Abstract
Animals are adapted to their natural habitats and lifestyles. Their brains perceive the external world via their sensory systems, compute information together with that of internal states and autonomous activity, and generate appropriate behavioral outputs. However, how do these processes evolve across evolution? Here, focusing on the sense of olfaction, we have studied the evolution in olfactory sensitivity, preferences and behavioral responses to six different food-related amino acid odors in the two eco-morphs of the fish Astyanax mexicanus. To this end, we have developed a high-throughput behavioral setup and pipeline of quantitative and qualitative behavior analysis, and we have tested 489 six-week-old Astyanax larvae. The blind, dark-adapted morphs of the species showed markedly distinct basal swimming patterns and behavioral responses to odors, higher olfactory sensitivity and a strong preference for alanine, as compared to their river-dwelling eyed conspecifics. In addition, we discovered that fish have an individual “swimming personality”, and that this personality influences their capability to respond efficiently to odors and find the source. Importantly, the personality traits that favored significant responses to odors were different in surface fish and cavefish. Moreover, the responses displayed by second-generation cave x surface F2 hybrids suggested that olfactory-driven behavior and olfactory sensitivity is a quantitative genetic trait. Our findings show that olfactory processing has rapidly evolved in cavefish at several levels: detection threshold, odor preference, and foraging behavior strategy. Cavefish is therefore an outstanding model to understand the genetic, molecular and neurophysiological basis of sensory specialization in response to environmental change.
Introduction
With more than 26.000 species representing half of vertebrates, bony fishes are extremely diverse and have colonized all possible ecological niches (Helfman et al., 2009), making them outstanding models to study the neurophysiological, genetic and evolutionary underpinnings of adaptive behaviors. To colonize, survive and thrive in various environments, sensory systems are particularly crucial as they serve as windows to the external world, and they display an exceptional diversity in fishes. From the visual system of deep-sea fish shaped to catch the rare photons or the point-like bioluminescent signals to the mobile barbels covered with taste buds to probe the sea bottom and detect buried prey of bottom feeders, examples abound (de Busserolles et al., 2020; Kiyohara et al., 2002).
The anatomical sensory specializations, that is the relative importance taken by specific sensory organs or brain sensory areas, arise during embryonic development through regulatory processes that control the patterning of the neuroepithelium and that define boundaries between presumptive neural regions (Krubitzer et al., 2011). The relative investment in different sensory modules governs anatomical and behavioral specializations. Among fish, sand-dwelling cichlids that rely almost exclusively on vision to execute behaviors develop a large optic tectum, whereas rock-dwelling cichlids, which inhabit complex environments, have small optic tecta but an enlarged telencephalon, and these variations arise early in development (Sylvester et al., 2010). Similarly, in the embryos of dark-adapted blind Mexican cavefish, the presumptive eyefield territory is reduced but the presumptive olfactory epithelium is increased in size, presumably as a compensation for the loss of visual modality in the dark environment (Agnès et al., 2022; Hinaux et al., 2016a; Pottin et al., 2011; Torres-Paz et al., 2019).
While the comparative anatomy of fish brains is reasonably well documented, behavioral studies have mainly focused on a few model species, primarily zebrafish (Kalueff et al., 2013). The behavioral correlates of sensory systems diversity and evolution in fishes are poorly described and understood, which hampers the interpretation of cross-species comparisons. For example, the size and complexity of fish olfactory organs is highly variable and correlates with the richness of odorant receptors repertoire, defining a morpho-genomic space in which olfactory specialists and non-specialists species distribute (Burguera et al., 2023; Policarpo et al., 2022). On the lower end, sunfish or pipefish have a small and flat olfactory epithelium and possess ∼30 odorant receptor genes. On the higher end, polypteriforms have large and complex olfactory rosettes with up to 300 lamellae and possess ∼1300 odorant receptor genes. Yet, the differences in olfactory behaviors, sensitivity and preferences between these species are completely unknown.
