). We observed that the sweet arabinose food environment elicited increased event durations that were comparable to flies in the 20% sucrose environment, with no statistical difference in total event number (Figs. 3H, 3I, Fig. 3 – Fig. Supp. 1B) and that the caloric but not sweet sorbitol food environment was not sufficient to increase event durations or total event number (Figs. 3J, 3K). Finally, we tested several additional food choice environments containing a range of sucrose concentrations and observed that event durations were shortest when the sucrose choice contained only water or 1% sucrose and increased linearly as the sucrose concentration increased, while event durations with yeast food and total number of sucrose events were not statistically different (Fig. 3L, 3M, Fig. 3 – Fig. Supp. 1C). Thus, sweet taste was sufficient to evoke longer feeding event durations, while calories alone were not, indicating that this metric obtained from our hedonic food choice paradigm may reliably quantify hedonic feeding.
Whether and how homeostatic and hedonic feeding mechanisms interact or overlap is controversial, and thus, we asked whether our metrics of homeostatic and hedonic hunger are correlated among individual flies. To obtain flies with a range of homeostatic needs, we used fully-fed and starved flies to obtain feeding measures in our control or hedonic food choice environments. Starvation elicited an increase in yeast interactions compared to 2% sucrose (Fig. 4A) that was more pronounced in flies starved for 48 vs. 24 hours (Fig. 4B), which is consistent with our expectation for increased homeostatic drive. We found that starvation seemed to marginally increase differences in sucrose event durations in hedonic vs. control environments, indicating that homeostatic hunger may enhance hedonic feeding (Fig. 4C). For each fly, we next calculated metrics for homeostatic drive (# interactions with yeast food) as well as hedonic drive (sucrose event durations) and plotted their relationship along the x- and y-axes, respectively. We reasoned that if the circuits or programs that drive hedonic and homeostatic feeding were completely independent, then we should observe a slope close to zero. A significantly positive or negative slope, however, would suggest that increasing physiological need might also modulate hedonic hunger, or vice versa. We observed a significantly positive correlation between our measures of homeostatic and hedonic feeding (Fig. 4D), suggesting that increasing physiological need might also potentiate hedonic hunger, or vice versa, which is consistent with the observation in mammals that hunger increases the reward value of food (Fulton, 2010). Interestingly, we also noted that separating regression lines from flies in the control vs. hedonic food environments revealed parallel lines whose slopes were not significantly different but were separated by a shift along the y-axis, indicating again that a sweeter food environment promotes longer event durations but may not modulate the relationship between hunger drives. These findings indicate that homeostatic state is a potentiator of hedonic feeding and establish that inferences of hunger states and measures of feeding should include considerations of each component independently as well as their interaction.
Identification of neural correlates of hedonic hunger
A good deal is known about neural circuits associated with homeostatic hunger in Drosophila, and we wondered whether our behavioral and quantitative classification metrics could aid in identification of representative neural correlates of a hedonic hunger state. We began by conducting an unbiased screen for neurons that respond to hedonic food environments using the genetically-encoded calcium indicator CaMPARI driven by a pan-neuronal GAL4 driver (Nsyb-GAL4). CaMPARI is a green fluorescent protein that irreversibly photoconverts to red when flies are simultaneously exposed to increases in intracellular calcium and UV light provided by the experimenter (Edwards et al., 2020; Fosque et al., 2015). We placed groups of 3 naïve, fully-fed flies in the hedonic food choice environment or the control food environment and allowed them to habituate for 1 hour. Flies in each chamber were then exposed to cycles of UV light (10s on/10s off) for a total of 1 hour to label neuronal populations that were differentially active when flies are in the hedonic vs. the control food environment. Brains were dissected and imaged immediately, and the ratio of red:green fluorescent protein was analyzed, with a higher ratio indicating increased neuronal activity. We observed that the mushroom body had a higher ratio of red:green fluorescence when flies were in the hedonic food environment, indicating that this population was more active (Fig. 5A).
To validate the findings of our unbiased screen and as a direct test of mushroom body activity in hedonic food environments, we restricted the expression of CaMPARI to the mushroom body using the 238Y enhancer-trap line (Yang et al., 1995). We observed that the hedonic food environment increased red:green fluorescence in the horizontal mushroom body lobes compared to the control food environment (Fig. 5B). The Drosophila mushroom body is a well-described structure known to be involved in reinforcement and motivational control and is innervated by dopaminergic populations that are thought to encode reinforcing properties of food, including relaying information about sweet taste (Aso et al., 2009; Aso et al., 2014; Das et al., 2016). Thus, our observation that mushroom body neurons are activated in hedonic food environments is consistent with prior knowledge of their roles in motivation and reward (Landayan et al., 2018).
