Abstract
Foraging animals optimize feeding decisions by adjusting both common and rare behavioral patterns. Here, we characterize the relationship between an animal’s arousal state and a rare decision to leave a patch of bacterial food. Using long-term tracking and behavioral state classification, we find that food leaving decisions in C. elegans are coupled to arousal states across multiple timescales. Leaving emerges probabilistically over minutes from the high arousal roaming state, but is suppressed during the low arousal dwelling state. Immediately before leaving, animals have a brief acceleration in speed that appears as a characteristic signature of this behavioral motif. Neuromodulatory mutants and optogenetic manipulations that increase roaming have a coupled increase in leaving rates, and similarly acute manipulations that inhibit feeding induce both roaming and leaving. By contrast, inactivating a set of chemosensory neurons that depend on the cGMP-gated transduction channel TAX-4 uncouples roaming and leaving dynamics. In addition, tax-4-expressing sensory neurons promote lawn-leaving behaviors that are elicited by feeding inhibition. Our results indicate that sensory neurons responsive to both internal and external cues play an integrative role in arousal and foraging decisions.
Introduction
Persistent behavioral states subdivide continuous behavior into discrete modules that accomplish adaptive goals (Tinbergen, 1951). An example from behavioral ecology is foraging behavior, which is composed of locomotion patterns that unfold across short and long timescales. For example, the brief darting maneuvers that drive prey capture in hunting zebrafish are embedded within persistent locomotory arousal states that last for minutes (Marques et al., 2020). In recent years, classical ethological studies of behavioral states have been supplemented with machine vision, clustering, and classification algorithms that are well-suited to quantitative and systematic analysis (Berman et al., 2014; Schwarz et al., 2015; Wiltschko et al., 2015). A current challenge is to identify circuit mechanisms that couple long-term behavioral states to short-term motor actions in foraging and other goal-directed behaviors.
Foraging behaviors and the neural mechanisms that generate them have been a fruitful subject of study in the nematode Caenorhabditis elegans. While exploring a bacterial lawn, which corresponds to a food patch, C. elegans adopts two major behavioral states: roaming and dwelling (Ben Arous et al., 2009; Fujiwara et al., 2002). Roaming is an aroused state characterized by high locomotion speed with few reversals and turns, whereas dwelling is defined by low speed and high reorientation rates (Ben Arous et al., 2009; Flavell et al., 2013; Fujiwara et al., 2002). A third behavioral state, quiescence, is induced by stress, molting, or satiety (Gallagher et al., 2013; Hill et al., 2014; Raizen et al., 2008; Van Buskirk & Sternberg, 2007). The bistability of roaming and dwelling states is governed by neuromodulatory signaling via the neuropeptide Pigment Dispersing Factor (PDF-1), which promotes roaming by signaling through its cognate receptor PDFR-1, and the biogenic amine serotonin, which promotes dwelling by activating the serotonin-gated chloride channel MOD-1. These modulators act through distributed circuits within the 302 neurons of the animal’s nervous system, with multiple sources of each modulator and multiple receptor-expressing sites (Flavell et al., 2013).
Another foraging behavior studied in C. elegans is the decision to stay or leave a lawn of bacterial food. Leaving is infrequent on high quality food lawns but increases when feeding is impaired, the bacterial food is inedible, or food is depleted (Bendesky et al., 2011; Milward et al., 2011; Olofsson, 2014; Shtonda, 2006). Animals also leave more frequently from lawns of pathogenic bacteria (Pujol et al., 2001) and lawns spiked with aversive or toxic compounds (Melo & Ruvkun, 2012; Pradel et al., 2007).
The genes that regulate lawn-leaving behavior overlap with those that regulate roaming and dwelling states, although these behaviors have largely been studied separately. For example, roaming, dwelling, and leaving are all strongly influenced by sensory neurons, particularly those that express the cGMP-gated channel tax-4 (Davis et al., 2023; Fujiwara et al., 2002; McCloskey et al., 2017; Milward et al., 2011) (Table 1), and by endocrine DAF-7 (TGF-beta) and Insulin/DAF-2 (insulin receptor) signaling pathways whose peptide ligands are produced by sensory neurons (Ben Arous et al., 2009; Bendesky et al., 2011)(Bendesky et al., 2011).
Here, we characterize the behavioral patterns and circuit mechanisms that accompany lawn leaving decisions on short and long timescales. Using high-resolution imaging to monitor the behaviors of many individual animals, we find that leaving is a discrete behavioral motif that is generated probabilistically during the high-arousal roaming state. A signature of leaving behavior is a rapid acceleration in speed immediately before the leaving event. Roaming states evoked by neuromodulatory mutations, acute feeding inhibition, or optogenetic circuit manipulation all stimulate lawn leaving accompanied by the rapid acceleration motif. In addition, chemosensory neurons play an unexpectedly central role in linking internal states and behavioral dynamics.
Results
Animals make foraging decisions at the boundaries of bacterial food lawns
To study foraging decisions at high resolution, we filmed and quantified the behavior of individual animals on small lawns of bacterial food (∼3 mm diameter) on an agar surface (Fig. 1A,B). During a 40-minute assay, animals spent ∼97% of their time on the lawn, and preferentially occupied the edge of the bacterial lawn where bacterial density was highest (Fig. 1C, Fig. 1-S1A-D). As a result, the tip of the animal’s head encountered the lawn boundary approximately once per minute. A few of these encounters resulted in a lawn leaving event, in which the animal exited the bacterial lawn (Fig. 1D, Fig. 1 – supplemental video 1). Lawn-leaving occurred in 26% of the assays, with a mean value of one event per 100 minutes (Fig. 1D,H-I, Supplementary Table 1).
Once an animal left the lawn, it typically remained outside of the food for 1-2 minutes before re-entering the lawn (Fig. 1-S1E, Supplementary Table 1). Most edge encounters were not followed by leaving; instead, animals that poked their head outside of the lawn either continued forward locomotion (head poke-forward), paused (head poke-pause), or executed a reversal (head poke-reversal) (Fig. 1E-G, Fig. 1 – supplemental video 1). Head poke-reversals were the most common event after lawn edge encounter (1.1 events/min), followed by head poke-forward and head poke-pause events; lawn leaving events were the least frequent (Fig. 1I, Supplementary Table 1).
