The magnitude and time course of pre-saccadic foveal prediction depend on the conspicuity of the saccade target

  1. Department of Psychology, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Berlin, Germany
  2. Berlin School of Mind and Brain, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Berlin, Germany
  3. Exzellenzcluster Science of Intelligence, Technische Universität Berlin, Berlin, Germany
  4. Bernstein Center for Computational Neuroscience Berlin, Berlin, Germany

Peer review process

Revised: This Reviewed Preprint has been revised by the authors in response to the previous round of peer review; the eLife assessment and the public reviews have been updated where necessary by the editors and peer reviewers.

Read more about eLife’s peer review process.

Editors

  • Reviewing Editor
    Krystel Huxlin
    University of Rochester, Rochester, United States of America
  • Senior Editor
    Joshua Gold
    University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, United States of America

Reviewer #1 (Public review):

Summary:

This study examines to what extent this phenomenon varies based on the visibility of the saccade target. Visibility is defined as the contrast level of the target with respect to the noise background, and it is related to the signal-to-noise ratio of the target. A more visible target facilitates the oculomotor behavior planning and execution, however, as speculated by the authors, it can also benefit foveal prediction even if the foveal stimulus visibility is maintained constant. Remarkably, the authors show that presenting a highly visible saccade target is beneficial for foveal vision as detection of stimuli with an orientation similar to that of the saccade target is improved, the lower is the saccade target visibility, the less prominent is this effect.

Strengths:

The results are convincing and the research methodology is technically sound.

Weaknesses:

It is still unclear why the pre-saccadic enhancement would oscillate for targets with higher opacity levels, and what would be the benefit of this oscillatory pattern. The authors do not speculate too much on this and loosely relate it to feedback processes, which are characterized by neural oscillations in a similar range.

Reviewer #2 (Public review):

Summary:

In this manuscript, the authors ran a dual task. Subjects monitored a peripheral location for a target onset (to generate a saccade to), and they also monitored a foveal location for a foveal probe. The foveal probe could be congruent or incongruent with the orientation of the peripheral target. In this study, the authors manipulated the conspicuity of the peripheral target, and they saw changes in performance in the foveal task. However, the changes were somewhat counterintuitive.

Strengths:

The authors use solid analysis methods and careful experimental design.

Comments on revisions:

The authors have addressed my previous comments.

One minor thing is that I am confused by their assertion that there was no smoothing in the manuscript (other than the newly added time course analysis). Figure 3A and Figure 6 seem to have smoothing to me.

Another minor comment is related to the comment of Reviewer 1 about oscillations. Another possible reason for what looks like oscillations is saccadic inhibition. when the foveal probe appears, it can reset the saccade generation process. when aligned to saccade onset, this appears like a characteristic change in different parameters that is time-locked to saccade onset (about a 100 ms earlier). So, maybe the apparent oscillation is a manifestation of such resetting and it's not really an oscillation. so, I agree with Reviewer 1 about removing the oscillation sentence from the abstract.

Author response:

The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

Reviewer #1 (Public Reviews):

Summary:

This study examines to what extent this phenomenon varies based on the visibility of the saccade target. Visibility is defined as the contrast level of the target with respect to the noise background, and it is related to the signal-to-noise ratio of the target. A more visible target facilitates the oculomotor behavior planning and execution, however, as speculated by the authors, it can also benefit foveal prediction even if the foveal stimulus visibility is maintained constant. Remarkably, the authors show that presenting a highly visible saccade target is beneficial for foveal vision as the detection of stimuli with an orientation similar to that of the saccade target is improved, the lower the saccade target visibility, the less prominent the effect.

Strengths:

The results are convincing and the research methodology is technically sound.

Weaknesses:

Discussion on how this phenomenon may unfold in natural viewing conditions when the foveal and saccade target stimuli are complex and are constituted by different visual properties is lacking. Some speculations regarding feedforward vs feedback neural processing involved in the phenomenon and the speed of the feedforward signal in relation to the visibility of the target, are not well justified and not clearly supported by the data.

We thank the reviewer for their comment. In general, we tried to address conceptual points only briefly in this Research Advance if we had discussed them in depth in our main article which this advance will be linked to (Kroell & Rolfs, 2022: https://elifesciences.org/articles/78106). However, the reviews showed us that this rendered our theoretical reasoning in the current manuscript appear incomplete. In the revised Discussion section, we have elaborated on several conceptual questions. In particular, we expand on the transferability of our findings to natural viewing conditions:

“Foveal prediction in natural visual environments

As noted above, human observers typically move their eyes towards the most conspicuous objects in their environment (‘t Hart, Schmidt, Roth, & Einhäuser, 2013). Foveal prediction seems to benefit from this strategy as the strength of the predicted signal increases with the conspicuity of the eye movement target. Nonetheless, natural visual environments as well as naturalistic viewing behavior pose several challenges for the foveal prediction mechanism (see Kroell & Rolfs, 2022, for an initial discussion).

First, naturalistic saccade target stimuli will likely exhibit complex shapes and, more often than not, will include feature conjunctions rather than isolated features. Previous findings suggest that the foveal feedback mechanism is capable of operating at this level of complexity: High-level peripheral information such as the category of novel, rendered objects (Williams et al., 2008) has been successfully decoded from activation in foveal retinotopic cortex. If, indeed, temporal objectspecific areas such as area TE send feedback, the foveal prediction mechanism may even be specialized for the transfer of complex visual properties.