To determine whether mushroom body neurons are involved in hedonic hunger beyond correlative increases in activity, we tested for their requirement in hedonic and homeostatic feeding. We used the genetically-encoded optogenetic tool GtACR to selectively inhibit mushroom body neurons in our food choice assay. GtACR is a channelrhodopsin that is activated upon green light exposure and increases chloride conductance, resulting in hyperpolarization and silencing of neuronal populations (Mohammad et al., 2017). We expressed GtACR in either the γ, α/β, or α’/β’ lobes of the mushroom body (Aso et al., 2014) and found that communication from the γ and α/β lobes is dispensable for both hedonic and homeostatic feeding (Fig. 5C, 5D, Fig.5 – Fig. Supp. 1A, 1B). In contrast, signaling specifically from the α’/β’ lobes was required for hedonic, but not homeostatic, feeding (Fig. 5E, Fig. 5 – Fig. Supp. 1C). The mushroom body horizonal lobes are innervated by dopaminergic neurons from the protocerebral anterior medial (PAM) cluster, which were also required for hedonic feeding (Fig. 5F) (Mao & Davis, 2009). These results indicate that a distinct PAM to α’/β’ mushroom body circuit regulates hedonic feeding, which is consistent with known descriptions of anatomical and functional segregation of mushroom body lobes (Aso et al., 2014; Landayan et al., 2018).
We next sought to determine whether communication from the α’/β’ lobe was required for the potentiating effects of homeostatic hunger on hedonic feeding. We reasoned that if the α’/β’ lobe also functions as a potentiator between the two hunger states, then inhibition of these neurons would be expected to prevent increases in hedonic feeding when flies are starved. Instead, we observed that starvation restored hedonic feeding in flies whose α’/β’ lobes were inhibited (Fig. 5G), suggesting that potentiation of hedonic feeding by homeostatic state involves the recruitment of additional and independent circuits for hedonic feeding.
Discussion
Animals exquisitely regulate feeding according to internal needs, but they are also subject to their desires. Thus, food intake only sometimes occurs in response to homeostatic hunger and can also occur in response to emotional stress, social environment, illness, and pursuit of reward (Siemian et al., 2021; Xu et al., 2019). Because animals eat for many reasons other than hunger, food intake alone may be a poor indicator of hunger states and may obscure investigations into the motivational forces that drive behavior. Here, we observed the feeding micro-behaviors of flies and developed a behavioral classification system to assist in the identification of feeding drives. We established that flies exhibit behavioral signatures of two hunger states, homeostatic and hedonic, and we used them to identify neurocircuits that are required for hedonic, but not homeostatic, feeding. We also showed that homeostatic and hedonic feeding are both independent and overlapping depending on environmental context, and we developed a simple interaction framework that researchers can use to investigate these relationships in detail. The development of this paradigm presents entry-points into the discovery of new homeostatic and hedonic hunger circuits in flies, better classification of feeding circuits which have already been described, and dissection of mechanistic relationships among them.
The Drosophila MB supports a wide variety of behaviors and functions, in part due to its complexity and heterogeneity in anatomical structure (Aso et al., 2009). The lobes of the MB are formed by the interconnected axons of ∼2000 Kenyon cells and 34 MB output neurons, and they receive modulatory input from ∼20 dopaminergic cell types (Aso et al., 2014). The MB lobes have been anatomically and functionally divided into three classes: γ, α/β, and α’/β’, which have been further divided into functionally distinct compartments (Aso et al., 2014; Crittenden et al., 1998; Tanaka et al., 2008). Our data suggest that compartments of the α’/β’ MB lobes, but not the γ or α/β lobes, are required for hedonic feeding. Although our experiments isolated a discrete location within the functionally heterogenous mushroom body, the α’/β’ lobe still contains more than 350 Kenyon cells that are divided into middle (m) and anterior-posterior (ap) compartments (Aso et al., 2014). Future investigations that utilize existing and emerging intersectional technologies will be useful for discerning the specific cells within the α’/β’ MB lobes that promote hedonic feeding, as well as their modulatory inputs and outputs, and for evaluation of the cell-intrinsic mechanisms that determine how hedonic hunger is encoded.
