Abstract
When mice run, activity in their primary visual cortex (V1) is strongly modulated. This observation has altered conceptions of a brain region assumed to be a passive image processor. Extensive work has followed to dissect the circuits and functions of running-correlated modulation. However, it remains unclear whether visual processing in primates might similarly change during locomotion. We therefore measured V1 activity in marmosets while they viewed stimuli on a treadmill. In contrast to mouse, running-correlated modulations of marmoset V1 were small, and tended to be slightly suppressive. Population-level analyses revealed trial-to-trial fluctuations of shared gain across V1 in both species, but while strongly correlated with running in mice, gain modulations were smaller and more often negatively correlated with running in marmosets. Thus, population-wide fluctuations of V1 may reflect a common feature of mammalian visual cortical function, but important quantitative differences point to distinct consequences for the relation between vision and action in primates versus rodents.
Introduction
Sensation and action are traditionally thought to involve separate brain circuits serving distinct functions: Activity in early sensory areas is driven nearly exclusively by the corresponding sensory input, whereas activity in motor areas is exclusively related to movement. Recent work in mice, a major mammalian model system in neuroscience, has called for a re-evaluation of this distinction, given recent demonstrations that activity in mouse primary visual cortex (V1) depends as much on whether the mouse is running or stationary as on what visual stimulus is shown[1]. Neurons in V1 of virtually all mammals are selective for simple image features, a presumably critical early step of image processing that continues throughout a hierarchy of visual brain areas [2, 3], and this is true of mice as well [4]. The observation that running modulates (mouse) V1 of a comparable magnitude to the visually-driven activity has motivated substantial effort in the field to understand the biological mechanisms and functional consequences of this powerful interaction between sensation and action [5–17].
However, these observations have all been made in rodents; similar measurements have not been made in primates. Although rodents certainly rely on vision for important behaviors [18, 19], primates are more fundamentally visual organisms, with exquisite acuity and specialized functional characteristics such as foveas and corresponding high-resolution representations of the central visual field in V1[20], in addition to a larger network of areas involved in vision [3]. And while experiments that allow subjects to run while viewing visual stimuli may now be commonplace in mice, analogous experiments in nonhuman primates have remained technically daunting. It has thus remained unclear whether the large effect of running on early visual processing is a general property of mammalian brains revealed by work in mice, or whether the early stages of primate visual processing are less affected by nonvisual factors. Here, we fill this major gap in cross-species understanding by taking advantage of the relatively small size and peaceable nature of the common marmoset (Callithrix jacchus), which allowed us to have animals on a custom-designed treadmill and to use high-channel-count electrode arrays, including Neuropixels. Our comparative study fits into a larger emerging enterprise to assess whether substantial signals due to animal movements affect sensory processing similarly in rodents and primates [21–23].
Results
We tested for running-based modulations in V1 of the common marmoset, a highly visual new world primate. Marmosets were head-fixed, placed on a wheel-based treadmill suited to their arboreal nature (Fig. 1a), and alternated between running and not running while we presented various visual stimuli designed to assess the properties and responsiveness of V1 neurons (Fig. 1b). We recorded from foveal and parafoveal neurons in 2 marmosets (using chronically-implanted N-Form 3D electrode arrays), and in one marmoset were also able to simultaneously record from both foveal and peripheral V1 (using Neuropixels 1.0 probes). To support precise comparison to rodent V1, we used the same analysis pipeline on a publicly-available mouse dataset that used matching stimuli in a treadmill paradigm [24]. This let us perform direct quantitative and statistical comparisons of the effects of running on V1 activity in a rodent and a primate.
First, we mapped the receptive fields of marmoset V1 neurons using reverse-correlation techniques adapted to free-viewing [25] while we measured gaze using a video-based eyetracker (Fig. 1c). In V1 of both marmosets, we found receptive fields within the central few degrees of vision, with sizes expected at those eccentricities (1-5 deg, Fig. 1f, blue and green; these can be compared to those in mouse, Fig. 1e). As expected for primary visual cortex, marmoset V1 (both well-isolated single units and well-tuned multi-unit clusters) responded robustly to oriented gratings and exhibited orientation- (and sometimes direction-) selectivity [26, 27], similar to that in the mouse V1 dataset (Fig. 1g,h). Orientation tuning spanned a range from weak to strong tuning, with many units exhibiting strong and conventional tuning curves (Fig. 1i,j).
As a first test for effects of running on V1 activity, we assessed whether running speed was correlated with aggregate V1 activity by comparing the time series of these variables throughout each session. In the mouse, such modulations are easily visually evident when inspecting the time series of neural activity and running: when the mouse runs, V1 spiking often increases substantially. Fig. 2a,b shows example sessions with the maximal and median amounts of correlation between the time series of running speed and a generic low-dimensional representation of the population activity (the first principal component [PC] of the simultaneously-recorded V1 trial spike counts). This correlation could be seen when running / not running alternated on slow (Fig. 2a) or fast (Fig. 2b) time scales.
A starkly different impression comes from visual inspection of the relationship between running and the activity of marmoset V1 neurons representing the central visual field. Any relation between V1 activity and running appears considerably smaller. In examples showing the maximal and median relationships between running and V1 activity (Fig. 2c,d), V1 activity did not track running speed as clearly, although the activity did tend to increase when the monkey stopped running, explaining the modest negative correlations.