Second, foveal input will often be of high contrast in natural visual environments. If fed-back predictive signals can influence foveal perception in the presence of high-contrast feedforward input remains to be established. In our main investigation (Kroell & Rolfs, 2022; Figure 2B) as well as in previous studies (Hanning & Deubel, 2022b), pre-saccadic foveal detection performance decreased markedly in the course of saccade preparation, presumably because visuospatial attention gradually shifted towards the saccade target and away from the foveal location. This presaccadic decrease in foveal sensitivity may boost the relative weight of fed-back signals by attenuating the conspicuity of high-contrast feedforward input. In other words, the strength of feedforward input to the fovea is reduced gradually across saccade preparation. At the same time, the strength of the fed-back predictive signal should profit from the high contrast of naturalistic saccade targets.

Third, while foveal and peripheral information was congruent on 50% of all ‘probe present’ trials in our investigation, peripheral and foveal features will often be weakly correlated or even uncorrelated in natural environments (see Samonds, Geisler, & Priebe, 2018). Again, the presaccadic attenuation of foveal feedforward processing may allow fed-back peripheral signals to influence perception even if they are uncorrelated with foveal information. Moreover, in piloting variations of our paradigm, we observed that the subjective impression of perceiving the saccade target at the pre-saccadic foveal location is most pronounced if the foveal noise region is replaced with a black Gaussian blob at certain time points before saccade onset (unpublished phenomenological accounts). In consequence, fed-back signals do not seem to require correlated feedforward input to influence perception. Quantitative evidence, however, remains to be established.

Lastly, pre-saccadic foveal input is likely less relevant during natural viewing behavior than it is in our task. It is possible that this task-induced prioritization of the foveal location facilitated the emergence of congruency effects. In a previous experiment (Kroell & Rolfs, 2022; Figure 1D), however, the perceptual probe could appear anywhere on a horizontal axis of 9 dva length around the fixation location. Despite this spatial unpredictability, congruency effects peaked at the presaccadic foveal location, even after peripheral baseline performances had been raised to a foveal level through an adaptive increase in probe opacity. On a similar note, the orientation of the saccade target is irrelevant to the behavioral task in our design, mirroring naturalistic situations: The eye movement can be planned and executed based on local contrast variations alone, and observers are never required to report on the orientation of the peripheral target stimulus. Ultimately, however, an influence of task demands on visual processing can only be fully excluded through techniques that provide a direct readout of perceptual contents without requiring overt responses. In psychophysical investigations, a prediction of saccade target motion may be read out from observers’ eye velocities (Kroell, Mitchell, & Rolfs, 2023; Kwon, Rolfs, & Mitchell, 2019). In electroencephalographic (EEG) and electrophysiological studies, foveal predictions should manifest in early visually evoked potentials (e.g., Creel, 2019) and increased firing rates of featureselective foveal neurons in early visual areas, respectively. In conclusion, previous findings (Williams et al., 2008), the assumed properties of the neuronal feedback mechanism (Williams et al., 2008; Bullier, 2001) and characteristics of our current and previous experimental paradigms collectively suggest that foveal feature predictions are likely to transfer to naturalistic environments and viewing situations. Experimental evidence remains to be established.”

We have furthermore modified the Abstract to emphasize the connection of the current manuscript to the main article.

With respect to the reviewer’s point that “speculations regarding feedforward vs feedback neural processing involved in the phenomenon and the speed of the feedforward signal in relation to the visibility of the target, are not well justified”:

Again, we understand that we should have elaborated on our theoretical reasoning in this Research Advance. The assumption that our initial findings rely on neuronal feedback to foveal retinotopic cortex is derived from Williams et al.’s (2008) seminal findings: In an fMRI study, the category of peripherally presented objects could be decoded from voxels in foveal retinotopic cortex, suggesting that peripheral visual information was available to neurons with strictly foveal receptive fields. We extended these findings to saccade preparation, suggesting that feedback from higher-order, non-retinotopically organized visual areas may transmit information without the requirement of efference copies (see Kroell, 2023; Dissertation; https://doi.org/10.18452/27204, pp. 54-59): Irrespective of the vector of the upcoming saccade, the features of the attended saccade target would invariably be relayed to foveal retinotopic cortex. Ultimately, only anatomical and functional studies in non-human primates can conclusively establish the role of feedback connections in the observed foveal prediction effects. At present, however, this parsimonious model could account for all of our current and previous findings, that is, a temporally, spatially and feature-specific anticipation of saccade target properties in the presaccadic center of gaze. Nonetheless, we are open to considering any other mechanism that may account for our findings, and have integrated the explanation provided by the reviewer into the paragraph on potential thalamic mechanisms (see the reviewer’s Major Point 1).

Concerning the point that the “some speculations regarding feedforward vs feedback neural processing […] and the speed of the feedforward signal in relation to the visibility of the target are not well justified and not clearly supported by the data”:

Theoretical considerations on the impact of peripheral target contrast on feedforward processing speed were a main motivation for the current study. We apologize if our theoretical reasoning was incomplete and have added additional references and elaborations to the Introduction:

“In particular, neuronal response latencies decrease systematically as the contrast of visual input increases. While this phenomenon is reliably observed at varying stages of the visual processing hierarchy—such as the lateral geniculate nucleus (Lee, Elepfandt, & Virsu, 1981b), primary visual cortex (e.g., Albrecht, 1995; Carandini & Heeger, 1994; Carandini, Heeger, & Movshon, 1997; Carandini, Heeger, & Senn, 2002), and anterior superior temporal sulcus (STSa; Oram, Xiao, Dritschel, & Payne, 2002; van Rossum, van der Meer, Xiao, & Oram, 2008)—influences of contrast on neuronal response latency are particularly pronounced in higher-order visual areas: A doubling of stimulus contrast has been shown to decrease the latency of V1 neurons by 8 ms, compared to a reduction of 33 ms in area STSa (Oram et al., 2002; van Rossum et al., 2008). Assuming that the peripheral target is processed in a bottom-up fashion until it reaches higher-order object processing areas, the time point at which peripheral signals are available for feedback should be dictated by the temporal dynamics of visual feedforward processing.”