Connections from dopaminergic populations in the PAM cluster with MB-intrinsic Kenyon cells have been implicated in appetitive learning, signaling reward in response to sugar, motivation to overcome environmental stress, and promoting wakefulness (Burke et al., 2012; Haynes et al., 2015; Hermanns et al., 2022; Liu et al., 2012). PAM projections to Kenyon cell axons in the β’ compartment of the MB respond to sugar by increasing calcium transients, which has led to the idea that these synapses transmit information about sweet taste (Liu et al., 2012; May et al., 2020). Interestingly, these responses are increased 2-fold by starvation, suggesting that they scale in response to environmental context or degree of need (Liu et al., 2012), and artificial activation of PAM neurons also increases foraging behavior (Tsao et al., 2018). Our finding that the PAM>α’/β’ circuit is required for promoting hedonic hunger builds upon a rich body of literature, and together these observations offer a model that may unite the many described functions of this node. One possibility is that this circuit acts as a permissive or motive force that gates or fuels downstream mechanisms involved in behavioral responses to environmental stimuli. Indeed, learning, reward, and wakefulness are all processes that might benefit from or require a force that sustains them. This view offers an attractive entry into understanding the seat of motivation in the fly brain.
Decades of research on the neural control of feeding have resulted in the identification of mammalian neurocircuits involved in the control of feeding, and technological advances have allowed researchers to manipulate them with an increasing degree of specificity. We are, however, approaching the limits of what we can learn from manipulations and technologies of these sorts and thus, complimenting them with discoveries from model systems in which more specific manipulations can be tested may accelerate our ability to understand how the brain generates motivated states. The hedonic circuits we have identified here in flies, for example, can be immediately investigated at multiple levels to assess the neuronal and molecular mechanisms that determine their role in feeding, including spatiotemporal manipulation of cell-signaling pathways, electrophysiological recordings in live animals in response to various food environments or energetic deficits, and optogenetic manipulation paired with close behavioral monitoring. These types of experiments would fulfill several main goals in the study of the neural control of appetite regulation (Ahn et al., 2022) and, despite differences in the precise identity of feeding neurocircuits between flies and mammals, discoveries about foundational principles and conserved signaling pathways may inform and direct mammalian studies.
Materials and Methods
Key Resources Table
Fly stocks and husbandry
Fly stocks were maintained on a standard cornmeal-based larval growth medium and in a controlled environment (21C, 60% humidity) with a 12:12hr light:dark cycle. We controlled the developmental larval density by manually aliquoting 32 ul of collected eggs into individual bottles containing 25-50mL of food at 25C. For FLIC experiments, following eclosion mixed sex flies were kept on cornmeal medium at 18C until they were 7-14 days old. One day prior to the experiments, flies were sorted by sex and transferred onto either SY10 medium (10% [w/v] sucrose and 10% [w/v] yeast) or wet starved (1 Kimwipe with 2mL of H20) at 25C. Experiments that used flies starved for 48 hours were sorted to starvation vials and kept at 25C two days prior to the experiments. We used mated female and male flies that were between 7-14 days old for all video characterization experiments and mated female flies for all subsequent experiments, except where noted. The following stocks were used for experiments: Canton-S, UAS-CaMPARI (BDSC #68762), MB238Y-GAL4 (BDSC #81009), MB131B-GAL4 (BDSC #68265), MB008B-GAL4 (BDSC #68291), MB461B-GAL4 (BDSC #68327), GMR58E02-GAL4 (BDSC #41347), and LexAop2-CsChrimson;UAS-CaMPARI2;GMR57C10-GAL4 (BDSC #81089) were obtained from the Bloomington Drosophila Stock Center. UAS-GtACR was provided by M. Dus (University of Michigan, MI).
Video Recording and Scoring
The video recording setup for initial behavioral characterizations (Fig. 1B, 1C, Supp. Table 1) consisted of the Fly Liquid-Food Counter (FLIC), a custom single-well enclosure, and a light to illuminate the recording area. A FLIR Grasshopper camera (GS3-U3-28S5C-C) fitted with a Fujinon varifocal lens (MFR #DV3.4×3.8SA-1) was positioned against the lid to record the FLIC well and the surrounding enclosed area. Video recording and FLIC data collection were started simultaneously, and video length was dependent on the maximum number of frames that could be stored. This setup recorded continuous video of the fly and saved this footage unaltered. For experiments in Figures 1D-1F, we developed a direct communication link between our custom DTrack video software and the FLIC system to trigger video capture only when there was physical contact between a fly and the food source, together with 5s windows before and after the contact (see Fig. 1A).