We then quantified the relationship between the timecourses of aggregate V1 activity and running across all experiments on a session-by-session basis, in both species. For mice, this confirmed a strong positive correlation (Fig. 2e; median=0.407, n=25, p=9.04 × 10-5, stat=308, Mann-Whitney U Test). For marmosets, the distribution of correlations between V1 activity and running was subtly but reliably negative (Fig. 2f, median=-0.033, p=0.034, stat=101, n=27, Mann-Whitney U Test). Most importantly, the correlation between V1 activity and running was significantly different between the two species (p=6.93 × 10-7, stat=934, Mann-Whitney U Test). This session-level analysis confirmed that running modulations in mice are large and mostly reflect increases in response. In contrast, running modulations in marmoset foveal V1 are small, and if anything, reflect slight reductions in activity.
To perform additional quantitative tests at the level of individual V1 units, we divvied up each unit’s spiking responses to drifting gratings based on whether or not the animal was running (Fig. 3). This analysis confirmed, in mouse, a tendency for large response increases during running to both the preferred orientation stimulus (Fig. 3a, geometric mean ratio [running/stationary] = 1.523, 95% CI [1.469, 1.579], n=743 tuned units) and to all visual stimuli (Fig 3b, 1.402 [1.365, 1.440], n=1168). Many individual units had significant running modulations, and were more often increases rather than decreases (803/1168 [69%] increased firing rate, and 115/1168 [10%] decreased, bootstrapped t-test). In marmoset V1, there was again a modest decrease evident in the response to the preferred stimulus (Fig. 3c; geometric mean ratio [running/stationary] = 0.899, 95% CI [0.851, 0.949], n=228 tuned units). Not even modest suppression was evident in responses aggregated across all stimuli (Fig. 3d, 1.011 [0.995, 1.027], n=786). The number of significantly modulated units was relatively small, and was more balanced between decreases and increases in firing rate (172/786 [22%] increased and 161/786 [20%] decreased, bootstrapped t-test). Because we performed quantitative comparisons on subsets of the data for which the stimuli were nearly identical across species, and used the same data analysis code to calculate response metrics, these analyses solidly confirm a substantial difference between the form of running modulations of V1 activity in mouse versus marmoset (log ratio of running:stationary was significantly different between mouse and marmoset for all units: p=6.62 × 10-99, stat=1399874, Mann-Whitney U Test, and tuned units: p=4.69 × 10-57, stat=4030135). Thus, the overall impacts of running on V1 units again appear large and positive in mice, and much smaller (and perhaps slightly negative) in marmoset.
Given these apparently categorical differences between the two species at the levels of both experimental sessions (Fig. 2) and individual units (Fig. 3), a key question is whether mouse and marmoset visual cortices are modulated by non-visual input in fundamentally different ways. To answer this, we employed more powerful model-based neuronal population analyses that inferred trial-to-trial variations in shared gain modulations across V1 (Fig. 4a,d) [29], in a manner totally agnostic to running (or any other aspect of behavior). This shared-gain model improved descriptions of the population data over simpler models that only took the stimulus (and slow drifts in baseline firing rate) into account for all sessions (Fig. 4b,c; marmoset p=1.52 × 10-82, stat=27174, n=754, Wilcoxon signed rank test; mouse p=4.64 × 10-181, stat=25966, n=1257). This was true in both species, bolstering the emerging notion that population-level gain modulations are a general principle of mammalian V1 function [29–33]. This shared gain term modulated more strongly in mice compared to marmosets (Fig. 4e, std. dev. in mouse = 2.170 [2.106, 2.245], marmoset = 1.188 [1.072, 1.274], p<1 × 10-9, stat=1013202, Mann-Whitney U Test). Furthermore, in the mouse, shared gain was higher for running than stationary as estimated during stimulus presentations (mean difference 0.970 [0.761, 1.225], p∼0, stat 8.017, t test), demonstrating that a substantial portion of modulations of mouse V1 can be explained by a shared gain term that increases with running (Fig. 4f, orange point). In marmoset, shared gain was slightly but reliably lower when running (mean difference = -0.125 [-0.203, -0.059], p=0.002, stat=-3.360, t test, (Fig. 4f, blue point), a quantitatively very different relation to running than in mouse (p = 8.77 × 10-9, stat=6.615, 2 sample t test). Thus, a common mechanism (shared gain) can describe running modulations in both species– but with quantitatively different correlations with behavior that make for potentially distinct downstream impacts on perception and action.
Although our marmoset dataset focused on V1 neurons representing the central portion of the visual field, we were also able to record simultaneously from neurons with peripheral and central (foveal) receptive fields by advancing a Neuropixels probe into both the superficial portion of V1 (foveal/central) and the calcarine sulcus (peripheral), resulting in simultaneous recordings of 110 and 147 (stimulus-driven) units representing the central and peripheral portions of the visual field, respectively. Analyzing neurons with peripheral receptive fields separately revealed a difference in running modulations between these retinotopically-distinct portions of V1: peripheral neurons had slightly higher stimulus-driven responses during running (aggregating over all stimuli, geometric mean ratio [running/stationary] = 1.129 [1.068, 1.194], n=147; difference with the central units was significant, p=2.100e-03, stat=12376, Mann-Whitney U Test), and the two sessions in which we were able to perform these measurements had higher positive correlations than any sessions in our entire foveal V1 dataset (assessed by correlating running speed either with the First Neural PC or with a shared gain term). Although the foveal representation in V1 (accessible in marmosets on the dorsal surface of the brain) is slightly suppressed by running, it appears that quantitative differences exist in the peripheral representation (which we recorded from in the calcarine sulcus). This initial set of recordings suggests that subtle increases in response might occur in the peripheral representation in marmoset V1. This finding calls for a larger-scale study of how such modulations might differ across portions of the retinotopic map, and for further consideration of the implications for cross-species comparisons. An intriguing conjecture is that the primate foveal representation might be functionally unique, but that the primate peripheral representation might be more functionally similar to that of mouse V1 [34].