High resolution analysis revealed characteristic behavioral dynamics prior to different events. Each head poke encounter with the lawn edge was preceded by an increase in speed over three seconds, and resolved by a reversal, pause, or continued forward movement within 5 seconds (Fig. 1J). Lawn leaving events encompassed a longer acceleration that began 30 seconds before the leaving event (Fig. 1J). At longer time scales, the average speed over one minute before and after the edge encounter varied slightly for different classes of events, with head poke-forward events and lawn-leaving associated with the highest speeds. The different behavioral patterns preceding lawn leaving and head pokes suggest that lawn leaving is a distinct foraging decision, and not a random resolution of an edge encounter. In particular, it suggests that lawn leaving is linked to a persistent behavioral state reflected in ongoing locomotion speed.
Lawn leaving behavior is associated with high arousal states on short and long timescales
To gain further insight into the behavioral states preceding lawn leaving, we coarse-grained our behavioral measurements and expanded the time axis to allow analysis across different durations. On average, a slow rise in speed began two minutes before lawn leaving, with a rapid acceleration in the last minute before leaving (Fig. 2A). We analyzed these behaviors in the context of the well-characterized roaming and dwelling states (Ben Arous et al., 2009; Flavell et al., 2013; Fujiwara et al., 2002). Training a two-state Hidden Markov Model (HMM) based on absolute speed and angular speed (reorientations) recovered a bifurcation of roaming and dwelling states (Fig. 2-S1A-D) (Methods). Using these criteria, wild type animals on small lawns roamed for a median of 12% of the duration of the assay (Fig. 2-S1C, Supplementary Table 2). Although 80% of lawn leaving events occurred within roaming states, less than 1% of roaming states contained a lawn leaving event (Fig. 2B, Supplementary Table 2). Five minutes before leaving, 34% of animals were roaming, a fraction that steadily increased such that 80% of animals were roaming immediately before leaving (Fig. 2C, Supplementary Table 2). By contrast, the fraction of animals roaming before head poke-reversals increased only slightly before the event (Fig. 2-S3C). Within roaming states, the average speed was constant until the rapid acceleration ∼30 seconds prior to lawn leaving (Fig. 2D).
The roaming-dwelling HMM was initially developed on uniform bacterial lawns (Flavell et al., 2013), which elicit more roaming than the small lawns used here (Fig. 2-S1E-G, Supplementary Table 2). We therefore generated an orthogonal HMM behavioral state model trained on our experimental conditions to validate our conclusions. An Autoregressive Hidden Markov model (AR-HMM) (Buchanan et al., 2017; Linderman et al., 2020), trained over an expanded set of locomotion parameters, converged on four states that largely reflect forward locomotion speed and related features (Fig. 2-S2A-K, Methods).
These four states provided an orthogonal segmentation of locomotory arousal that partly overlaps with roaming and dwelling states (Fig. 2-S2M-N). Most lawn leaving events occurred during the high speed, high arousal AR-HMM state 3 (Fig. 2E). Animals spent a median of 9% of the time in state 3 (Fig 2-S2K, Supplementary Table 2), but five minutes before leaving, 26% of animals were in state 3, which ramped to 82% immediately before leaving (Fig. 2F, Supplementary Table 2). Like roaming, state 3 increased only slightly before head poke reversals (Fig. 2-S3E).
Both the two-state model and the four-state model show that arousal states increase before lawn leaving over at least two phases: an enrichment of a high arousal state 3-5 minutes before leaving, followed by a rapid acceleration in the last 30 seconds. Because the widely used two-state roaming-dwelling model captured the key features of lawn-leaving as well as these alternative methods, we applied that analysis to subsequent experiments.
Food intake regulates arousal states and lawn leaving
Animals roam more and leave lawns more frequently when bacteria are hard to ingest or have low nutritional value (Ben Arous et al., 2009; Shtonda, 2006). Conversely, simply increasing food concentration suppressed roaming and leaving on small lawns (Fig. 3A-B, Supplementary Table 3) (Bendesky et al., 2011). To separate the behavioral effects deriving from sensation of food odors from ingestion of food, we treated E. coli OP50 bacteria with aztreonam, an antibiotic that renders bacteria inedible to C. elegans by preventing bacterial cell septation (Ben Arous et al., 2009; Gruninger et al., 2008). Both lawn leaving and roaming were dramatically increased on aztreonam-treated E. coli lawns (Fig. 3C-D, Fig. 3-S1A-B, Supplementary Table 3). Animals on aztreonam-treated lawns maintained the characteristic speed acceleration in the last 30 seconds before leaving (Fig. 3-S1C).
Locomotion speed is regulated by direct interoception of pharyngeal pumping, the motor behavior associated with intake of bacterial food (Rhoades et al., 2019). To ascertain whether similar mechanisms affect arousal and lawn leaving, we acutely inhibited pharyngeal pumping by optogenetic activation of the red-shifted channelrhodopsin ReaChR in pharyngeal muscle (Lin et al., 2013) (Fig. 3E-J). Acute feeding inhibition strongly stimulated both roaming and lawn leaving (Fig. 3E-H, Fig. 3-S1D-E, Supplementary Table 3).
While roaming and head pokes began immediately upon feeding inhibition, leaving events did not occur immediately but instead accumulated throughout the 10-minute stimulation interval (Fig. 3I). In each case, leaving was preceded by a 30 second acceleration in speed (Fig. 3J). Thus, feeding inhibition elicits both roaming and lawn-leaving behaviors, and lawn-leaving occur probabilistically during roaming states.
Parallel regulation of arousal states and leaving by neuromodulatory mutants
To further examine the relationship between arousal states and lawn leaving, we examined mutants with well-characterized alterations in roaming and dwelling. Animals deficient in serotonin (tph-1), dopamine (cat-2), or the neuropeptide receptor NPR-1 (npr-1) roam at a higher rate than wild type (Cermak et al., 2020; Cheung et al., 2005; Flavell et al., 2013; Omura et al., 2012; Sawin et al., 2000). We found that each of these mutants showed increased lawn leaving, strengthening the observed correlation between roaming and leaving rates (Fig 4A-C, E-G). In addition, the fraction of animals roaming increased over several minutes before leaving events, as was observed in wild type animals (Fig. 4I-K). Finally, each mutant had a brief acceleration in speed during the 30 seconds prior to leaving (Fig. 4M-O). Thus, although the molecular basis of arousal is different in each of these mutants, the overall dynamics of roaming and lawn leaving are preserved across genotypes.