Videos of sated or 24-hour wet-starved male and female Canton-S flies were recorded. A single fly was aspirated into the FLIC, which contained 5% or 10% sucrose food solution in 4 mg/l MgCl2. Videos were recorded at various times of day between 10AM and 7PM and had an average length of about 51 minutes, except for videos collected for circadian analysis, which were recorded over a 24-hour period. “Meal-times” were defined as 7AM-10AM and 7PM-10PM. All videos were recorded on the lab bench. Manual, frame-by-frame annotation was used to classify user-defined behaviors. Experimenters were blinded to the treatment for video scoring. Data was analyzed using the open-source JWatcher software and R studio.
Fly Liquid-Food Interaction Counter (FLIC) assays
Flies were tested on the Fly Liquid-Food Interaction Counter (FLIC) system as previously described (Ro et al., 2014) (Sable Systems International). FLIC Drosophila Feeding Monitors (DFMs, Sable Systems International, model DFMV3) were used in the two-choice configuration and each chamber was loaded with food solutions containing a sugar choice (1%-20% sucrose, [18% arabinose + 2% sucrose], or [18% sorbitol + 2% sucrose]) and a yeast choice ([2% bacto yeast extract + 1% sucrose]) in 4 mg/l MgCl2. Flies were briefly anesthetized on ice and aspirated into the DFM chambers, ensuring that each DFM received flies from all treatments. We began recording immediately after loading flies (generally, loading all DFMs requires <10 minutes) and measured FLIC interactions for 6 hours during the “non-meal” time of day. Each FLIC experiment contains pooled data from two independent replicates. FLIC data were analyzed using custom R code, which is available at https://github.com/PletcherLab/FLIC_R_Code. Default thresholds were used for analysis except for the following: minimum feeding threshold = 10, tasting threshold = (0,10). Animals that did not participate (i.e. returned zero values), whose DFM returned an unstable baseline signal, or who produced extreme outliers (criteria for outliers were predetermined as exceeding twice the mean of the population) were excluded from analysis.
Consumption-excretion feeding assays
Con-Ex experiments were carried out as previously described (Shell et al., 2021; Shell et al., 2018). Experimental female flies were sorted to appropriate control (choice between SY10 food or 2% sucrose) or hedonic (choice between SY10 or 20% sucrose) food environments (10 flies per vial) when they were 4 days old and maintained on this food for 3 weeks. Food was changed every 2-3 days. After the dietary pre-treatment period, blue test food was prepared in removable choice caps by adding 1% (w/v) FD&C Blue No. 1 to only the side of the cap containing the sucrose food choice. Flies were moved to fresh vials with the removable blue-food caps on the top of the vials and were allowed to feed and excrete for 24 hours. Caps and flies were removed after the 24-hour test period and flies were counted. Excreted dye was collected by vortexing each vial with 3mL of water. Concentration of the dye was determined by absorbance at 630nm and compared to a standard curve of known concentrations.
Optogenetic FLIC Assays
Flies expressing UAS-GtACR in desired neuronal populations were reared as described above. When 7-14 days old, flies were sorted to cornmeal medium containing 200 µm all-trans-retinal (from a stock solution of 100 mM ATR in 100% ethanol) and kept in the dark for 1-4 days at 18C. Flies were shifted to 25C for 24 hours before the experiment and flipped to either SY10 media containing 800 µm all-trans-retinal or starvation vials. Custom FLIC lids were fitted with 530 nm LEDs (Luxeon). Custom hardware and firmware were designed to allow the experimenter to control LED intensity and a range of light stimulus parameters. We used a stimulus frequency of 40 Hz and a pulse width of 800 ms for the entire duration of the 6 hour FLIC experiment.