Although this is an interesting potential distinction that further work will investigate more systematically, we emphasize that the main result described earlier still holds: the effects of running are small in marmoset V1. Even though there are slightly positive modulations in the peripheral representation (and hence, are of the same sign as those in mouse), the magnitude of whatever running-correlated modulations we could measure in marmoset V1 are still small relative to those in mouse V1 (median spike rate modulation by running significantly different between mouse and marmoset calcarine/peripheral recordings: p=7.639 × 10-11, stat=7967825, Mann-Whitney U Test).
Finally, we assessed whether the modest running-correlated modulations we observed in marmoset V1 might be explained by eye movements. If eye movements differed when the animal ran versus when it did not run, that would mean that the retinal input differed between the two conditions [34]. In that case, running modulations would not reflect a direct effect of running (a fundamentally non-visual effect), but rather a consequence of changes in the patterns of retinal stimulation (which we already know affects V1 responses). To test this possibility, we quantified the number of saccades per stimulus presentation, as well as saccade size (vector magnitude), and then assessed whether these eye movement metrics differed as a function of running.
We found that eye movements were quite similar between running and stationary periods, although subtle quantitative differences were revealed (Fig. 5). In short, saccades were slightly more frequent and larger during running (saccade frequency during running: 2.653 Hz, 95% CI [2.600, 2.697]; stationary/not running: 2.525 Hz [2.475, 2.573]; saccade magnitude during running: 9.261 deg [9.140, 9.374]; stationary/not running: 8.337 deg [8.190, 8.470]). This result motivated us to then assess whether these running-correlated eye movement differences might quantitatively explain the running-correlated modulations of V1 response. Our initial analyses found that differences in retinal stimulation due to differences in eye movements are unlikely to explain running-correlated suppression of V1 activity. We used linear regression to estimate the relationship between number of saccades and the firing rate of each unit in each trial. This enabled us to predict how much change in firing rate we should expect given the differences in saccade rate between the running and stationary conditions. The response change predicted from saccades was much less than the already-small running-correlated changes in response we observed in our experiment. On the aggregate, saccades slightly increased activity (predicted spike rate increase during running based on saccades = 0.05 Hz; expressed as gain, < 1%), and thus cannot explain the sign or magnitude of the subtle decreases we observed. In short, the decreases in V1 activity we saw in our main dataset are not likely to be explained by differential patterns of eye movements. (Likewise, the distinction we saw between running-correlated modulations in foveal versus peripheral V1 is unlikely attributable to eye movements, as we recorded simultaneously from both parts of the retinotopic map, meaning that the eye movements were the same despite the difference in modulations). Regrettably, the mouse dataset with which we compared our marmoset recordings did not reliably have eye video with quality required to do precise gaze estimation (at least for many of the sessions), so we could not perform a definitive analysis in mouse. However, the degree to which movement-correlated modulations of sensory processing contain a retinal contribution is an important issue [34], and one that we hope to tackle more directly in future cross-species studies wherein eyetracking and knowledge of individual receptive fields is highly, and equally, prioritized in mice and marmosets.
We also analyzed the pupil size from our eyetracking videos. Pupil size was ~8% larger during running. This finding is consistent with the idea that the marmosets were in a higher arousal state during running. Such a result is at least loosely consistent with effects seen in mice (although in that literature, there is some degree of dissociation between modulations due to arousal and those due to running per se [10]). Thus, the changes in pupil size we detected do suggest that when a marmoset runs, it is likely in a higher arousal state, similar to that in mice. However, more work would be required to perform cross-species calibrations to understand how the magnitude of changes in pupil size corresponds to changes in levels of arousal. At this point, we can conclude that the differences we see in the size (and sometimes, sign) of V1 modulations across species are unlikely due to a categorical difference in a link between running and arousal in mice versus marmosets, but possible quantitative distinctions deserve further consideration.
Discussion
In short, running does not affect V1 activity in marmosets like it does in mouse. The large, typically positive correlations between running and V1 activity often found in mice are simply not evident in marmosets. Although we matched our experimental protocol to mouse experiments and used the same metrics and analysis pipeline, the difference in results across species was stark. We hypothesize that this distinction holds at the level of taxonomic order, distinguishing how much behavioral state interacts with early stages of visual processing in primates versus rodents.