Animals lacking the G-protein coupled receptor Pigment Dispersing Factor Receptor (PDFR-1) roam less than wild type (Flavell et al., 2013; Ji et al., 2021; Meelkop et al., 2012). On the small lawns used here, pdfr-1 mutants roamed less, but left food lawns at the same low rate as wild type (Fig. 4D,H). Roaming rates increased similarly during the 3 minutes prior to lawn leaving in wild type and pdfr-1 animals, suggesting that the coupling of roaming and leaving does not require PDFR-1 signaling (Fig. 4L). Although their basal locomotion speed is lower (Flavell et al., 2013; Ji et al., 2021), pdfr-1 did accelerate slightly before lawn leaving (Fig. 4P). In summary, neuromodulatory mutants varied in the fraction of time spent roaming and dwelling, but in each case lawn-leaving behaviors were coupled to roaming and a brief speed acceleration.
Optogenetic inhibition of feeding elicited immediate roaming and probabilistic lawn leaving in both pdfr-1 and tph-1 mutants (Fig. 4-S1), indicating that neither of these neuromodulators is essential for interpreting feeding inhibition. This was surprising because the tph-1-expressing NSM neurons sense bacterial ingestion and signal food availability via serotonin release (Rhoades et al., 2019), and were therefore candidates to relay feeding signals. NSM may release additional transmitters as well as serotonin, or it may be redundant with additional neurons that detect feeding inhibition.
Acute circuit manipulation drives deterministic roaming and probabilistic leaving
As a complement to the neuromodulatory mutants, we employed a circuit-based approach to manipulate arousal levels and examine effects on lawn leaving. Roaming is strongly stimulated by pdfr-1; we defined sites of pdfr-1 expression that stimulate roaming using an intersectional Cre-Lox system that restores pdfr-1 expression in targeted groups of cells in pdfr-1 mutant animals (Fig. 5-S1, Fig. 5-S2A) (Flavell et al., 2013). Previous work identified a moderate effect of AIY, RIM, and RIA neurons as mediators of pdfr-1-dependent roaming (Flavell et al., 2013). We observed a stronger rescue of roaming upon pdfr-1 expression solely in the RIB neurons, which are active during rapid forward locomotion (Fig. 5-S2B-D) (Ji et al., 2021; Wang et al., 2020).
Following this result, we asked whether optogenetic activation of RIB might be sufficient for roaming in wild type animals. PDFR-1 signals through the heterotrimeric G protein Gαs to increase cAMP levels (Janssen et al., 2008), and optogenetic activation of groups of PDFR-1-expressing neurons with the bacterial light-activated adenylyl cyclase BlaC increases roaming (Flavell et al., 2013; Ryu et al., 2010). We found that acute optogenetic activation of only RIB with BlaC induced immediate roaming in over 80% of animals (Fig. 5A-C, Fig. 5-S3A-B).
RIB::BlaC stimulation also potentiated lawn leaving, with over 70% of animals leaving lawns during the stimulation period (Fig. 5D-E). Like lawn-leaving during feeding suppression, this behavior was probabilistic across the 10-minute light pulse (Fig. 5F). Animals accelerated slightly in the 30 seconds before leaving, but the speed increase appeared less pronounced than in other manipulations, suggesting that RIB activity may partially occlude the acceleration motif in lawn leaving (Fig. 5-S3F). Neither roaming nor lawn leaving was potentiated by blue light exposure in wild type control animals not expressing BlaC (Fig. 5C, Fig. 5-S3A, C-D).
Chemosensory neurons couple roaming dynamics, internal state, and lawn leaving
In addition to the neuromodulatory arousal systems, multiple food- and pheromone-sensing chemosensory neurons affect roaming, dwelling, and leaving behaviors (Table 1). Most of these neurons use the cyclic nucleotide-gated channel gene tax-4 for sensory transduction, and tax-4 mutants have diminished roaming behavior (Ben Arous et al., 2009; Fujiwara et al., 2002) (Fig. 6A, Table 1). However, we found that tax-4 mutant animals continued to leave lawns – indeed, they left at slightly higher rates than wild-type animals (Fig. 6B). Moreover, the temporal relationship between roaming and leaving was altered in tax-4 mutants, which typically roamed for only ∼1 minute prior to lawn leaving (Fig. 6C, Fig. 6-S1A). These results indicate that loss of tax-4 disrupted the characteristic arousal dynamics associated with leaving.
Many of the 15 classes of sensory neurons that express tax-4 have been implicated in roaming or leaving behaviors (Table 1). We rescued tax-4 separately in AWC, which senses food odors; ASK, which senses amino acids and pheromones; ASJ and ASI, which sense pheromones, food, and toxins; and URX/AQR/PQR, which sense environmental oxygen (Ben Arous et al., 2009; Bendesky et al., 2011; Greene et al., 2016; Milward et al., 2011). Significant effects on roaming or leaving behavior were observed upon ASJ, ASK, or AWC rescue (Fig. 6-S1 and S2). The strongest effects resulted from tax-4 rescue in the ASJ neurons, which partially restored roaming levels before lawn leaving (Fig. 6A-C, Fig 6-S1), and tax-4 rescue in the ASK neurons, which paradoxically suppressed leaving to a level below that of either wild type or tax-4 animals (Fig. 6-S2). Because ASJ and ASK showed opposite effects in these experiments, we also examined strains in which both neurons were rescued. Combined ASJ and ASK rescue normalized roaming and leaving compared to ASK rescue alone, albeit not to fully wild-type levels (Fig. 6-S2). These results suggest that multiple tax-4-expressing sensory neurons have roles in arousal-related behaviors and highlight ASJ as a regulator of roaming and leaving dynamics.
Next, we asked how tax-4 mutants responded to acute inhibition of feeding. As in wild-type animals, optogenetic inhibition of pharyngeal pumping resulted in an immediate and strong increase in roaming in tax-4 mutants (Fig. 6D, Fig. 6-S3A). However, feeding inhibition in tax-4 mutants increased leaving only slightly, unlike in the wild-type (Figure 6E). Both of these effects were rescued by expressing tax-4 in ASJ neurons (Fig. 6C-E, Fig. 6-S3B-D). Similarly, tax-4 animals on inedible food roamed at the same high rate as wild type animals but produced significantly fewer lawn leaving events (Fig. 6-S4); rescuing tax-4 in ASJ restored lawn leaving to wild-type levels. Thus tax-4 sensory mutants uncouple leaving behavior from its normal context in multiple ways: they can leave edible food lawns without an extended roaming state, and they are less likely to leave when feeding is inhibited, even while they are roaming.