CaMPARI
CaMPARI photoconversion was carried out according to previously established protocols with some modifications (Edwards et al., 2020). A 405 nM LED (Thorlabs M405L3) was connected to a 1.2A LED driver (ThorLabs LEDD1B). Custom hardware and firmware were designed to allow the experimenter to control LED intensity and a range of light stimulus parameters. Female flies expressing UAS-CaMPARI were reared as described above. Custom choice caps were filled with fresh SY10% on one side and either 2% or 20% sucrose in 2% bacto agar on the other side. Groups of 5 flies were briefly anesthetized on ice and aspirated into the appropriate food environments. A custom clear cap was placed on top of the mocap to enclose the flies. The 405 nM LED was placed on top (∼1/4 inch above the food surface) and the flies were allowed to habituate to the environment for 1 hour. After habituation, flies received UV light stimulation for 1 hour (cycling 10s on and 10s off, 1.0A). Immediately following light stimulation, flies were protected from light and brains were dissected as previously described (Wu & Luo, 2006). Brains were mounted between a glass microscope slide and a #1.5 cover glass separated by a custom bridge in VECTASHIELD Antifade Mounting Medium (Vector Laboratories). Samples were imaged on a Nikon A1 Confocal Microscope with a 20X air objective using 488 nm and 561 nm laser lines. Z-stacks were collected at a 1-2 µm step. All treatments were mounted under the same cover slip. Image processing was performed using ImageJ (NIH), and the experimenter was blinded to treatment. Slices containing the ROI were identified and collapsed into an average intensity projection. ROIs were measured in the red and green channel after background subtraction, and a ratio of red:green fluorescence is reported.
Statistics
For comparisons involving only one level, we used Student’s t-test to detect significant differences between two treatments or one-way ANOVA followed by Tukey’s post-hoc test after verifying normality and equality of variances. T-tests were two-tailed during initial characterization experiments, or one-tailed in future experiments where the predicted direction of change was known. For comparisons involving more than one level, we used two-way ANOVA to detect significant interactions between the levels and followed up with Tukey’s post-hoc when significance was detected (p<0.05). In cases where experimental replicates were pooled, a two-way ANOVA with blocking for experiment was performed to ensure non-significant experimental effects. For all dot and bar plots, error bars represent the SEM. All statistical tests and graphing were performed using R. Specific details of statistical analyses are presented in the figure legends.
Acknowledgements
We wish to thank past and present members of the Pletcher laboratory for their support and comments about the experimental design of these studies, and express gratitude to David Paris for his help engineering and creating some tools used in this study. We acknowledge Binyamin Jacobovitz in the Michigan Medicine Microscopy Core for training and advice on confocal imaging. Figure 1A was created with BioRender.com.
Funding
This research was supported by The National Science Foundation Graduate Research Fellowship Program (No. DGE 1256260) and the Howard Hughes Medical Institute through the James H. Gilliam Fellowships for Advanced Study program to K.J.W (#GT11426) and the US National Institute of Health, National Institute on Aging (R01 AG051649, R01 AG030593, and R01 AG063371) and the Glenn Medical Foundation to S.D.P.
Competing interests
The senior author (S.D.P) is a share holder in the company, Flidea, which has developed technology related to the FLIC feeding system.
Data and materials availability
All data generated during this study are provided with the manuscript.
FLIC data were analyzed using custom R code, which is available at https://github.com/PletcherLab/FLIC_R_Code.
Supplementary Files
Figure 5 – video 1 NsybCaMPARI stacks control environment green
Figure 5 – video 2 NsybCaMPARI stacks control environment red
Figure 5 – video 3 NsybCaMPARI stacks control environment merge
Figure 5 – video 4 NsybCaMPARI stacks hedonic environment green
Figure 5 – video 5 NsybCaMPARI stacks hedonic environment red
Figure 5 – video 6 NsybCaMPARI stacks hedonic environment merge
Figure 5 – video 7 MBCaMPARI stacks control environment green
Figure 5 – video 8 MBCaMPARI stacks control environment red Figure 5 – video 9 MBCaMPARI stacks control environment merge
Figure 5 – video 10 MBCaMPARI stacks hedonic environment green
Figure 5 – video 11 MBCaMPARI stacks hedonic environment red
Figure 5 – video 12 MBCaMPARI stacks hedonic environment merge
Figure 5 – Videos 1-12. Movies of confocal stacks, related to Figure 5A-B. Movies of confocal z-stacks through whole brains of flies carrying Nsyb-Gal4>UAS-CaMPARI (videos 1-6) or MB238Y-GAL4>UAS-CaMPARI (videos 7-12) in control or hedonic food environments. Green channels represent unconverted CaMPARI and magenta channels represent photoconverted CaMPARI.