Diving deeper into the pattern of results, we did detect small (but statistically nonzero) modulations of marmoset V1 response correlated with running. In the foveal representation in V1– where we made the majority of our recordings– responses on average were slightly smaller during running; In the peripheral representation, responses were slightly larger. Despite the main result of this study being that running-correlated modulations in marmoset V1 are small, and hence quantitatively different than that in mouse V1, our population-level analyses did point towards a possible cross-species generalization. The same shared-gain model improved accounts of both mouse and marmoset V1 activity. These population-level gain modulations likely reflect modulatory inputs associated with behavioral state and arousal. This commonality connects with mechanistic knowledge of how V1 activity is modulated. The primate-rodent difference in the magnitude and sign of V1 gain modulations we observed is in fact consistent with known differences in neuromodulatory inputs related to arousal in rodent and primate V1 [35, 36]. In primates, the locations of ACh receptors allow cholinergic inputs to increase the activity of the majority of GABAergic neurons and hence suppress net activity via inhibition [37, 38], but pharmacologically and anatomically distinct cholinergic influences in rodent likely exert more complex effects on net activity, including disinhibition which can increase net activity [15, 17, 39]. Our population-level analyses also lay groundwork for connections to indirect and aggregate measures of neural activity made in humans under related conditions [40–42], as well as the typically small modulations seen in primate visual cortices elicited by carefully-controlled attentional tasks, which are more clear when population-level modulations are considered [43–45].
We also performed an analysis of whether eye movements might contribute to differential visual (retinal) stimulation, which in turn could differentially modulate visually-driven activity in V1 during running versus stationary periods. We found that there were subtle increases in eye movement frequency and saccade amplitude during running. However, saccades on average slightly increased V1 activity, so it seems unlikely that eye-movement mediated changes in retinal stimulation explain the modest decreases we observed during running. We found that analysis of eye movements was difficult in some of the mouse datasets. Because receptive fields in mouse V1 can be very large, uncontrolled (and/or uncharacterized) eye movements can not only create visual modulations of the stimulus on the screen, but can also hit the edges of the monitor under some viewing conditions. A related study [21] found that eye movements (or, their effects on retinal stimulation) explained all of the modulations of V1 activity that were correlated with facial/body movements in seated macaques. Further work will be needed to understand how much eye movements play a role in both running-correlated and movement-correlated modulations in the mouse. This will require monitoring eye movements and dissecting the ensuing retinal effects from those of other (body and face) movements [23]; all of these types of motor activity (and subsequent “sensory reafference”) may be partially correlated.
Our results (as well as those of Talluri et al. [21]) reveal a number of additional issues that should be addressed in follow-up work to even more tightly relate work across the two species. In our study, we attempted to match the overall treadmill apparatus and the visual stimuli used in the mouse studies. Even that required species-specific customization of the treadmill, as well as taking into account the higher spatial acuity of primate vision (which is why our study used much higher spatial frequencies in our set of drifting gratings). We describe how additional unresolved issues could be addressed for improved cross-species integration.
First, we analyzed pupil size and found that it was larger when the marmosets ran. At first glance, this suggests that running does indicate a more aroused internal state in the marmosets, as it likely does in mice [10]. However, it is less clear whether the magnitude of pupil size changes in marmosets corresponds to the same amount of arousal change that occurs in mice. Relative calibration of the dynamic range of pupil size (and measuring other biomarkers of arousal) may make for more satisfying inferences about internal states across species, as it has been shown that some (but not all) of running-correlated modulations are likely due to arousal [10, 46].
Second, although we found only small effects (relative to mouse) at the aggregate level, our results call for more specific investigations of modulations at the level of cell types and subcircuits [1, 11, 15, 16]. Such investigations may reveal more nuanced effects in primate V1, using tools that can better unpack the circuitry associated with factors such as cholinergic modulation, which are known to differ in important ways across rodent and monkey [35, 36]. Additionally, differences in feedback circuits also exist across the visual field representation within primate V1 [47]. This– and the proposition that mouse V1 may be a better model of primate peripheral vision [34] – have motivated us to perform more systematic and larger-scale recordings to compare the foveal and peripheral representations.
Third, our results call for additional study across other visual areas. In mice, the large effects on V1 activity are likely to affect all subsequent stages of processing [8], but in marmosets, the small effects are less likely to have pronounced downstream effects. That said, running may directly and more strongly interact with later stages of visual processing in primates. This would be consistent with differences in where canonical computations occur across species with different numbers of visual areas [3, 48, 49]. Such measurements in primate extrastriate visual areas are already in progress in our laboratory.
Finally, larger effects of behavioral state may still be found in primate V1: Other behaviors that more directly recruit active vision may reveal stronger modulations. In mice, running may have a more direct functional relation to visual processing. Marmosets may instead wish to recruit head or body movements that are not realizable in the head-fixed preparation that we used for eye-movement and neural recording. These questions will be addressed in freely-moving and head-free subjects.
Although our main result is simply that running-correlated modulations in marmoset V1 are small relative to those in mouse, we did find evidence for behaviorally-correlated population-level gain modulations in both species. This sort of commonality may support further cross-species generalizations that transcend simpler observations of empirical similarity or dissimilarity [50, 51]. Further work explicating how shared basic mechanisms may ultimately result in rather different patterns of interaction between vision and action will be critical for linking our understanding of cortical function between currently-preferred model organisms and across taxonomic orders. The results in this report reflect just the starting point for a larger comparative inquiry.
Materials and methods
We performed electrophysiological recordings in V1 of two common marmosets (1 male, “marmoset G”, and 1 female, “marmoset B”, both aged 2 years). Both subjects had chronically implanted N-form arrays (Modular Bionics, Modular Bionics, Berkeley CA) inserted into left V1. Implantations were performed with standard surgical procedures for chronically-implanted arrays in primates. Additional recordings were also performed using Neuropixels 1.0 probes [52] acutely inserted into small craniotomies (procedure described below). All experimental protocols were approved by The University of Texas Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee and in accordance with National Institute of Health standards for care and use of laboratory animals.