Discussion
Both in the wild and in laboratory settings, foraging locomotion patterns exhibit scale invariance, meaning that similar statistics of movement displacement and duration arise on short and long timescales (Ayala-Orozco et al., 2004; Proekt et al., 2012). Long term arousal changes are regulated by neuromodulators that signal widely throughout the brain (Flavell et al., 2022; Taghert & Nitabach, 2012; Weissbourd et al., 2014), whereas moment-by-moment decision-making involves fast sensorimotor circuits (Gold & Shadlen, 2007). Here we show that C. elegans couples food leaving decisions, which unfold over seconds, to high arousal roaming states that last minutes. Although food intake and neuromodulatory signaling both alter the frequency of sustained C. elegans roaming states, these changes do not disrupt the coupling of roaming to food leaving decisions. Instead, sensory neurons link roaming and leaving behaviors, integrating these behavioral motifs, their dynamic properties, and their regulation by food intake.
Behavioral arousal alters probabilistic decision-making
From an ethological perspective, lawn-leaving is a classical foraging decision based on an animal’s assessment of the quality of a food patch (Charnov, 1976). Previous studies of lawn-leaving have identified features of the environment, like food availability, population density, and toxic repellents, that affect leaving probability as predicted by foraging theory. Here, we have extended these observations using the framework of computational neuroethology (Datta et al., 2019), examining behavior in detail across time to identify rare leaving events and the behavioral context in which they occurred. We found that leaving was tightly coupled to roaming, both during spontaneous behaviors and after acute manipulations that induced roaming states. In each case, and in neuromodulatory mutants that altered roaming frequency, leaving occurred when animals were roaming, occurred probabilistically over minutes during the roaming state, and was preceded by a brief 30-second acceleration in speed. The stereotyped features of leaving behavior, which were not shared by other lawn edge encounters, suggest that it is a discrete behavioral motif associated with roaming states. Leaving can be viewed as a form of decision-making—a discrete action that drives an adaptive behavioral choice between alternatives, influenced by internal states such as arousal (Kennedy et al., 2014).
Roaming and dwelling are a widely-used framework for defining arousal states in C. elegans, although changing experimental conditions or analysis methods can reveal sub-states or alternative classification frameworks (this work and Cermak et al., 2020; Gallagher et al., 2013; Raizen et al., 2008; Van Buskirk & Sternberg, 2007). Classical ethology presents such internal states as hierarchical and mutually exclusive (Tinbergen, 1951), while molecular and circuit analysis has extended and refined these ideas (Asahina et al., 2014; Flavell et al., 2013; Hindmarsh Sten et al., 2021; Hong et al., 2014; Moy et al., 2015; Pan et al., 2012). Our results indicate that lawn leaving behaviors are coupled to the roaming state, but with this state they are relatively rare and apparently probabilistic. More detailed studies may reveal additional regulators, such as an individual’s sensory experience, that shape the pattern of leaving behavior (Coen et al., 2014).
Sensory neurons integrate internal state and external sensation to guide foraging decisions
Our results identify two distinct biological mechanisms that regulate lawn leaving. First, leaving is coupled to arousal state. Leaving rates are 20-fold higher in roaming animals than in dwelling animals, and the leaving rates of most arousal mutants are largely explained by the fact that they spend more time roaming (Table 2). The second mechanism is controlled by tax-4 sensory neurons, which shape the behavioral dynamics of roaming and leaving across a range of conditions and stimulate leaving during feeding inhibition.
At a straightforward level, sensory neurons are well-placed to evaluate environmental quality in the framework of foraging theory. For example, ASK and AWC neurons sense amino acids and food odors (Bargmann, 2006), while ASJ neurons sense pheromones and toxins (Greene et al., 2016; Meisel et al., 2014), and other sensory neurons detect distinct environmental features (Bargmann, 2006).
Accordingly, many sensory neurons affect roaming, dwelling, or leaving (Table 1), and their relative importance varies with context, such as the presence of chemical repellents or pheromones that signal population density (Dal Bello et al., 2021; Greene et al., 2016; Milward et al., 2011; Pradel et al., 2007). Under the conditions used here, ASK neurons suppressed and ASJ neurons promoted roaming and leaving, respectively. Rescuing tax-4 in ASJ partially restored features of normal leaving dynamics, including roaming before leaving.
Others have shown that chronic feeding deprivation across hours drives lawn leaving through the action of tax-4 in several sensory neurons (Olofsson, 2014). An unexpected result, however, was that tax-4 sensory neurons were required to drive the high leaving rates after acute inhibition of feeding. Together, these results suggest that the sensory neurons are the sites at which internal feeding cues, behavioral states, and specific foraging decisions are integrated. Additional experiments are needed to define the full set of sensory neurons that couple feeding inhibition to leaving, the nature of the interoceptive signal of pharyngeal pumping, and the readout of the sensory neurons. Pharyngeal interoception is mediated in part by the serotonergic NSM neurons (Rhoades et al., 2019), but we found that serotonin was not essential for the effects of acute feeding inhibition. With respect to interoception, some of the tax-4- expressing sensory neurons detect tyramine, one of several biogenic amines that regulate lawn leaving (Bendesky et al., 2011; O’Donnell et al., 2020), and many detect neuropeptides as well (Hapiak et al., 2013).
Sensory neurons in C. elegans and other animals are most often studied in the context of rapid behavioral responses, but they also have critical roles in endocrine signaling. For example, intrinsically photosensitive retinal ganglion cells drive circadian entrainment in mammals (Hattar et al., 2002) and in cichlid fish, visual stimuli from females are sufficient to induce an endocrine androgen response in males (O’Connell et al., 2013). We speculate that the combination of endocrine signals, neuropeptides, and fast transmitters released by sensory neurons couples roaming states to leaving states. The insulin peptides and DAF-7-TGF-beta protein that regulate roaming, dwelling, and leaving are primarily produced by tax-4- expressing sensory neurons, so these neurons bridge sensory and endocrine regulation of foraging (Taylor et al., 2021). Expression of these genes is regulated by pheromones and metabolic conditions, providing an additional layer beyond neural activity for long-term regulation of behavioral state. Many tax-4 sensory neurons also release classical neurotransmitters such as glutamate (ASK, AWC) and acetylcholine (ASJ), and therefore have the potential to modulate rapid behaviors such as the lawn-leaving motif (Taylor et al., 2021). Future studies can test this hypothesis while determining the mechanisms by which sensory neurons integrate signals to trigger lawn leaving.