References
- Brain circuits for promoting homeostatic and non-homeostatic appetitesExp Mol Med 54:349–357https://doi.org/10.1038/s12276-022-00758-4
- Acts of appetite: neural circuits governing the appetitive, consummatory, and terminating phases of feedingNat Metab 4:836–847https://doi.org/10.1038/s42255-022-00611-y
- Long-term high fat diet consumption reversibly alters feeding behavior via a dopamine-associated mechanism in miceBehav Brain Res 414https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2021.113470
- Toward a Wiring Diagram Understanding of Appetite ControlNeuron 95:757–778https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2017.06.014
- Chronic high-fat diet affects food-motivated behavior and hedonic systems in the nucleus accumbens of male ratsAppetite 153https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2020.104739
- The Nicomachean ethics (Further rev. edPenguin Books. Publisher description
- The mushroom body of adult Drosophila characterized by GAL4 driversJ Neurogenet 23:156–172https://doi.org/10.1080/01677060802471718
- The neuronal architecture of the mushroom body provides a logic for associative learningElife 3https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.04577
- Food reward: brain substrates of wanting and likingNeurosci Biobehav Rev 20:1–25https://doi.org/10.1016/0149-7634(95)00033-b
- ‘Liking’ and ‘wanting’ food rewards: brain substrates and roles in eating disordersPhysiol Behav 97:537–550https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2009.02.044
- Neurons for hunger and thirst transmit a negative-valence teaching signalNature 521:180–185https://doi.org/10.1038/nature14416
- Quantifying behavior the JWatcher waySinauer Associates. Table of contents only
- Layered reward signalling through octopamine and dopamine in DrosophilaNature 492:433–437https://doi.org/10.1038/nature11614
- Remembering nutrient quality of sugar in DrosophilaCurr Biol 21:746–750https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2011.03.032
- An explanation of hunger. 1911Obes Res 1:494–500https://doi.org/10.1002/j.1550-8528.1993.tb00033.x
- Combining Quantitative and Qualitative Data in the Study of Feeding Behavior in Male Wistar RatsFront Psychol 10https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00881
- T-pattern analysis for the study of temporal structure of animal and human behavior: a comprehensive reviewJ Neurosci Methods 239:34–46https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneumeth.2014.09.024
- Fighting fruit flies: a model system for the study of aggressionProc Natl Acad Sci U S A 99:5664–5668https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.082102599
- Tripartite mushroom body architecture revealed by antigenic markersLearn Mem 5:38–51
- Remembering Components of Food in DrosophilaFront Integr Neurosci 10https://doi.org/10.3389/fnint.2016.00004
- Palatability Can Drive Feeding Independent of AgRP NeuronsCell Metab 22:646–657https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cmet.2015.07.011
- Quantifying Drosophila food intake: comparative analysis of current methodologyNat Methods 11:535–540https://doi.org/10.1038/nmeth.2899
- A Brain Module for Scalable Control of Complex, Multi-motor Threat DisplaysNeuron 100:1474–1490https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2018.10.027
- The Photoconvertible Fluorescent Probe, CaMPARI, Labels Active Neurons in Freely-Moving Intact Adult Fruit FliesFront Neural Circuits 14https://doi.org/10.3389/fncir.2020.00022
- The emergence and influence of internal statesNeuron 110:2545–2570https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2022.04.030
- Neural circuits. Labeling of active neural circuits in vivo with designed calcium integratorsScience 347:755–760https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1260922
- Appetite and rewardFront Neuroendocrinol 31:85–103https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yfrne.2009.10.003
- Palatable food induces an appetitive behaviour in satiated rats which can be inhibited by chronic stressBehav Pharmacol 8:619–628https://doi.org/10.1097/00008877-199711000-00018
- Testing protein leverage in lean humans: a randomised controlled experimental studyPLoS One 6https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0025929
- The utilization of sugars and other substances by DrosophilaBiol Bull 95:114–123
- A single pair of neurons links sleep to memory consolidation in Drosophila melanogasterElife 4https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.03868
- Octopamine mediates sugar relief from a chronic-stress-induced depression-like state in Drosophilahttps://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2022.07.016
- Food wanting is mediated by transient activation of dopaminergic signaling in the honey bee brainScience 376:508–512https://doi.org/10.1126/science.abn9920
- Sweet taste and nutrient value subdivide rewarding dopaminergic neurons in DrosophilaCurr Biol 25:751–758https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2015.01.036