To start addressing the question of the evolution of olfactory sensory-driven behaviors in an amenable laboratory fish model, we used the blind cave and the river dwelling morphs of the Mexican tetra, the characiform Astyanax mexicanus. The species has become an established model for evolutionary biology at large, including evolution of behaviors (Duboué and Keene, 2016; Hinaux et al., 2016b; Kowalko, 2020; Yoshizawa, 2016). Cavefish embryos and larvae have larger olfactory pits than surface fish and their neuronal composition is changed (Blin et al., 2018; Hinaux et al., 2016a), while adults of the two forms have a similar olfactory rosette with 20-25 lamellae (Schemmel, 1967). The recent divergence between the two forms probably did not allow for a substantial evolution of their olfactory receptor gene repertoire, which is almost identical in the two eco-morphotypes (245 genes in cavefish, 233 genes in surface fish, from recent genome assemblies) (Policarpo et al., 2022). Yet, cavefish larvae tested in groups display outstanding olfactory detection capacities, as they are attracted to extremely low concentration of the amino acid alanine (10-10M) while surface fish larvae have a more “classical” threshold (10-5M) (Hinaux et al., 2016a) which approximates levels of free amino acids found in natural waters (Hara, 1994). In wild natural caves too, groups of cavefish adults respond to low concentrations of odorants (Blin et al., 2020). We hypothesized that the evolution of olfactory system development and olfactory skills in blind cavefish is an adaptive trait that, together with other constructive sensory changes in mechano-sensory and gustatory systems (Varatharasan et al., 2009; Yamamoto et al., 2009; Yoshizawa and Jeffery, 2011; Yoshizawa et al., 2014), may have contributed to their adaptation and survival in the extreme environment of their subterranean habitat. Focusing on olfaction, here we developed a sensitive, high-throughput behavioral assay and a pipeline for analysis to compare the types of behavioral responses elicited by food-related, amino acid odors in cavefish and surface fish, at population and individual levels. We determined their olfactory preferences and sensitivity thresholds for six amino acids, and we analyzed their behavioral responses in detail. We also described olfactory-driven behaviors in second-generation (F2) hybrids resulting from crosses between surface and cave morphs, as an attempt to understand the genetic component of the “olfaction trait” in Astyanax morphs. We discovered that the behaviors triggered by odorant stimulation has markedly evolved in cavefish.
Results
A high-throughput, sensitive behavioral assay to compare individual’s behaviors in Astyanax morphotypes
Our goal was to compare 1) olfactory discrimination capacities, 2) odor preferences and 3) behavioral responses to olfactory stimuli in Pachón cavefish (CF), surface fish (SF) and F2 hybrids (F2) individuals. As the three types of fish are markedly different in terms of basal swimming activity and patterns, we first sought to characterize the diversity of these behaviors in order to be able to interpret accurately fish responses to odors.
Six weeks old fish were habituated either 1h or 24h in their individual test box and they were first recorded for 30 minutes in the dark, without any stimulus (Figure 1; see Methods). We observed and categorized several typical and distinctive “baseline” swimming behaviors for individual fish (Figure 2A): random/haphazard swim (R), wall following (WF; defined as the fish continuously following the 4 sides of the box and turning around it, in a clockwise or counterclockwise fashion), large or small circles (C), thigmotactism (T, along the X- or the Y- axis of the box; defined as the fish swimming back and forth along one of the 4 sides of the box), or combinations thereof. The distribution of these different types of swimming patterns was significantly different in Pachón CF, SF and F2 types of fish (Figure 2B; Fisher’s exact test; see also Methods and Suppl. Fig8A for another representation using Correspondence Analysis). A majority of SF swam in a random pattern (blue shades), while a majority of CF performed wall following (red/brown shades) and F2 fish showed more diversified swimming patterns. Importantly, the distribution of these baseline-swimming patterns was affected as a function of the habituation time, in all three types of fish (Figure 2B; p=0.044 for SF, p=0.0005 for CF and p=0.0005 for F2, Fisher’s exact tests). Moreover, thigmotactism behavior, which we have previously shown to represent stress behavior (Pierre et al., 2020), was frequent in SF after 1h habituation (70% of individuals) but reduced after 24h acclimation (37%). In the same line, the swimming speed was different for all fish types when computed after 1h or after 24h of habituation (Figure 2C). This suggested that natural, unperturbed behavior is better observed after long, 24h habituation, which we applied thereafter.