Methods
Nematode and bacterial culture
Bacterial food used in all experiments was E. coli strain OP50. Nematodes were grown at 20°C on nematode growth media plates (NGM; 51.3 mM NaCl, 1.7% agar, 0.25% peptone, 1 mM CaCl2, 12.9 μM cholesterol, 1 mM MgSO4, 25 mM KPO4, pH 6) seeded with 200 μL of a saturated E. coli liquid culture that had been grown at room temperature for 48 hours or overnight at 37°C (without shaking) from a single colony of OP50 in 100 mL of sterile LB (Brenner, 1974). All experiments were performed on young adult hermaphrodites, picked as L4 larvae the evening before an experiment. Wild type controls were the PD1074 sequenced strain derived from the N2 Bristol strain (Yoshimura et al., 2019), except for Figure 4 tph-1 and cat-2 controls, which were the CX0001 isolate of the N2 Bristol strain. Mutant strains were backcrossed into wild type to reduce background mutations. Transgenic strains were always compared to matched controls tested in parallel on the same days. Full genotypes and detailed descriptions of all strains and transgenes appear in Table 3: Strain details.
Molecular biology and transgenics
Strains tested for pdfr-1 rescue using PCR-amplified genomic fragments (Figure 5-S1) were from (Flavell et al., 2013). For cell-selective pdfr-1 cDNA expression under the pdfr-1 distal promoter using the floxed rescue construct (Figure 5-S2), Cre expression was driven by a pan-neuronal promoter (tag-168), in RIM (tdc-1), in AIY and others (mod-1), in RIA (glr-3), and in RIB (sto-3). A 972 bp region upstream of the sto-3 gene that drives expression solely in the RIB neurons was cloned using these primers:
sto-3: gatgcccaatcagttttttttcaccaa, aagccaaaccaagtgagaagaagtattca
Strains for tax-4 rescue were reported in Macosko et al., 2009 and Worthy et al., 2018. The sequences of the promoter ends are given below, along with the concentration at which the plasmid was injected for generating transgenic lines.
ASJ tax-4 rescue
srh-11: gggcaaggacaatgttgccgcag, tgggaataaaataacgacgtatgaata, 50 ng/μl ASK tax-4 rescue
sra-9: gcatgctatattccaccaaaagaaa, tagcttgtgcatcaatcatagaaca 50 ng/μl AWC tax-4 rescue
ceh-36: ctcacatccatctttctggcgact, ttgtgcatgcgggggcaggcga, 30 ng/μl ASI tax-4 rescue
str-3: gtgaacttgaaaagcgcaagtgatat, ttccttttgaaattgaggcagttgtc, 100 ng/μl URX/AQR/PQR tax-4 rescue
gcy-36: tggatgttggtagatggggtttgga, aaattcaaacaagggctacccaaca 2 ng/μl
Transgenic animals were generated by microinjection of DNA containing the genetic construct of interest, a fluorescent co-injection (myo-2p::mCherry, myo-3p::mCherry, elt-2p::nGFP, elt-2p::mCherry), and empty pSM vector to reach a final DNA concentration of 100 ng/μL. Transgenes were maintained as extrachromosomal arrays.
Small lawn foraging assay
For all assays, E. coli OP50 was grown overnight in a shaking LB liquid culture from a single colony at 37°C. On the morning of the assay, 400 μL of saturated liquid culture was diluted into 5 mL of LB and allowed to grow to OD1 at 37 °C (∼1.5 hours), as measured by spectrophotometer. The liquid culture was then spun down and resuspended in M9 buffer (3 g KH2PO4, 6 g Na2HPO4, 5 g NaCl, 1 ml 1 M MgSO4, H2O to 1 liter) then concentrated to a density of OD2 (and OD1 or OD4 in Figure 3A-B). To generate the test lawns, 2 μL of this concentrated bacterial resuspension was seeded onto NGM agar in the center of each well of a custom-made laser-cut multi-well plate (Stern et al., 2017). 50 μL of bacterial resuspension was seeded onto a separate NGM agar plate to be used as a food density acclimation plate. Lawns were grown at 20-22°C for 2 hours before the assay. Adult hermaphrodites picked as L4s 16-20 hours before the assay were then transferred to acclimation plates. After 45-90 minutes, animals were transferred to an unseeded NGM plate, cleaned of E. coli, and transferred singly into each well of the assay plates, where they were placed on bacteria-free agar and allowed to find the small food lawn on their own. Assays were recorded for 1 hour at 3 frames per second using 12 8.8 MP USB3 cameras (Pointgrey, Flea3) and 35 mm high-resolution objectives (Edmund Optics). LED backlights (Metaphase Technologies) provided uniform illumination of the assay plates. Commercial software (Flycapture, Pointgrey) was used to record the movies.
Uniform lawn assay
Assays testing worm behavior on uniform bacterial lawns were performed as in the small lawn foraging assay with the exception that instead of 2 μL of bacteria seeded in the center of the well, 15 μL of OD2 bacterial suspension was spread evenly throughout the well. Bacteria were grown for 2 hours before acclimation and starting the assay.
Optogenetic feeding inhibition assay
Pharyngeal pumping was inhibited by expressing the red-shifted channelrhodopsin ReaChR (Lin et al., 2013) under the myo-2 pharyngeal muscle promoter (a strain generously provided by Steve Flavell).
Animals were stimulated while navigating small food lawns described above. Experimental animals were grown on bacterial lawns containing 50 μM all-trans retinal (+ATR) overnight before assays. Control animals were placed on lawns made in parallel that did not contain retinal (-ATR). A 590 nm Precision LED with Uniform Illumination (Mightex) controlled with custom MATLAB software was used to deliver optogenetic stimuli. Animals were acclimated to small lawns (no ATR) for 20 minutes before being exposed to alternating 10-minute intervals of light OFF and light ON using 590 nm light at 60 μW/mm2 strobed at 10 Hz with a 50% duty cycle (Fig. 3, Fig. 4-S1). Recording hardware and software was identical to that of off-food foraging assays without optogenetic stimulation except that 475 nm short-pass filters were used on recording optics (Edmund Optics) to prevent overexposure of the video recording during light pulse delivery.
For behavioral quantification in Fig. 3E-F, Fig. 4-S1A,C-D, Fig 5B-C, and Fig 6-S3A, only data before, during and after the first light pulse is shown. For all other aggregate comparisons of behavior in light OFF and light ON conditions, both light OFF and light ON pulses were merged.