Concerning the interpretation of the observed time courses, and regarding the reviewer’s Major points 3 & 6, we substantially revised the Results and Discussion section. In brief, we deemphasized the claim/interpretation of faster enhancement with increasing target opacity and instead focus on describing the oscillatory pattern mentioned by the reviewer. We provide a more temporally resolved pre-saccadic time course using a moving-window analysis and discuss all suggested and further alternative explanations (i.e., saccade-locked perceptual or attentional oscillations, longer signal accumulation intervals for low-contrast information, oscillatory nature of feedback signaling). Details and full revised paragraphs are provided in the response to this reviewer’s Major points 3 & 6.

Unfortunately, there is no line numbering in the manuscript version I downloaded so I cannot refer to the specific lines of text here.

We apologize for the inconvenience and have added line numbers.

Major:

(1) The authors speculate that the phenomenon of pre-saccadic foveal prediction arises from feedback connections from higher-order visual areas, which relay relevant saccade target features to the foveal retinotopic cortex. These feedback signals are then presumably combined with feedforward foveal input to the early visual cortex and facilitate the detection of target-congruent features at the center of gaze. This interpretation is sensible, however, it may not be the only plausible scenario. The thalamus receives copies of feedforward and feedback connections between all visual areas and is a likely candidate hub for combining information across visual space. In this latter case, the phenomenon of pre-saccadic foveal prediction may not arise from feedback from higher-order visual areas, but rather from a combination of signals occurring at the level of the thalamus. The authors should either acknowledge this possibility and the fact that this phenomenon is not necessarily the result of a feedback loop, or they should explain their rationale for excluding this scenario.

We thank the reviewer for their highly thoughtful suggestion, and for alerting us to relevant literature. We have added the following paragraph to the Discussion section. In brief, we discuss the thalamic pulvinar as either an intermediate modulatory region or as the final receiver of the fed-back signal. Yet, we assume that—to solve the combinatorial issue associated with a transfer of feature information before saccades with any possible direction and amplitude—the contribution of non-retinotopic, higherorder object processing areas is likely required.

“Neural implementation of foveal prediction

Based on the body of our findings as well as previous literature, we suggested a parsimonious feedback mechanism to underly the observed effects: the preparation of a saccadic eye movement, and the concomitant shift of pre-saccadic attention (e.g., Kowler, Anderson, Dosher, & Blaser, 1995; Deubel & Schneider, 1996), selects the peripheral target stimulus among competing information. Higher-order visual areas feed selected feature input back to early retinotopic areas— specifically, to neurons with foveal receptive fields. Fed-back feature information combines with congruent, foveal feedforward input, resulting in the enhancement effects we observe. Especially in the context of active vision, this feedback mechanism is appealing as it resolves a combinatorial issue associated with feature-specific information transfer before saccades. Consider a simplified case in which, right before a saccadic eye movement, the activation of a feature-selective neuron that encodes a certain retinal location is transferred to a neuron within the same brain area that will encode said retinal location after saccade landing. For this mechanism to function for any possible saccade direction and amplitude, most neurons would need to be connected to most other neurons (or, in a simplified version, to neurons with foveal receptive fields) in a given brain area. Assuming an information transmission via feedback rather than horizontal connections significantly reduces this dimensionality: Higher-order visual areas that encode object properties (largely) detached from retinotopic or spatiotopic reference frames selectively transfer feature information to neurons with foveal receptive fields, irrespective of the vector of the upcoming saccade. This parsimonious mechanism would have shortcomings. In particular, foveal feedback should become less effective during saccade sequences where several peripheral targets are simultaneously attended. Feature information at both attended target locations may be fed back in temporal succession or weighted and erroneously combined into a single fed-back signal. In most cases, however, foveal feedback may reasonably achieve what established transsaccadic mechanisms struggle to explain: An anticipation of the features of a single saccade target—which typically constitutes the currently most relevant object in the visual field—in foveal vision.

While direct feedback connections from higher-order to early visual areas would constitute the most straightforward implementation, it is conceivable that feedback signals are relayed through and modulated by subcortical areas. In particular, the thalamic pulvinar has been identified as a connection hub for visual processing that receives copies of feedforward and feedback connections from different visual areas and may even combine information across visual space (Cortes, Ladret, Abbas-Farishta, & Casanova, 2024). In the case of foveal prediction, thalamic neurons may receive fed-back signals from higher-order areas and enhance those signals before passing them on to cortical neurons with foveal receptive fields. Perhaps, a modification of foveal activation within the thalamic pulvinar itself is sufficient to influence perception. To the best of our understanding, however, the fed-back signal must originate in non-retinotopic, higher-order object processing areas to reduce the number of necessary neuronal connections.”

(2) The results presented are very compelling. I wonder to which extent they generalize to situations in which the foveal input and the peripheral input are more heterogenous (e.g., faces or complex objects composed of many different features, orientations, and other visual properties). I think the current research raises a number of interesting questions. In general, it would be important for the readers to elaborate more on how the mechanism of pre-saccadic foveal prediction may play out in normal viewing conditions or in conditions in which the foveal input is completely irrelevant to the task.

We agree and have reiterated this point in the current manuscript (see our first reply to “Weaknesses”). We also explicitly refer to Kroell & Rolfs (2022) for an extensive initial discussion of this question.

(3) On page 10 the authors state that their data suggest that foveal enhancement emerges in earlier stages of saccade preparation as target opacity increases. However, this is not clear from the figures, when performance is locked to saccade onset (Fig 3 C), for the highest opacity targets performance seems to oscillate, however, the authors do not comment on that. There is literature showing how saccades can reset perceptual oscillations, and maybe what is observed here is just a stronger performance oscillation when the saccade target is more visible. Why would performance drop systematically 75 ms before saccade onset and then increase again 25 ms before the onset? Can the authors elaborate more on this?