As described above at population level, cavefish, surface fish and F2 displayed overall markedly different basal swimming patterns (Figure 2B; see also Methods and Suppl. Fig8A for another representation using Correspondence Analysis). Yet, at individual level, there was a substantial degree of variability across individuals within a morphotype. That is, even though most SF tended to swim randomly (75% after 24h habituation; blue shades on graphs) and most CF performed wall following (50%; red/brown shades), these behaviors were not exclusive as some SF individuals displayed thigmotactism behavior or some CF individuals swam in circles. Moreover and importantly, the type of swim pattern of each individual fish was remarkably stable and reproducible along several days of recordings, and swimming kinetics (position in the box, swimming speed, number of round trips in X and Y) showed no or very little variation (Figure 2D and Suppl. Fig1AA’). Together, these observations strengthened the importance of 1) using long habituation times, and 2) studying individual behaviors. They further highlighted an overlooked aspect of fish behavioral analyses: fish may have a “personality” that we sought to take into account when comparing behavioral responses to odors below.
Finally, Pachón CF, SF and F2 hybrids possess markedly different sensory apparatuses and capacities, not only for chemo- but also for mechano-sensation (Lunsford et al., 2022; Yoshizawa et al., 2010). Therefore, to test behavioral responses specific to olfactory stimuli, we wanted to find a way to deliver odors that would be vibration-less. We choose to inject odorant solutions inside tubes attached at the two extremities of the test box, designed to stop the wave and flow that would otherwise arise at the surface of the water upon injection (Figure 1). The efficacy of the device was demonstrated by the lack of perturbation in swimming behavior, neither qualitative (i.,e., behavioral response and swimming pattern; Figure 3AB) nor quantitative (i.,e., swimming kinetics; Figure 3C) for the 3 types of fish when water/control injections were performed on one side of the test box (or two sides; Suppl. Fig1BC). This suggested that our way to deliver odorant solutions did not generate vibratory perturbations. Moreover, when no injection was made, individual fish behaviors were stable over one hour of test (Suppl. Fig1D), allowing to observe accurately the effects of an olfactory stimulus if applied after 30 minutes. Finally, all recordings were performed in the dark under infra-red lights to neutralize the visual modality in sighted fish, a procedure which did not affect SF behavior (Suppl. Fig1E). Hence, the behavioral responses to odors recorded thereafter in Pachón CF, SF and F2 are driven by olfaction and can be compared (see also Hinaux et al., 2016a).
Behavioral responses to alanine in Astyanax morphotypes
Amino acids are food-related odorant cues for fish. Next, we characterized individual behavioral responses elicited by different concentrations of alanine, a potent aliphatic amino acid cue for most fish species including Astyanax.
Injection of alanine 10-2M, 10-3M, 10-4M or 10-5M (hence 10-4M, 10-5M, 10-6M or 10-7M final concentration in the odorant third of the testing box, respectively) induced a strong behavioral response in cavefish (Figure 4A, 10-4M injection shown; Suppl. Fig2A for other examples). Upon stimulus, CF decreased the numbers of back-and-forth swims along the X- and the Y-axes of the box (Figure 4A, top left graph and Figure 4FG), and they shifted and restricted their swimming activity towards the odorant side of the arena, close to the odor delivery tube (Figure 4A, bottom left graph, and Figure 4E). A tendency to decreasing swimming speed was observed but was significant only for 10-3M injections (Figure 4D). Alanine injection also markedly changed CF swimming patterns, as they completely switched from WF and T to R and C swimming modes (Figure 4H and Suppl. Fig2D; Fisher’s exact tests p values: 0.014, 0.0005, 0.0005 and 0.099 for 10-2M, 10-3M, 10-4M or 10-5M alanine injections, respectively; see also Methods and Suppl. Fig8B for another representation using Correspondence Analysis). In sum, CF individuals displayed robust behavioral responses and attraction to alanine. Of note, further decreasing the concentration of injected alanine to 10-6M, 10-7M, 10-8, 10-9M and 10-10M had milder to no effects on cavefish. Swimming patterns remained globally unchanged (not shown), but swimming speed was decreased (p= 0.005 for 10-7M; n=16), numbers of round trips in Y were decreased (p= 0.012 for 10-7M; n=16), and numbers of round trips in X were decreased (p= 0.017 for 10-7M; p= 0.029 for 10-8M; p= 0.003 for 10-9M; n=16 each; data not shown). This suggested that in the present experimental setup, cavefish can detect very low concentrations of alanine, in agreement with previously published data (Hinaux et al., 2016a).