RIB::BlaC assay
Activation of the RIB neurons was accomplished using blue light-activated adenylyl cyclase BlaC (Ryu et al., 2010). BlaC was cloned into the pSM vector under the sto-3 promoter to drive expression in the RIB neurons. Stimulation was carried out as described for ReaChR (above), except no all-trans retinal was applied and the animals were exposed to blue light (455 nm) at 3 μW/mm2 strobed at 10 Hz with a 50% duty cycle (Fig. 5, Fig. 5-S3). 525 nm long pass filters were used on recording optics (Edmund Optics) to prevent overexposure of the video recording during light pulse delivery.
Inedible food assay
Experiments with inedible bacterial food generated by addition of aztreonam were performed following the protocol of (Gruninger et al., 2008). E. coli OP50 was grown in LB from a single colony to saturation overnight. On the morning of the assay, 400 μL of saturated liquid culture was diluted into 5 mL of LB with aztreonam (10 μg/ml) and allowed to grow to OD1 at 37 °C. To generate test lawns, 2 μL of this bacterial suspension was plated on NGM agar test plates containing aztreonam (10 μg/ml). Small lawn assays were performed as described above (see “Small lawn foraging assay”), except that bacteria on NGM+aztreonam plates were allowed to grow for 4 hours before assay testing. To test for any acute behavioral responses to aztreonam, we also performed a control “post-add” experiment, in which 2 μL of either 4 μg/mL aztreonam dissolved in DMSO or DMSO alone was added to normal small bacterial lawns after 4 hours of growth on NGM agar (Fig. 3-S1).
Behavioral tracking and lawn feature detection
Because each video recorded the behavior of up to 6 individual animals, videos were manually cropped so each surrounded just a single animal using FFmpeg software (Tomar, 2006). To extract animal positions and postures, captured movies were analyzed by custom made scripts in MATLAB (Mathworks, version 2021a) using the Image Processing Toolbox and the Computer Vision Toolbox. In each frame of the movie, the worm is segmented by background subtraction and its XY position is tracked over time using a Kalman filter. From the background-subtracted worm image, a smooth spline of 49 points was computationally applied and features relating to the movement of points along the body were derived following (Javer et al., 2018). Disambiguation of the head versus tail was determined by assigning the head as the end of the spline that had greater cumulative displacement over the video assay, facilitating determination of times when the animal moved forward and backward.
Behavioral features extracted
Features were defined as described in (Javer et al., 2018). Briefly, body parts were defined based on a skeletonized spline containing 49 points equally distributed along the length of the worm body. The head comprises spline points 1-8. The midbody comprises spline points 17-33. The positions of head and midbody were calculated by deriving the centroid of each of these point sets by averaging their x and y positions before subsequent analyses. See Fig. 2-S2.
Midbody speed: the derivative of displacement of the midbody across frames. Positive and negative numbers indicate forward and backward motion, respectively. Units: mm/sec.
Midbody angular speed: The angle change across midbody positions over time: Two vectors are measured: v0-1, representing the change in midbody position from time frame 0 to 1, and v1-2, representing the change in midbody position from time frame 1 to 2. the position change over three frames at each time point was quantified. Angular speed is defined as the arc-cosine of the dot product of v0-1 and v1-2 divided by the scalar product of the norms of these vectors. Units: degrees per second.
Head speed: the derivative of displacement of the head across frames. Units: degrees per second.
Head angular speed: Same as midbody angular speed but calculated for the head position. Units: degrees per second.
Head radial velocity relative to the midbody: The derivative of displacement of the head relative to the midbody across frames. This is calculated by subtracting the midbody position from the head position and shifting to polar coordinates (Φ,r) where Φ is the angular dimension and r is the radial dimension. Head radial velocity relative to midbody is the derivative of r with respect to time. Units: mm/sec.
Head angular velocity relative to the midbody: the derivative of the angular displacement of the head relative to the midbody across frames. The same procedure as above is used to generate polar coordinates. Head angular velocity relative to the midbody is the derivative of Φ with respect to time. Units: degrees per second.
Quirkiness: , where a is the minor and A is the major axis of a bounding box surrounding the animal, respectively. Values closer to 1 means the animal’s shape is more elongated and thinner; closer to 0 indicates a more rounded shape.
All of these features were binned into contiguous 10-second intervals for subsequent analyses. There are also several features only defined for 10-second bins:
Fraction of time moving forward per bin.
Fraction of time moving reverse per bin.
Fraction of time paused per bin.
Midbody forward speed, the mean of Midbody speed where values are >= 0.
Midbody reverse speed, the mean of Midbody speed where values are <= 0.
Lawn boundary-related metrics and Lawn boundary interaction behaviors
The outline of the bacterial lawn was determined by edge detectors applied to the background averaged across movie frames. Across every frame, the closest boundary point to the animal’s head was determined and used to calculate the “Lawn Boundary Distance.”
Lawn Boundary Distance and movement direction were used to classify a set of lawn boundary interaction behaviors. Head pokes were classified based on an excursion of the head lawn boundary distance that peaks outside the lawn before returning to the lawn interior. In the period following maximal displacement outside the lawn and before resuming locomotion inside the lawn (“recovery interval”), 3 types of head pokes were categorized: head poke forward, in which at least half of the recovery intervals is spent moving forward, head poke reversal, in which the animal executes a reversal during the recovery interval, or head poke pauses, in which an animal spends at least half of the recovery interval with speed less than 0.02 mm/sec. Lawn leaving events were marked as the first frame when the animal’s head emerged from the lawn before its entire body exited the lawn.
Quality control for including animals in subsequent analyses
Behavior and lawn features were detected and tracked over the 1-hour assay but only the latter 40 minutes of data were retained for analysis to minimize the effects from manipulating animals prior to recordings. Data from single animals were only retained in subsequent analyses if the following conditions were met: 1) the worm was visible in the video for at least half of the time the worm was recorded (cumulatively 30 minutes), 2) the worm was inside the bacterial lawn for at least one minute within the first 20 minutes of the video (before usable data was collected), 3) the plate was not bumped during the recording.
Quantitative Locomotion Analysis and HMM analyses
All quantitative analyses of locomotion and Hidden Markov Model building after behavioral tracking were performed in Python. For all analyses of animal locomotion and model-building, behavioral data from animals outside the food lawn was censored.
To classify roaming and dwelling states, speed and angular speed of animal centroid position was averaged into contiguous 10 second intervals. Roaming and dwelling states were classified as in (Flavell et al., 2013). Briefly, two classes of intervals corresponding to high angular speed / low speed and low angular speed / high speed were identified and separated by a line drawn at y (mm/sec) = x (deg/sec)/450. Behavior can then be instantaneously classified into roaming intervals when values are above the line, or dwelling intervals, when values are below the line. A 2-state categorical Hidden Markov Model was then trained on these behavioral sequences to generate roaming and dwelling hidden states using the SSM package (Linderman et al., 2020).