Subjects perched quadrupedally on a 12” diameter wheel while head-fixed facing a 24” LCD (BenQ) monitor (resolution = 1920x1080 pixels, refresh rate = 120 Hz) corrected to have a linear gamma function, at a distance of 36 cm (pixels per degree = 26.03) in a dark room. Eye position was recorded via an Eyelink 1000 eye tracker (SR Research) sampling at 1 kHz. A syringe pump-operated reward line was used to deliver liquid reward to the subject. Timing events were generated using a Datapixx I/O box (VPixx) for precise temporal registration. All of these systems were integrated in and controlled by MarmoView. Stimuli were generated using MarmoView, custom code based on the PLDAPS [53] system using Psychophysics Toolbox [54] in MATLAB (Mathworks). For the electrophysiology data gathered from the N-Form arrays, neural responses were recorded using two Intan C3324 headstages attached to the array connectors which sent output to an Open Ephys acquisition board and GUI on a dedicated computer. In electrophysiology data gathered using Neuropixels probes, data was sent through Neuropixels headstages to a Neuropixels PXIe acquisition card within a PXIe chassis (National Instruments). The PXIe chassis sent outputs to a dedicated computer running Open Ephys with an Open Ephys acquisition board additionally attached to record timing events sent from the Datapixx I/O box. Spike sorting on data acquired using N-Form arrays was performed using in-house code to track and merge data from identified single units across multiple recording sessions [55]. Spike sorting for data acquired using Neuropixels probes was performed using Kilosort 2.5.
Chronic N-Form array recordings
Chronic array recordings were performed using 64-channel chronically-implanted 3D N-Form arrays consisting of 16 shanks arrayed in a 4x4 grid with shanks evenly spaced 0.4 mm apart (Modular Bionics, Berkeley, CA, USA). Iridium oxide electrodes are located at 1, 1.125, 1.25, and 1.5 mm (tip) along each shank, forming a 4x4x4 grid of electrodes. Arrays were chronically inserted into the left dorsal V1 of marmosets G and B at 1.5 and 4 degrees eccentric in the visual field, respectively (confirmed via post-hoc spatial RF mapping).
Well-isolated single units were detectable on the arrays in excess of 6 months after the initial implantation procedure.
Acute Neuropixels recordings
Acute Neuropixels recordings were performed using standard Neuropixels 1.0 electrodes (IMEC, Leuven, Belgium). Each probe consists of 384 recording channels that can individually be configured to record signals from 960 selectable sites along a 10 mm long, 70 x 24 µm cross-section straight shank. Probes were lowered into right dorsal V1 of marmoset G via one of 3 burr holes spaced irregularly along the AP axis 4-5 mm from the midline for a single session of experiments. Natural images were played to provide visual stimulus as well as occupy the subject and keep them awake during insertion and probe settling. The temporary seal on the burr hole was removed, the intact dura nicked with a thin needle and the burr hole filled with saline. The probe was then lowered through the dural slit at 500 µm/minute, allowing 5 minutes for settling every 1000 µm of total insertion. The whole-probe LFP visualization was monitored during insertion for the characteristic banding of increased LFP amplitude that characterizes cortical tissue. The probe was inserted until this banding was visible on the electrodes nearest the tip of the probe, indicating that the probe tip itself had passed through the dorsal cortex and was within the white matter. The probe was then advanced until a second band became visible on the electrodes nearest the tip, indicating the tip of the probe had exited through the cortex of the calcarine sulcus. The probe was then advanced slightly until the entirety of the second LFP band was visible to ensure that electrodes covered the full depth of the calcarine cortex and the tip of the probe was located confidently within the CSF of the sulcus. The probe was then allowed to settle for 10 minutes. Active electrode sites on the probe were configured to subtend both dorsal and calcarine cortex simultaneously. Post-hoc receptive field recreation confirmed that visually-driven, tuned, V1 neurons were recorded at both foveal and peripheral eccentricities.
Mouse dataset from Allen Institute
Mouse data were downloaded from the publicly-available Visual Coding database at https://portal.brain-map.org/explore/circuits/visual-coding-neuropixels. We used the same analysis code to analyze these data and the marmoset data we collected.
General experimental procedure
Marmoset recording sessions began with eye tracking calibration. Once calibration was completed, the wheel was unlocked and the subject was allowed to locomote freely, head-fixed, while free-viewing stimuli. Trials for all stimuli were 20 sec long with a 500 ms ITI and a 20 sec long natural image interleaved every fifth trial to keep the subject engaged. Stimuli were shown in blocks of 10 minutes and a typical recording session consisted of 50 trials of calibration followed by 1 or 2 blocks of a drifting grating stimulus and 1 block each of the two mapping stimuli. To elicit sufficiently reliable and frequent running behavior, subjects were rewarded at set locomotion distance intervals unrelated to the stimulus or gaze behavior (typical rewards were 50-70 µL and distance required to achieve a reward usually varied between 20-75 cm; reward amounts and intervals were adjusted daily to maximally motivate the subject.)