In response to this comment, we inspected the pre-saccadic time course of enhancement effects in a more temporally resolved fashion and, indeed, observed pronounced oscillations for the two higher target opacity conditions (see Results):

“Especially at higher target opacities, the temporal development of foveal enhancement appears to exhibit an oscillatory pattern. To inspect this incidental observation in a more temporally resolved fashion, we determined mean enhancement values in a boxcar window of 50 ms duration sliding along all saccade-locked probe offset time points (step size = 10 ms; x-axis values in Figure 4 indicate the latest time point in a certain window). We then fitted 6th order polynomials (with no constraints on parameters) to the resulting time courses and compared the fitted values against zero using bootstrapping (see Methods). The average foveal enhancement across target opacities reached significance starting 115 ms before saccade onset (gray curve in Figure 4; all ps < .046). For every individual target opacity condition, we observed significant enhancement immediately before saccade onset, although only very briefly for the lowest opacity (-2–0 ms for 25%; -39–0 ms for 39%, -106–0 ms for 59% & -13–0 ms for 90%; all ps < .050; yellow to dark red curves in Figure 4). Especially for the higher two target opacities, we observed a local maximum preceding eye movement onset by approximately 80 ms. Interestingly, assuming a peak in enhancement in approximately 80 ms intervals (i.e., at x-axis values of -80 and 0 ms in Figure 4) would correspond to an oscillation frequency of 12.5 Hz. In contrast to rapid feedforward processing, feedback signaling is associated with neural oscillations in the alpha and beta range (i.e., between 7 and 30 Hz; Bastos et al., 2015; Jensen, Bonnefond, Marshall, & Tiesinga, 2015; van Kerkoerle et al., 2015).”

We had observed an oscillatory pattern in multiple previous investigations, and in both Hit Rates to foveal orientation content and reflexive gaze velocities in response to peripheral motion information. So far, we have been unsure how to explain it. The literature on thalamic visual processing mentioned by the reviewer alerted us to the oscillatory nature of feedback signaling itself. Interestingly, the temporal frequency range of feedback oscillations includes the frequency of ~12.5 Hz observed in our data. We have included this and alternative explanations in the Discussion section (see below). Throughout, we highlight that we are aware that our analysis approach is purely descriptive and that the potential explanations we give are speculative.

“Moreover, foveal congruency effects appear to exhibit an oscillatory pattern, with peaks in a medium saccade preparation stage (~80 ms before the eye movement) and immediately before saccade onset. We have noticed this pattern in several investigations with substantially different visual stimuli and behavioral readouts. For instance, using a full-screen dot motion paradigm, we observed a pre-saccadic, small-gain ocular following response to coherent motion in the saccade target region (Kroell, Rolfs, & Mitchell, 2023, conference abstract; Kroell, 2023, dissertation). Predictive ocular following first reached significance ~125 ms before the eye movement, then decreased and subsequently ramped up again ~25 ms before saccade onset. Several explanatory mechanisms appear conceivable. Unlike rapid feedforward processing, feedback propagation has been shown to follow an oscillatory rhythm in the alpha and beta range, that is, between 7 and 30 Hz (Bastos et al., 2015; Jensen, Bonnefond, Marshall, & Tiesinga, 2015; van Kerkoerle, et al., 2015). In our case, it is possible that the object-processing areas that send feedback to retinotopic visual cortex do so at a temporal frequency of ~12.5 Hz. At higher stimulus contrasts, feedforward signals may be fed back instantaneously and without the need for signal accumulation in feedbackgenerating areas. The resulting perceptual time courses may reflect innate temporal feedback properties most veridically. Alternatively, the initial enhancement peak may be related to the sudden onset of the saccade target stimulus and not to movement preparation itself. In this case, the initial peak should become particularly apparent if enhancement is aligned to the onset of the target stimulus. Yet, Figure 3 and Figure 4 suggest more prominent oscillations in saccade-locked time courses. In accordance with this, perceptual and attentional processes have been shown to exhibit oscillatory modulations that are phase-locked to action onset (e.g., Tomassini, Spinelli, Jacono, Sandini, & Morrone, 2015; Hogendoorn, 2016; Wutz, Muschter, van Koningsbruggen, Weisz, & Melcher, 2016; Benedetto & Morrone, 2017; Tomassini, Ambrogioni, Medendorp, & Maris, 2017; Benedetto, Morrone & Tomassini, 2019). Whether the oscillatory pattern of foveal enhancement, as well as its increased prominence at higher target contrasts, relies on innate temporal properties of feedback signaling, signal accumulation, saccade-locked oscillatory modulations of feedforward processing or attention, or a combination of these factors, one conclusion remains: task-induced cognitive influences suggested to underlie the considerable variability in temporal characteristics of foveal feedback during passive fixation (e.g., Fan et al., 2016; Weldon et al., 2016; 2020) are not the only possible explanation. Low-level target properties such as its luminance contrast modulate the resulting time course and should be equally considered, at least in our paradigm.”

In the revised Abstract, we removed our claim on an earlier emergence of enhancement at higher opacities and have added this summary instead:

“Second, the time course of foveal enhancement appeared to show an oscillatory pattern that was particularly pronounced at higher target opacities. Interestingly, the temporal frequency of these oscillations corresponded to the frequency range typically associated with neural feedback signaling.”

(4) What was the average difference in latency between short and long latencies? It would be good to report it in the main text.

We apologize for the oversight. The difference was 61 ms, with latencies of md = 247±18 ms for short- and md = 308±18 ms for long-latency saccades. We have added this information to the main text.

(5) From the saccade latency graphs in Figure S1 it seems there is some variability in the latency of saccades across subjects, I wonder if there is a correlation between saccade latency and the magnitude of the foveal prediction effect across subjects.