An Autoregressive Hidden Markov Model (AR-HMM) was trained to segment animal behavioral states on food using a different set of behavioral features relating to forward body movement and head movements: fraction of time moving forward per 10-sec bin, midbody forward speed, midbody angular speed, head angular velocity relative to midbody and head radial velocity relative to midbody (Fig. 2, Fig. 2-S2, see above for feature definitions).
Formally, at each time step, hidden states zt follow Markovian dynamics, , where is the transition matrix and corresponds to the k-th row. Given hidden states zt, the resulting feature dynamics are given by , where are linear dynamics and covariance matrices, respectively, and is the bias. The linear dynamics matrix A defines a set of equations that define a vector field in the feature dimensions. The bias term b can drive the dynamics in a particular direction. In the case where A is all zeros, the system has no dynamics and this amounts to a Hidden Markov Model with Gaussian emissions. AR-HMMs were also trained using the SSM package (Linderman et al., 2020). AR-HMM performance was evaluated by calculating the ratio of the log-likelihood of held-out test data set using the AR-HMM same data under the AR-HMM and a multivariate Gaussian model .
Sample Size Determination, Replicates and Group Allocation
The number of animals in each experiment is detailed in the figure legends. Assays were typically repeated across at least two days of experiments to account for day-to-day variability. Control animals were always run on the same days as experimental animals.
Using G*POWER 3.1, we chose sample sizes based on the desired ability to detect an effect size of 1 with 80% power and a 5% alpha, yielding a minimum value of n=18 animals per group for a two-sample unmatched comparison of means and n=11 animals per group for a matched comparison of means (Faul et al., 2009).
Quantification and Statistical Analysis
All comparisons in the fraction of time roaming were performed after logit-transformation , where p is the fraction of time spent roaming per animal.
Either Wilcoxon rank sum test or paired t-tests were used to compare matched data (roaming speed at -3 to -1 minutes vs. last 30 seconds before leaving, number of lawn leaving events during light OFF vs. ON, fraction of time roaming during light OFF vs. ON).
The Kolmogorov-Smirnov test was used for comparing cumulative distributions.
If multiple pairwise tests were done, multiple hypothesis correction was always performed.
Statistical details for each experiment are described in the figure legends. The p values resulting from all statistical tests performed in the paper can be found in Table 4.
Materials Availability Statement
All primary behavioral data and relevant code for data analysis are available at Dryad (URL goes here) and Github (https://github.com/BargmannLab/Scheer_Bargmann2023). Source data files contain the summarized data for all plots in Figures 1-6.
Acknowledgements
We thank Cheryl Mai for help with optogenetics experiments. We thank Philip Kidd, Audrey Harnagel, Yarden Wiesenfeld, and Steven Flavell for thoughtful discussions and comments on the manuscript. This work was supported by the Chan Zuckerberg Initiative and by an NIH F31 Predoctoral NRSA Fellowship to E.S.
Competing interests
No competing interests declared.
Supplemental Videos
Figure 1 – supplement video 1:
Lawn boundary encounters lead to different behavioral responses: lawn leaving, head poke forward, head poke pause, and head poke reversal. Green dot indicates the head position. White line indicates the lawn boundary. Forward and Reverse motion indicated in top left corner.
Figure 2 – supplement video 1:
Example of animal behavior with roaming and dwelling annotated. Animal shown corresponds to the behavior before and after the first lawn leaving event in Fig. 2-S2P.
Green dot indicates the head position. White line indicates the lawn boundary. Forward and Reverse motion indicated in top left corner. Speed indicated top middle. Time stamps indicated top right. Video sped up 7x.
Figure 2 – supplement video 2:
The same animal and time steps as in Fig. 2-Supplemental video 1, except annotated with AR-HMM states instead of roaming and dwelling.
References
- Tachykinin-expressing neurons control male-specific aggressive arousal in DrosophilaCell 156:221–235https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cell.2013.11.045
- Lévy walk patterns in the foraging movements of spider monkeys (Ateles geoffroyi)Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 55:223–230https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-003-0700-6
- The C. elegans Research CommunityWormBook https://doi.org/10.1895/wormbook.1.123.1
- Molecular and Sensory Basis of a Food Related Two-State Behavior in C. elegansPLoS ONE 4https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0007584
- Catecholamine receptor polymorphisms affect decision-making in C. elegansNature 472:313–318https://doi.org/10.1038/nature09821
- Mapping the stereotyped behaviour of freely moving fruit fliesJournal of the Royal Society Interface 11https://doi.org/10.1098/rsif.2014.0672
- Quantifying the behavioral dynamics of C . elegans with autoregressive hidden Markov models:1–5
- Whole-organism behavioral profiling reveals a role for dopamine in state dependent motor program coupling in C. elegansELife 9:1–34https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.57093
- Optimal foraging, the marginal value theoremTheoretical Population Biology 9:129–136https://doi.org/10.1016/0040-5809(76)90040-X
- Experience-dependent modulation of C. elegans behavior by ambient oxygenCurrent Biology 15:905–917https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2005.04.017
- Dynamic sensory cues shape song structure in DrosophilaNature 507:233–237https://doi.org/10.1038/nature13131
- Inversion of pheromone preference optimizes foraging in C. elegansELife 10:1–21https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.58144
- Computational Neuroethology: A Call to ActionNeuron 104:11–24https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2019.09.038
- G protein-coupled receptor kinase-2 (GRK-2) controls exploration through neuropeptide signaling in Caenorhabditis elegansPLOS Genetics 19https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pgen.1010613
- Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analysesBehavior Research Methods 41:1149–1160
- The emergence and influence of internal statesNeuron 110:2545–2570https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2022.04.030
- Serotonin and the Neuropeptide PDF Initiate and Extend Opposing Behavioral States in C. elegansCell 154:1023–1035https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cell.2013.08.001
- Regulation of body size and behavioral state of C. elegans by sensory perception and the egl-4 cGMP-dependent protein kinaseNeuron 36:1091–1102https://doi.org/10.1016/S0896-6273(02)01093-0
- The geometry of locomotive behavioral states in C. elegansPLoS ONE 8https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0059865
- The neural basis of decision makingAnnual Review of Neuroscience 30:535–574https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.29.051605.113038
- Regulatory changes in two chemoreceptor genes contribute to a Caenorhabditis elegans QTL for foraging behaviorELife 5:1–19https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.21454
- Sensory perception of food and insulin-like signals influence seizure susceptibilityPLoS Genetics 4https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pgen.1000117
- Neuropeptides amplify and focus the monoaminergic inhibition of nociception in Caenorhabditis elegansJournal of Neuroscience 33:14107–14116https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1324-13.2013