- Automated monitoring and quantitative analysis of feeding behaviour in DrosophilaNat Commun 5https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms5560
- Rapid, reversible activation of AgRP neurons drives feeding behavior in miceJ Clin Invest 121:1424–1428https://doi.org/10.1172/JCI46229
- Satiation state-dependent dopaminergic control of foraging in DrosophilaSci Rep 8https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-24217-1
- A subset of dopamine neurons signals reward for odour memory in DrosophilaNature 488:512–516https://doi.org/10.1038/nature11304
- Branch-specific plasticity of a bifunctional dopamine circuit encodes protein hungerScience 356:534–539https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aal3245
- Hedonic hunger: a new dimension of appetite?Physiol Behav 91:432–439https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.04.006
- Eight different types of dopaminergic neurons innervate the Drosophila mushroom body neuropil: anatomical and physiological heterogeneityFront Neural Circuits 3https://doi.org/10.3389/neuro.04.005.2009
- Dietary sugar inhibits satiation by decreasing the central processing of sweet tasteElife 9https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.54530
- High Dietary Sugar Reshapes Sweet Taste to Promote Feeding Behavior in Drosophila melanogasterCell Rep 27:1675–1685https://doi.org/10.1016/j.celrep.2019.04.027
- Optogenetic inhibition of behavior with anion channelrhodopsinsNat Methods 14:271–274https://doi.org/10.1038/nmeth.4148
- Behavioral signatures of structured feature detection during courtship in DrosophilaCurr Biol 32:1211–1231https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2022.01.024
- Hedonic hot spots in the brainNeuroscientist 12:500–511https://doi.org/10.1177/1073858406293154
- FLIC: high-throughput, continuous analysis of feeding behaviors in DrosophilaPLoS One 9https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0101107
- Serotonin signaling mediates protein valuation and agingElife 5https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.16843
- Overlapping Brain Circuits for Homeostatic and Hedonic FeedingCell Metab 27:42–56https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cmet.2017.09.021
- The need to feed: homeostatic and hedonic control of eatingNeuron 36:199–211https://doi.org/10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00969-8
- Expansion and application of dye tracers for measuring solid food intake and food preference in DrosophilaSci Rep 11https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-99483-7
- Measurement of solid food intake in Drosophila via consumption-excretion of a dye tracerSci Rep 8https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-29813-9
- Hypothalamic control of interoceptive hungerCurr Biol 31:3797–3809https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2021.06.048
- Geometric analysis of macronutrient intake in humans: the power of protein?Appetite 41:123–140https://doi.org/10.1016/s0195-6663(03)00049-7
- Behavior of Hector’s Dolphin: Classifying Behavior by Sequence AnalysisJournal of Mammalogy 75:956–964https://doi.org/10.2307/1382477
- Three Pillars for the Neural Control of AppetiteAnnu Rev Physiol 79:401–423https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-physiol-021115-104948
- Neuronal assemblies of the Drosophila mushroom bodyJ Comp Neurol 508:711–755https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.21692
- Drosophila mushroom bodies integrate hunger and satiety signals to control innate food-seeking behaviorElife 7https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.35264
- Encoding of hunger by the neuronal epigenome slows aging in Drosophila [preprint]https://doi.org/10.1101/2022.07.21.501022
- Co-localization of growth hormone secretagogue receptor and NPY mRNA in the arcuate nucleus of the ratNeuroendocrinology 70:306–316https://doi.org/10.1159/000054491
- A protocol for dissecting Drosophila melanogaster brains for live imaging or immunostainingNat Protoc 1:2110–2115https://doi.org/10.1038/nprot.2006.336
- Identification of a neurocircuit underlying regulation of feeding by stress-related emotional responsesNat Commun 10https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-019-11399-z
- Subdivision of the Drosophila mushroom bodies by enhancer-trap expression patternsNeuron 15:45–54https://doi.org/10.1016/0896-6273(95)90063-2
- Expression of ghrelin receptor mRNA in the rat and the mouse brainJ Comp Neurol 494:528–548https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.20823
Article and author information
Author information
Version history
- Preprint posted:
- Sent for peer review:
- Reviewed Preprint version 1:
- Reviewed Preprint version 2:
- Version of Record published:
Copyright
© 2023, Weaver et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
- views
- 3,325
- downloads
- 246
- citations
- 5
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.