We had inspected a connection between saccade latency and congruency in our first investigation (Kroell & Rolfs, 2022; not reported) and observed that participants with lower latencies tended to show more enhancement, albeit non-significantly. Likewise, we observed a non-significant negative correlation between the median saccade latency and the mean foveal prediction effect (across opacities and time points) in the current investigation, r = -0.22, p = .572. While our study involved a small number of observers (n = 9), the analysis approach illustrated in Figure 2 A-C instead makes use of the large number of trials collected per participant (mean n = 2841 trials per observer) and demonstrates a reliable influence of saccade latency on an individual-observer level.

(6) Page 14, the authors state that their findings suggest that the feedforward processing of the peripheral saccade target is accelerated when it is presented at high contrast. I find this a bit too speculative, both in terms of assuming that there is a feedforward vs a feedback process (see my point 1) and in terms of speculating that the feedforward process is accelerated as I do not see a clear hint of this in the data (see my point 3) and it is a bit of a stretch to speculate on delays or accelerations of neural processing. It is possible that the feedforward signal is always delivered at the same speed but it is weaker in one case and the effect needs more time to build up.

We fully agree and hope to have addressed the reviewer’s arguments in the sections preceding this point. We included the reviewer’s last sentence in the Discussion section as well:

“Alternatively, or in addition, it is conceivable that weaker feedforward signals require a longer accumulation interval before the feedback process can be initiated.”

Minor:

(1) I think the description of the linear mixed-effects model can go in the supplemental methods, if possible, and its results can be briefly mentioned in the text.

In previous work, we have been asked to move linear mixed-effects model descriptions from supplemental to main method (or even results) sections for clarity. We have followed this suggestion ever since and, due to the relevance of the models for the interpretation of the presented results, would like to keep their description in the methods section.

(2) This is just a minor point, but I would suggest using a different word instead of opacity (maybe visibility?).

We had gone back and forth on this. We decided to use the term ‘conspicuity’ when we discuss our findings conceptually and the term ‘opacity’ when we refer to the experimental manipulation (since we directly manipulate the transparency, i.e., 1-opacity, of the target patch against the background). To compute the slopes in Figures 2 and 5, we ordered observers’ performances by the linearly spaced opacity conditions. Since the term ‘opacity’ is closest to both the experimental manipulation and the variable entered into analysis, we would like to adhere to this terminology. However, we have added an explicit note to the end of our introduction to avoid confusion:

“Throughout the paper, we use the term ‘opacity’ when we refer to the experimental manipulation (that is, a variation of the transparency, i.e., 1-opacity of the target patch against the background noise) and the term ‘conspicuity’ when we discuss our findings conceptually.”

Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

Summary:

In this manuscript, the authors ran a dual task. Subjects monitored a peripheral location for a target onset (to generate a saccade to), and they also monitored a foveal location for a foveal probe. The foveal probe could be congruent or incongruent with the orientation of the peripheral target. In this study, the authors manipulated the conspicuity of the peripheral target, and they saw changes in performance in the foveal task. However, the changes were somewhat counterintuitive.

Strengths:

The authors use solid analysis methods and careful experimental design.

Weaknesses:

I have some issues with the interpretation of the results, as explained below. In general, I feel that a lot of effects are being explained by attention and target-probe onset asynchrony etc, but this seems to be against the idea put forth by the authors of "foveal prediction for visual continuity across saccades". Why would foveal prediction be so dependent on such other processes? This needs to be better clarified and justified.

We address the described weaknesses in the respective sections below. In general, as we point out in response to Reviewer 1 as well, the current submission is a Research Advance article meant to supplement our main article (Kroell & Rolfs, 2022, https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.78106). To comply with the eLife recommendations for Research Advance submissions, we addressed conceptual points only briefly, especially if they had been explained in detail in our main article. To make the nature and format of the current submission as explicit as possible, and to emphasize its connection to our previous work, we refer to the submission format in our abstract and introduction now.

Specifics:

The explanation of decreased hit rates with increased peripheral target opacity is not convincing. The authors suggest that higher contrast stimuli in the periphery attract attention. But, then, why are the foveal results occurring earlier (as per the later descriptions in the manuscript)? And, more importantly, why would foveal prediction need to be weaker with stronger pre-saccadic attention to the periphery? What is the function of foveal prediction? What of the other interpretation that could be invoked in general for this type of task used by the authors: that the dual task is challenging and that subjects somehow misattribute what they saw in the peripheral task when planning the saccade. i.e. foveal hit rates are misperceptions of the peripheral target. When the peripheral target is easier to see, then the foveal hit rate drops.

We will address these comments one by one:

The authors suggest that higher contrast stimuli in the periphery attract attention. But, then, why are the foveal results occurring earlier (as per the later descriptions in the manuscript)?

We consider these observations to rely on separate processes. Already in the main publication (Kroell & Rolfs, 2022), we had observed a continuous decrease of target-congruent and target-incongruent foveal Hit Rates (HRs) during saccade preparation, and suggested that this decrease (similarly observed in Hanning & Deubel, 2022b is likely caused by the pre-saccadic shift of visuospatial attention to the target. In other words, as attentional resources shift towards the periphery, foveal detection performance is hampered, irrespective of peripheral and foveal feature (in-)congruency. In the current investigation, we again observed a pronounced pre-saccadic decrease of foveal HRs, irrespective of foveal probe orientation. Our argument that high-contrast peripheral saccade targets attract more attention relies on the clear observation that this decrease becomes more pronounced as the contrast of the saccade target increases. To the best of our judgment and experience with doing the task ourselves, this interpretation appears very conceivable. We explain this rationale in the Abstract and the Results sections of the manuscript (see below).