- Melanopsin-containing retinal ganglion cells: Architecture, projections, and intrinsic photosensitivityScience 295:1065–1070https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1069609
- Cellular stress induces a protective sleep-like state in C. elegansCurrent Biology 24:2399–2405https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2014.08.040
- Sexual arousal gates visual processing during Drosophila courtshipNature 595:549–553https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-021-03714-w
- Antagonistic control of social versus repetitive self-grooming behaviors by separable amygdala neuronal subsetsCell 158:1348–1361https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cell.2014.07.049
- Functional characterization of three G protein-coupled receptors for Pigment Dispersing Factors in Caenorhabditis eleganshttps://doi.org/10.1074/jbc.M709060200
- Powerful and interpretable behavioural features for quantitative phenotyping of Caenorhabditis elegansPhilosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2017.0375
- A neural circuit for flexible control of persistent behavioral statesELife 10https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.62889
- Global Brain Dynamics Embed the Motor Command Sequence of Caenorhabditis elegansCell 163:656–669https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cell.2015.09.034
- Internal states and behavioral decision-making: Toward an integration of emotion and cognitionCold Spring Harbor Symposia on Quantitative Biology 79:199–210https://doi.org/10.1101/sqb.2014.79.024984
- ReaChR: A red-shifted variant of channelrhodopsin enables deep transcranial optogenetic excitationNature Neuroscience 16:1499–1508https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3502
- Linderman, S., Antin, B., Zoltowski, D., & Glaser, J. (2020). SSM: Bayesian Learning and Inference for State Space Models (0..1).SSM: Bayesian Learning and Inference for State Space Models 0
- Internal state dynamics shape brainwide activity and foraging behaviourNature 577:239–243https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-019-1858-z
- Food responsiveness regulates episodic behavioral states in Caenorhabditis elegansJournal of Neurophysiology 117:1911–1934https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00555.2016
- PDF receptor signaling in Caenorhabditis elegans modulates locomotion and egg-layingMolecular and Cellular Endocrinology 361:232–240https://doi.org/10.1016/j.mce.2012.05.001
- Chemosensation of bacterial secondary metabolites modulates neuroendocrine signaling and behavior of C. elegansCell 159:267–280https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cell.2014.09.011
- Inactivation of conserved C. elegans genes engages pathogen- and xenobiotic-associated defensesCell 149:452–466https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cell.2012.02.050
- Neuronal and molecular substrates for optimal foraging in Caenorhabditis elegansProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 108:20672–20677https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1106134109
- Computational methods for tracking, quantitative assessment, and visualization of C. elegans locomotory behaviorPLoS ONE 10:1–22https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0145870
- Neuroendocrine Mechanisms Underlying Sensory Integration of Social SignalsJournal of Neuroendocrinology 25:644–654https://doi.org/10.1111/jne.12045
- A neurotransmitter produced by gut bacteria modulates host sensory behaviourNature 583:415–420https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-020-2395-5
- The olfactory neuron AWC promotes avoidance of normally palatable food following chronic dietary restrictionJournal of Experimental Biology 217:1790–1798https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.099929
- Dopamine signaling is essential for precise rates of locomotion by C. elegansPLoS ONE 7https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0038649
- Joint control of Drosophila male courtship behavior by motion cues and activation of male-specific P1 neuronsProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 109:10065–10070https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1207107109
- Detection and avoidance of a natural product from the pathogenic bacterium Serratia marcescens by Caenorhabditis elegansProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 104:2295–2300https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0610281104
- Scale invariance in the dynamics of spontaneous behaviorProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 109:10564–10569https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1206894109
- A reverse genetic analysis of components of the Toll signaling pathway in Caenorhabditis elegansCurrent Biology 11:809–821https://doi.org/10.1016/S0960-9822(01)00241-X
- Lethargus is a Caenorhabditis elegans sleep-like stateNature 451:569–572https://doi.org/10.1038/nature06535
- ASICs Mediate Food Responses in an Enteric Serotonergic Neuron that Controls Foraging BehaviorsCell 176:85–97https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cell.2018.11.023
- Natural and engineered photoactivated nucleotidyl cyclases for optogenetic applicationsJournal of Biological Chemistry 285:41501–41508https://doi.org/10.1074/jbc.M110.177600
- C. elegans locomotory rate is modulated by the environment through a dopaminergic pathway and by experience through a serotonergic pathwayNeuron 26:619–631https://doi.org/10.1016/S0896-6273(00)81199-X
- Changes in postural syntax characterize sensory modulation and natural variation of C. elegans LocomotionPLoS Computational Biology 11:1–16https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004322
- Dietary choice behavior in Caenorhabditis elegansJournal of Experimental Biology 209:89–102https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.01955
- Neuromodulatory control of long-term behavioral patterns and individuality across developmentCell 171:1649–1662https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cell.2017.10.041
- Peptide neuromodulation in invertebrate model systemsNeuron 76:82–97https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2012.08.035
- Molecular topography of an entire nervous systemCell 184:4329–4347https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cell.2021.06.023
- The study of instinctOxford: Clarendon Press
- Converting video formats with FFmpegLinux Journal 2006
- Epidermal growth factor signaling induces behavioral quiescence in Caenorhabditis elegansNature Neuroscience 10:1300–1307https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1981
- Flexible motor sequence generation during stereotyped escape responsesELife https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.56942
- Presynaptic partners of dorsal raphe serotonergic and GABAergic neuronsNeuron 83:645–662https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2014.06.024
- Mapping sub-second structure in mouse behaviorNeuron 88:1121–1135https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2015.11.031
- Identification of odor blend used by Caenorhabditis elegans for pathogen recognitionChem. Senses 43:169–180https://doi.org/10.1093/chemse/bjy001
- Recompleting the Caenorhabditis elegans genomeGenome Research 29:1009–1022https://doi.org/10.1101/gr.244830.118
Article and author information
Author information
Version history
- Preprint posted:
- Sent for peer review:
- Reviewed Preprint version 1:
- Reviewed Preprint version 2:
- Version of Record published:
Copyright
© 2023, Elias Scheer & Cornelia I. Bargmann
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
- views
- 2,319
- downloads
- 151
- citations
- 7
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.