Eye tracking calibration
While the wheel was locked, subjects were allowed to free-view a sequence of patterns of marmoset faces. Marmosets naturally direct their gaze towards the faces of other marmosets when allowed to free-view with little-to-no training, allowing for the experimenter to adjust the calibration offset and gain manually between pattern presentations. Faces were 1.5 degrees in diameter and were presented for 3 sec with a 2 sec ISI between patterns. A portion of presented patterns were asymmetrical across both the X and Y axes of the screen to allow for disambiguation in the case of axis sign flips in the calibration. 50 trials were presented before each recording session to verify and refine the calibration. Calibration drift between sessions was minimal, requiring minor (<1 deg) adjustments over the course of 1-2 months of recordings.
Drifting grating stimuli
The primary stimulus consisted of full-field drifting gratings. Gratings were optimized to drive marmoset V1 with 3 separate spatial frequencies (1, 2, and 4 cycles per degree), two drift speeds (1 or 2 degrees per second) and 12 orientations (evenly-spaced 30 degree intervals). Each trial consisted of multiple grating presentations, each with a randomized spatial frequency, drift speed, and orientation. Gratings were displayed for 833 ms followed by a 249-415 ms randomly jittered inter-stimulus interval. After each 20 second trial there was a longer 500 ms inter-trial interval. Every fifth trial was replaced with a natural image to keep subjects engaged and allow for visual assessment of calibration stability on the experimenter’s display.
Mapping of receptive fields
A spatiotemporal receptive field mapping stimulus, consisting of sparse dot noise, was shown during each recording session. One hundred 1 degree white and black dots were presented at 50% contrast at random points on the screen. Dots had a lifetime of 2 frames (16.666 ms). Marmosets freely viewed the stimulus and we corrected for eye position offline to estimate the spatial receptive fields using forward correlation [25].
Necessary differences between mouse and marmoset experiments
Although we sought to perform experiments in marmosets that were as similar as possible to mouse experiments, some differences in their visual systems and behavior made for differences. Because the spatial frequency tunings of marmoset and mouse V1 neurons are starkly different, we used stimuli with considerably higher spatial frequencies than in the mouse experiments. Relatedly, marmoset V1 receptive fields are much smaller than in mouse. Because we used full-field stimuli (to match mouse experiments), responses in marmoset V1 were likely affected by substantial amounts of surround suppression, which would reduce overall responses. We also learned that, although the marmosets were comfortable perched on the wheel treadmill, they did not naturally run enough for our experimental purposes. We therefore incorporated a reward scheme to motivate the subjects to run more frequently. Finally, the mouse dataset we analyzed comprised a large number of mice with a small number of sessions per mouse; as is required of work with nonhuman primates, we were limited to a smaller number of subjects (N=2), and ran many experimental sessions with each animal.
Session and cell inclusion criteria
For the analyses shown in Figure 2, sessions were included if they contained more than 250 trials and a proportion of trials running was not less than 10% or greater than 90%. For the mouse dataset, this yielded 25/32 sessions. For the marmoset dataset, this yielded 27/34 sessions. For the unit-wise analyses in Figure 3, super-sessioned units were included for analysis if they had more than 300 trials of data and a mean firing rate of >1 spike / second. This yielded 1168/2015 units in mouse and 786/1837 units in marmoset.
For the analyses shown in Figure 4, sessions were included using the same trial and running criterion as in Figure 2. Only units that were well fit by the stimulus + slow drift model (i.e., cross validated better than the null, see ’shared modulator model’) were included and sessions were excluded if fewer than 10 units met this criterion. This resulted in 31/32 sessions for mouse and 28/34 sessions for marmoset.
Analysis of tuning
We counted spikes between the 50ms after grating onset and 50ms after grating offset and divided by the interval to generate a trial spike rate. To calculate orientation tuning curves, we computed the mean firing rate each orientation and spatial frequency. Because we were limited by the animal’s behavior to determine the number of trials in each condition (i.e., running or not), we computed orientation tuning as a weighted average across spatial frequencies with with weights set by the spatial frequency tuning. We used these resulting curves for the all analyses of tuning. We confirmed that the results did not change qualitatively if we either used only the best spatial frequency or marginalized across spatial frequency.
Orientation selectivity index was calculated using the following equation
where θ is the orientation and r is the baseline-subtracted vector of rates across orientations.
Analysis of eye movement effects on neural response
To assess whether and how eye movements might differ between running and stationary periods (and perhaps account for some or all of the running-correlated modulations of V1 response), we started by counting the number of saccades within a bin corresponding to each stimulus presentation (from 0.2 sec before stimulus onset to 0.1 after offset), as well as calculating the average saccade size (vector magnitude) of those saccades. We then regressed these terms against the spike count in each bin, allowing us to estimate the effect of eye movements in units of spike rate (Hz). (We also analyzed the variance of the eye position signal and got similar results). For the analysis of pupil size, we used the values returned by our Eyelink eyetracker, averaged in the same bins as for the saccade analyses.
Shared modulator model
To capture shared modulator signals in an unsupervised manner, we fit our neural populations with a latent variable model [56]. The goal of our latent variable model was to summarize population activity with low-dimensional shared signal that operates as a gain on the stimulus processing (e.g. [30, 32]). In this model, the response of an individual neuron, ri on trial t is given by:
where the stimulus response fi[s(t)] is given by the tuning curve, gi(t) is a neuron-specific gain on the stimulus response, and bi is the baseline firing rate. Similar models have been employed to describe the population response in V1 in several species [29–32].