Our hypotheses and interpretations concerning the time course of foveal prediction refer to the difference between target-congruent and target-incongruent foveal HRs (i.e., to predictive foveal feature enhancement). Irrespective of the general, feature-unspecific decrease of foveal detection performances, we had hypothesized that the peripheral target is processed faster if it exhibits a high contrast. This assumption is based on temporal processing properties of many visual neurons that we have expanded on in our revision:

“In particular, neuronal response latencies decrease systematically as the contrast of visual input increases. While this phenomenon is reliably observed at varying stages of the visual processing hierarchy—such as the lateral geniculate nucleus (Lee et al., 1981b), primary visual cortex (e.g., Albrecht, 1995; Carandini et al., 1997, 2002; Carandini and Heeger, 1994), and anterior superior temporal sulcus (STSa; Oram et al., 2002; van Rossum et al., 2008)— influences of contrast on neuronal response latency are particularly pronounced in higher-order visual areas: A doubling of stimulus contrast has been shown to decrease the latency of V1 neurons by 8 ms, compared to a reduction of 33 ms in area STSa (Oram et al., 2002; van Rossum et al., 2008). Assuming that the peripheral target is processed in a bottom-up fashion until it reaches higher-order object processing areas, the time point at which peripheral signals are available for feedback should be dictated by the temporal dynamics of visual feedforward processing.”

Of note, both reviewers asked us to explore the oscillatory nature of the difference between targetcongruent and target-incongruent HRs. We will post our changes in response to the reviewer’s remark below.

And, more importantly, why would foveal prediction need to be weaker with stronger pre-saccadic attention to the periphery?

We hope that our previous reply has cleared up that the opposite is true: In general, and irrespective of the feature congruency of target and foveal probe, foveal HRs decrease as target contrast increases. As we have stated in our Abstract and Results, “foveal Hit Rates for target-congruent and incongruent probes decreased as target opacity increased, presumably since attention was increasingly drawn to the target the more salient it became. Crucially, foveal enhancement defined as the difference between congruent and incongruent Hit Rates increased with opacity”. This finding did not appear counterintuitive to us and was, in fact pre-registered as a main hypothesis (see https://osf.io/wceba).

We are unsure if this goes beyond the reviewer’s concern but we, in fact, speculate in the revised Discussion section as well as in our original eLife article that the overall, feature-unspecific decrease in foveal detection performances may aid feature-specific foveal prediction:

“This pre-saccadic decrease in foveal sensitivity may boost the relative weight of fed-back signals by attenuating the conspicuity of high-contrast feedforward input. In other words, the strength of feedforward input to the fovea is reduced gradually across saccade preparation. At the same time, the strength of the fed-back predictive signal should profit from the high contrast of naturalistic saccade targets.”

What is the function of foveal prediction?

Please refer to the section ‘What is the function of foveal prediction?’ in our main article. We have pasted this paragraph below for the reviewer’s convenience.

“What is the function of foveal prediction?

As stated above, previous investigations on foveal feedback required observers to make peripheral discrimination judgments. We, in contrast, did not ask observers to generate a perceptual judgment on the orientation of the saccade target. Instead, detecting the target was necessary to perform the oculomotor task. While the identification of local contrast changes would have sufficed to direct the eye movement, the orientation of the target enhanced foveal processing of congruent orientations. The automatic nature of foveal enhancement showcases that perceptual and oculomotor processing are tightly intertwined in active visual settings: planning an eye movement appears to prioritize the features of its target; commencing the processing of these features before the eye movement is executed may accelerate post- saccadic target identification and ultimately provide a head start for corrective gaze behavior (Deubel et al., 1982; Ohl and Kliegl, 2016; Tian et al., 2013).”

What of the other interpretation that could be invoked in general for this type of task used by the authors: that the dual task is challenging and that subjects somehow misattribute what they saw in the peripheral task when planning the saccade. i.e. foveal hit rates are misperceptions of the peripheral target. When the peripheral target is easier to see, then the foveal hit rate drops.

Alternative explanations in general: In our main article, we ruled out—either through direct experimentation or by considering relevant properties of our findings—the following alternative explanations: i) spatially global feature-based attention to the target orientation, ii) a multiplicative combination of spatial and feature-based attention, and iii) shifts of decision criterion. While dual tasks (i.e., simultaneous oculomotor planning and perceptual detection) are standard in psychophysical investigations of active vision, we acknowledge the potential influence of an explicit foveal task in the revised manuscript, and in response to both reviewers:

“Lastly, pre-saccadic foveal input is likely less relevant during natural viewing behavior than it is in our task. It is possible that this task-induced prioritization of the foveal location facilitated the emergence of congruency effects. In a previous experiment (Kroell & Rolfs, 2022; Figure 2D), the perceptual probe could appear anywhere on a horizontal axis of 9 dva length around the screen center. Despite this spatial unpredictability, however, congruency effects peaked at the pre-saccadic foveal location, even after peripheral baseline performances had been raised to a foveal level through an adaptive increase in probe opacity. Ultimately, an influence of task demands on visual processing can only be fully excluded through techniques that provide a direct readout of perceptual contents without requiring keyboard responses. In psychophysical investigations, a prediction of saccade target motion may be read out from observers’ eye velocities (Kroell, Mitchell, & Rolfs, 2023; Kwon, Rolfs, & Mitchell, 2019). In electroencephalographic (EEG) and neurophysiological studies, foveal predictions should manifest in early visual evoked potentials (e.g., Creel, 2019) and increased firing rates of feature-selective foveal neurons in early visual areas, respectively.”

Difficulty of the task: Concerning the perceptual detection task, every experimental session was preceded by an adaptive staircase procedure that adjusted the transparancy of the foveal probe—and, thus, task difficulty—depending on the respective observer’s performance (see Methods for details). Concerning the oculomotor task, observers were able to perform accurate saccades with typical movement latencies for all target opacity conditions (see Results, Supplements & Figure S1). In general, we are unsure how high task difficulty could produce a feature-, temporally and spatially specific enhancement of both filtered and incidental target-congruent foveal orientation information. In fact, a main finding of our current submission is that foveal HRs decrease as the target becomes easier to see and the oculomotor task thus becomes easier to perform.

Perceptual confusion of target and probe stimulus: We observe a specific increase in HRs for foveal probes that exhibit the same orientation as the peripheral saccade target. Just like in our main article, a response is defined as a ‘Hit’ if a foveal probe is presented and the observer generates a ‘present’ judgment. To our understanding, the suggestion that a confusion of target and probe stimuli may account for these effects necessarily implies that this confusion hinges on the congruency between peripheral and foveal feature inputs. In other words, peripheral and foveal signals should be more readily “confused” if they exhibit similar features. We assume that peripheral feature information is fed back to neurons with foveal receptive field and combines with feature-congruent feedforward input. Whether this combination of signals can be described as low-level perceptual “confusion” likely depends on individual linguistic judgments (it would certainly be a novel description of feedback-feedforward interactions). Perhaps a defining difference between the reviewer’s concern and our assumed mechanism is the spatial specificity of the resulting congruency effects. We suggest that only neurons with foveal receptive fields receive feature information via feedback. And indeed, we demonstrate a clear spatial specificity of congruency effects around the pre-saccadic foveal location, even after parafoveal performances had been raised to a foveal level by an adaptive increase in probe opacity (see Kroell & Rolfs, 2022; Figure 2C & Figure 3). In other words, observers’ perception is altered in their pre-saccadic center of gaze while the target is presented peripherally. We struggle to conceive a

scenario in which a confusion of signals should be feature-specific as well as specific to an interaction between peripheral and foveal signals without being meaningful at the same time. If the reviewer is referring to confusions on the response or decision level, we would like to point them towards the Discussion section ‘Can our findings be explained by established mechanisms other than foveal prediction?’ in our main article. In this paragraph, we provide detailed arguments for a dissociation between our findings and shifts in decision criterion that would exceed the scope of a Research Advance.

When the peripheral target is easier to see, then the foveal hit rate drops.

We agree. Target-congruent and incongruent foveal HRs decreased as the contrast of the probe increased. However, and as we stated in response to the reviewer’s first comment, the difference between target-congruent and target-incongruent foveal HRs (and, thus, foveal enhancement of the target orientation) increased with peripheral target contrast.

The analyses of Fig. 3C appear to be overly convoluted. They also imply an acknowledgment by the authors that target-probe temporal difference matters. Doesn't this already negate the idea that the foveal effects are associated with the saccade generation process itself? If the effect is related to target onset, how is it interpreted as related to a foveal prediction that is associated with the saccade itself?

We indeed conducted analyses that can reveal an influence of target presentation duration at probe onset, the saccade preparation stage at probe offset, as well as a combination of both factors. The fact that target presentation duration may have an influence on foveal prediction would not negate a simultanous influence of saccade preparation and vice versa. In the main article, we directly investigated the influence of saccade preparation on foveal enhancement by introducing a passive fixation condition (Kroell & Rolfs, 2022; Figure 5). At identical target-probe offset durations, pre-saccadic foveal enhancement was significantly more pronounced and accelerated compared to enhancement during passive fixation. We have added a purely saccade-locked time course (uncorrected by targetprobe interval) to our Results section and to Figure 3 (second row). We still believe that the target-locked, saccade-locked and combined analysis are informative for future investigations and would like to present them all for completeness.

Also, the oscillatory nature of the effect in Fig. 3C for 59% and 90% opacity is quite confusing and not addressed. The authors simply state that enhancement occurs earlier before the saccade for higher contrasts. But, this is not entirely true. The enhancement emerges then disappears and then emerges again leading up to the saccade. Why would foveal prediction do that?

In response to this comment and a suggestion by Reviewer 1, we inspected the pre-saccadic time course of enhancement effects in a more temporally resolved fashion and, indeed, observed pronounced oscillations for the two higher target opacity conditions (see Results):

“Especially at higher target opacities, the temporal development of foveal enhancement appears to exhibit an oscillatory pattern. To inspect this incidental observation in a more temporally resolved fashion, we determined mean enhancement values in a boxcar window of 50 ms duration sliding along all saccade-locked probe offset time points (step size = 10 ms; x-axis values in Figure 4 indicate the latest time point in a certain window). We then fitted 6th order polynomials to the resulting time courses and compared the fitted values against zero using bootstrapping (see Methods). The average foveal enhancement across target opacities reached significance starting 115 ms before saccade onset (gray curve in Figure 4; all ps < .046). For every individual target opacity condition, we observed significant enhancement immediately before saccade onset, although only very briefly for the lowest opacity (-2–0 ms for 25%; -39–0 ms for 39%, -106–0 ms for 59% & -13–0 ms for 90%; all ps < .050; yellow to dark red curves in Figure 4). Especially for the higher two target opacities, we observed a local maximum preceding eye movement onset by approximately 80 ms. Interestingly, assuming a peak in enhancement in approximately 80 ms intervals (i.e., at x-axis values of -80 and 0 ms in Figure 4) would correspond to an oscillation frequency of 12.5 Hz. In contrast to rapid feedforward processing, feedback signaling is associated with neural oscillations in the alpha and beta range (i.e., between 7 and 30 Hz; Bastos et al., 2015; Jensen, Bonnefond, Marshall, & Tiesinga, 2015; van Kerkoerle et al., 2015).”

We had observed an oscillatory pattern in multiple previous investigations, and in both Hit Rates to foveal orientation content and reflexive gaze velocities in response to peripheral motion information. So far, we have been unsure how to explain it. The literature on thalamic visual processing mentioned by the reviewer alerted us to the oscillatory nature of feedback signaling itself. Interestingly, the temporal frequency range of feedback oscillations includes the frequency of ~12.5 Hz observed in our data. We have included this and alternative explanations in the Discussion section (see below). We are aware, and acknowledge in the manuscript, that our analysis approach is purely descriptive, and that the potential explanations we give are speculative.