Because the gain signal is shared across neurons, we fit this model to all n neurons in a given recording at the same time. To capture the stimulus tuning curves, we represented the stimulus on each trial s(t) as an m-dimensional “one-hot” vector, where m is the number of possible conditions (Orientation × Spatial Frequency) and on each trial all elements are zero, except for the condition shown. Thus, f [s(t)] becomes a linear projection of the stimulus on the tuning curves, As(t), where A is an n × m matrix of tuning weights.
We decomposed the gain for each neuron on each trial into a rank 1 matrix that was rectified and offset by one, g(t) = ReLU[1 + z(t)w], where w is an n-dimensional vector of loadings that map the 1-dimensional trial latent z(t) to a population-level signal, z(t)w. This signal is offset by 1 and rectified such that it is always positive and a loading weight of zero equals a gain of 1.0.
To capture any unit-specific slow drifts in firing rate, we further parameterized b as a linear combination of five b0-splines evenly spaced across the experiment [57]. Thus, the baseline firing rate for each neuron, i, was a linear combination of five “tent” basis functions spaced evenly across the experiment,
Thus, the full model describes the population response as
The parameters of the model are the stimulus tuning parameters A, the shared gain, z, the gain loadings,
w, and the “tent” basis weights, bi,j’s.
We first fit a baseline model with only stimulus and baseline parameters
Following Whiteway and Butts (2017), we initialized A and b using fits from a model without latent variables and initialized the latent variable, z, and loadings, w, using an Autoencoder [58, 59]. We then fit the gain, loadings, and stimulus parameters using iterative optimization with L-BFGS, by minimizing the mean squared error (MSE) between the observed spikes and the model rates. The model parameters were regularized with a modest amount of L2-penalty and the amount was set using cross-validation on the training set. The latent variables were penalized with a small squared derivative penalty to impose some smoothness across trials. This was set to be small and the same value across all sessions. We reverted the model to the autoencoder initialization if the MSE on a validation set did not improve during fitting.
We cross-validated the model using a speckled holdout pattern [60] whereby some fraction of neurons were withheld on each trial with probability p=0.25. We further divided the withheld data into a validation set and a test set by randomly assigning units to either group on each trial with probability 0.5. The validation loss was used to stop the optimization during the iterative fitting and the test set was used to evaluate the models.
Acknowledgements
We thank Allison Laudano for animal and colony management and care, Christopher Badillo for apparatus design and fabrication, and Nika Hazen for assistance with animal work. Cris Niell, Cory Miller, Jude Mitchell, and Anne Churchland all provided valuable feedback on drafts of this paper. We thank the Visual Coding team at the Allen Institute for sharing the mouse data used in this paper (https://portal.brain-map.org/explore/circuits/visual-coding-neuropixels).
Funding
National Institutes of Health / BRAIN Initiative grant U01-UF1NS116377 (A.H.) National Science Foundation grant NSC-FO 2123605 (D.B.) National Institutes of Health grant K99EY032179-02 (J.Y.).
Competing interests
Authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Data and materials availability
All data in the main text or the supplementary materials are available upon request, and will be posted publicly at time of publication.
References
- 1.Modulation of Visual Responses by Behavioral State in Mouse Visual CortexNeuron 65:472–479
- 2.The evolution of visual cortex: where is V2?Trends in Neurosciences 22:242–248
- 3.Distributed hierarchical processing in the primate cerebral cortexCerebral Cortex :1–47
- 4.Highly Selective Receptive Fields in Mouse Visual CortexJournal of Neuroscience 28:7520–7536
- 5.Sensorimotor Mismatch Signals in Primary Visual Cortex of the Behaving MouseNeuron 74:809–815
- 6.Enhanced Spatial Resolution During Locomotion and Heightened Attention in Mouse Primary Visual CortexJournal of Neuroscience 36:6382–6392
- 7.Locomotion Controls Spatial Integration in Mouse Visual CortexCurrent Biology 23:890–894
- 8.Reduced neural activity but improved coding in rodent higher-order visual cortex during locomotionNature Communications 13
- 9.Vision and Locomotion Shape the Interactions between Neuron Types in Mouse Visual CortexNeuron 98:602–615
- 10.Arousal and Locomotion Make Distinct Contributions to Cortical Activity Patterns and Visual EncodingNeuron 86:740–754
- 11.Subthreshold Mechanisms Underlying State-Dependent Modulation of Visual ResponsesNeuron 80:350–357
- 12.Integration of visual motion and locomotion in mouse visual cortexNature Neuroscience 16:1864–1869
- 13.Effects of Locomotion Extend throughout the Mouse Early Visual SystemCurrent Biology Elsevier :2899–2907
- 14.Pupil Fluctuations Track Fast Switching of Cortical States during Quiet WakefulnessNeuron Elsevier :355–362
- 15.Behavioral-state modulation of inhibition is context-dependent and cell type specific in mouse visual cortexeLife eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd
- 16.Cellular mechanisms of brain state–dependent gain modulation in visual cortexNature Neuroscience 16
- 17.A Cortical Circuit for Gain Control by Behavioral StateCell 156:1139–1152