“Moreover, foveal congruency effects appeared to exhibit an oscillatory pattern, with peaks in a medium saccade preparation stage (~80 ms before the eye movement) and immediately before saccade onset. We have noticed this pattern in several investigations with substantially different visual stimuli and behavioral readouts. For instance, using a full-screen dot motion paradigm, we observed a pre-saccadic, small-gain ocular following response to coherent motion in the saccade target region (Kroell, Rolfs, & Mitchell, 2023, conference abstract; Kroell, 2023, dissertation). Predictive ocular following first reached significance ~125 ms before the eye movement, then decreased and subsequently ramped up again ~25 ms before saccade onset. Several explanatory mechanisms appear conceivable. Unlike rapid feedforward processing, feedback propagation has been shown to follow an oscillatory rhythm in the alpha and beta range, that is, between 7 and 30 Hz (Bastos et al., 2015; Jensen, Bonnefond, Marshall, & Tiesinga, 2015; van Kerkoerle, et al., 2015). In our case, it is possible that the object-processing areas that send feedback to retinotopic visual cortex do so at a temporal frequency of ~12.5 Hz. At higher stimulus contrasts, feedforward signals may be fed back instantaneously and without the need for signal accumulation in feedback-generating areas. The resulting perceptual time courses may reflect innate temporal feedback properties most veridically. Alternatively, the initial enhancement peak may be related to the sudden onset of the saccade target stimulus and not to movement preparation itself. In this case, the initial peak should become particularly apparent if enhancement is aligned to the onset of the target stimulus. Yet, Figure 3 and Figure 4 suggest more prominent oscillations in saccade-locked time courses. In accordance with this, perceptual and attention processes have been shown to exhibit oscillatory modulations that are phase-locked to action onset (e.g., Tomassini, Spinelli, Jacono, Sandini, & Morrone, 2015; Hogendoorn, 2016; Wutz, Muschter, van Koningsbruggen, Weisz, & Melcher, 2016; Benedetto & Morrone, 2017; Tomassini, Ambrogioni, Medendorp, & Maris, 2017; Benedetto, Morrone & Tomassini, 2019). Whether the oscillatory pattern of foveal enhancement, as well as its increased prominence at higher target contrasts, relies on innate temporal properties of feedback signaling, signal accumulation, saccade-locked oscillatory modulations of feedforward processing or attention, or a combination of these factors, one conclusion remains: task-induced cognitive influences suggested to underlie the considerable variability in temporal characteristics of foveal feedback during passive fixation (e.g., Fan et al., 2016; Weldon et al., 2016; 2020) are not the only possible explanation. Low-level target properties such as its luminance contrast modulate the resulting time course and should be equally considered, at least in our paradigm.”

The interpretation of Fig. 4 is also confusing. Doesn't the longer latency already account for the lapse in attention, such that visual continuity can proceed normally now that the saccade is actually eventually made? In all results, it seems that the effects are all related to the dual nature of the task and/or attention, rather than to the act of making the saccade itself. Why should visual continuity (when a saccade is actually made, whether with short or long latency) have different "fidelity"? And, isn't this disruptive to the whole idea of visual continuity in the first place?

We are unsure if we grasp the unifying concern behind these remarks. For the reviewer’s point on the dual-task nature of our paradigm, please consider our answer above. Perhaps it is important to note that we do not (and would never) claim that foveal prediction is the only mechanism underlying visual continuity. We believe that multiple mechanisms, including but not limited to pre-saccadic shifts of attention, predictive remapping of attention pointers and the perception of intra-saccadic signals interact and jointly contribute to visual continuity. It appears highly conceivable that, like most processes in biological systems, motor and perceptual performances are subject to fluctuations. We argue that saccade latencies as well as the magnitude of foveal prediction constitute read-outs of these variations. We also suggest that those read-outs are innately correlated beyond their common moderator of, perhaps, attentional state; we have previously presented clear evidence for a link between eye movement preparation and foveal prediciton (Kroell & Rolfs, 2022; Figure 2). To the best of our judgment, we consider it reasonable that the effectiveness of movement-contingent perceptual processes varies with the effectiveness (in programming or execution) of the very movement motivating them. We present evidence for this assumption in our submission. We would also like to make clear that we do not assume our vision to fail entirely, even if every single well-known mechanism of visual continuity were to break down at once. Upon saccade landing, the visual system receives reliable visual input. Nonetheless, the visual system has undeniably developed mechanisms to optimize this process. We believe foveal prediciton to rank among them.

Small question: is it just me or does the data in general seem to be too excessively smoothed?

We did not apply any smoothing to either the analysis or visualization of our data in the initial manuscript.

Every observer completed a large number of trials (mean n = 2841 trials per observer; total trial number > 25,500), which likely contributes to the clarity of our data. To inspect the oscillatory pattern of enhancement in a more temporally resolved fashion (in response to the reviewer’s point above), we applied a moving window analysis in this revision. Due to overlapping window borders, this analysis introduces a certain degree of smoothing. Nonetheless, data patterns are comparable to the time course with only few non-overlapping time bins (Figure 3B; second row). In general, we have described all steps of our analysis routine extensively in the Methods section and will make our data publicly available upon publication of the Reviewed Preprint.

General comment: it is important to include line numbers in manuscripts, to help reviewers point to specific parts of the text when writing their comments. Otherwise, the peer review process is rendered unnecessarily complicated for the reviewers.

We apologize and have added line numbers.

  1. Howard Hughes Medical Institute
  2. Wellcome Trust
  3. Max-Planck-Gesellschaft
  4. Knut and Alice Wallenberg Foundation