- 18.Vision Drives Accurate Approach Behavior during Prey Capture in Laboratory MiceCurrent Biology 26:3046–3052
- 19.Rapid Innate Defensive Responses of Mice to Looming Visual StimuliCurrent Biology 23:2011–2015
- 20.The evolution of eyesAnnual Review of Neuroscience 15:1–29
- 21.Activity in primate visual cortex is minimally driven by spontaneous movementsNature Neuroscience Nature Publishing Group :1953–1959
- 22.Spontaneous behaviors drive multidimensional, brainwide activityScience American Association for the Advancement of Science
- 23.Single-trial neural dynamics are dominated by richly varied movementsNature Neuroscience 22:1677–1686
- 24.Visual Coding - Neuropixels - brain-map.org
- 25.Beyond Fixation: detailed characterization of neural selectivity in free-viewing primatesbioRxiv
- 26.Uniformity and diversity of response properties of neurons in the primary visual cortex: Selectivity for orientation, direction of motion, and stimulus size from center to far peripheryVisual Neuroscience Cambridge University Press :85–98
- 27.Functional architecture of area 17 in normal and monocularly deprived marmosets (Callithrix jacchus)Visual Neuroscience Cambridge University Press :145–160
- 28.Nonsense correlations in neurosciencebioRxiv
- 29.Characterizing the nonlinear structure of shared variability in cortical neuron populations using latent variable modelsNeurons, Behavior, Data analysis, and Theory The neurons, behavior, data analysis and theory collective :1–22
- 30.The Nature of Shared Cortical VariabilityNeuron 87:644–656
- 31.Multiplicative and Additive Modulation of Neuronal Tuning with Population Activity Affects Encoded InformationNeuron 89:1305–1316
- 32.Partitioning neuronal variabilityNature Neuroscience 17:858–865
- 33.Mechanisms underlying gain modulation in the cortexNature Reviews Neuroscience 21:80–92
- 34.Walking humans and running mice: perception and neural encoding of optic flow during self-motionPhilosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 378
- 35.Translational implications of the anatomical nonequivalence of functionally equivalent cholinergic circuit motifsProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 116:26181–26186
- 36.Is There a Canonical Cortical Circuit for the Cholinergic System? Anatomical Differences Across Common Model SystemsFrontiers in Neural Circuits 12
- 37.Gain Modulation by Nicotine in Macaque V1Neuron 56:701–713
- 38.Tuned thalamic excitation is amplified by visual cortical circuitsNature Neuroscience 16:1315–1323
- 39.Inhibition of inhibition in visual cortex: the logic of connections between molecularly distinct interneuronsNature Neuroscience 16:1068–1076
- 40.Differential effects of walking across visual cortical processing stagesCortex 149:16–28
- 41.Overground Walking Decreases Alpha Activity and Entrains Eye Movements in HumansFrontiers in Human Neuroscience 14
- 42.The Effect of Locomotion on Early Visual Contrast Processing in HumansJournal of Neuroscience 38:3050–3059
- 43.Spatial attention decorrelates intrinsic activity fluctuations in macaque area V4Neuron 63:879–888
- 44.Attention improves performance primarily by reducing interneuronal correlationsNature Neuroscience 12:1594–1600
- 45.Attention stabilizes the shared gain of V4 populationseLife 4
- 46.Identification of a Brainstem Circuit Regulating Visual Cortical State in Parallel with LocomotionNeuron Elsevier :455–466
- 47.Retinotopic organization of feedback projections in primate early visual cortex: implications for active visionbioRxiv
- 48.Topography and Areal Organization of Mouse Visual CortexJournal of Neuroscience 34:12587–12600
- 49.Emergence of Orientation Selectivity in the Mammalian Visual PathwayJournal of Neuroscience 33:10616–10624
- 50.How Cortical Circuits Implement Cortical Computations: Mouse Visual Cortex as a ModelAnnual Review of Neuroscience 44:517–546https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-neuro-102320-085825
- 51.Mouse vision as a gateway for understanding how experience shapes neural circuitsFrontiers in Neural Circuits 8
- 52.Fully integrated silicon probes for high-density recording of neural activityNature 551:232–236
- 53.PLDAPS: A Hardware Architecture and Software Toolbox for Neurophysiology Requiring Complex Visual Stimuli and Online Behavioral ControlFrontiers in Neuroinformatics 6
- 54.he Psychophysics ToolboxSpatial Vision 10:433–436
- 55.A hardware/software system for electrophysiology “supersessions” in marmosetsbioRxiv
- 56.The quest for interpretable models of neural population activityCurrent Opinion in Neurobiology. Computational Neuroscience 58:86–93
- 57.Decision-related feedback in visual cortex lacks spatial selectivityNature Communications 12
- 58.Representation Learning: A Review and New PerspectivesIEEE Transactions on Pattern Analysis and Machine Intelligence 35:1798–1828
- 59.Revealing unobserved factors underlying cortical activity with a rectified latent variable model applied to neural population recordingsJournal of Neurophysiology 117:919–936
- 60.Unsupervised Discovery of Demixed, Low-Dimensional Neural Dynamics across Multiple Timescales through Tensor Component AnalysisNeuron 98:1099–1115
Article and author information
Author information
Version history
- Preprint posted:
- Sent for peer review:
- Reviewed Preprint version 1:
- Reviewed Preprint version 2:
- Version of Record published:
Copyright
© 2023, Liska et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
- views
- 1,684
- downloads
- 107
- citations
- 5
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.