Conversely, surface fish responses were more subtle and seemed restricted to some individuals (Figure 4B; Suppl. Fig2B for other examples). Notably, the numbers of back-and-forth swims along the X- and the Y-axes of the box were mostly unchanged and the position in the box did not vary for SF upon alanine injection (Figure 4B, top and bottom left graphs and Figure 4E- G). The swimming speed was unchanged (Figure 4D). Swimming patterns were globally unaffected upon alanine injection (Figure 4H and Suppl. Fig2D; Fisher’s exact tests p values: 0.16, 0.54 and 0.80 for 10-2M, 10-3M or 10-4M alanine injections, respectively; Suppl. Fig8B for another representation using Correspondence Analysis). As shown on Suppl. Fig2B, some rare SF individuals appeared to change their swimming mode upon alanine 10-2M (high concentration) injection, suggesting that they did perceive and react to the odorant stimulus. However, these rare individual responses were “diluted” at population level by the pooling of all fish in the distribution graphs. In summary, SF behavioral responses were modest and markedly different from CF.
Finally, F2 were tested with alanine 10-3M, a concentration that elicits strong responses in CF but not in SF. Upon stimulus, F2’s change in behaviors were similar to CF (Figure 4C; Suppl. Fig2C for other examples): they decreased back-and-forth swimming activity, decreased swimming speed, and swam close to the odor source (Figure 4D-G). They also shifted their swimming patterns (Suppl. Fig2D; Fisher’s exact test p=0.0065), including a loss of wall following mode that was reminiscent of the trend observed in CF. As an illustration, the individual shown in Figure 4C displays a striking change from a “large circles” to a “random” swimming pattern accompanied with variations in swimming kinematics.
Overall, these data suggested that, compared to SF, CF detection threshold and behavioral responses to the amino acid alanine have significantly evolved.
Behavioral responses to serine and cysteine in Astyanax morphotypes
We next systematically tested behavioral responses to injections of 10-2M, 10-3M or 10-4M of serine (polar amino acid, hydroxyl group) and cysteine (non-polar, sulfur containing), two other potent amino acid olfactory cues for fish (Figure 5 and Suppl. Fig3).
Serine elicited behavioral responses similar to alanine: CF (as well as F2 hybrids, Suppl. Fig3) moved towards the odor source, whereas SF did not (Figure 5AB and D). Swimming speed was unchanged in all fish types (Figure 5C), but a decrease or an increase of back-and-forth swimming activity was observed in CF and SF, respectively (Figure 5EF). At population level, significant changes in swimming patterns were observed for the three fish types upon 10-2M serine (Figure 5G; Fisher’s exact p-values 0.0005, 0.001 and 0.0085 for SF, CF and F2, respectively; Suppl. Fig8B for Correspondence Analysis). Of note, and contrarily to alanine, the lower concentrations of serine tested (10-4M and 10-3M in syringe; 10-6M and 10-5M in box, respectively) were unable to trigger a robust behavioral change in cavefish (not shown, see Table1), suggesting that their detection threshold for serine is higher (not as good as) than for alanine. Regarding SF, responses were also observed only for 10-2M serine injections.
Cysteine also produced significant effects (Figure 5H-N and Suppl. Fig3). Swimming speed and numbers of back-and-forth swims along the Y-axis increased only in SF and for the 3 concentrations tested (Figure 5J and 5M for 10-2M) (for 10-3M: p= 0.01 and p=2e-05, n=32; for 10-4M: p=2e-05 and p= 0.00028, n=32). CF were attracted to the odorant side for the 3 concentrations tested (p=0.013 to 0.025; n=16 to 38 each), whereas SF did not change position (Figure 5K) except for cysteine 10-3M to which they seemed to be repelled (p=0.0003, n=32). The three fish types changed significantly their swimming patterns (Figure 5N; Fisher’s exact p-values 0.03, 0.017 and 0.04 for SF, CF and F2, respectively; Suppl. Fig8B for Correspondence Analysis). After injection of cysteine at the lower concentrations of 10-3M or 10-4M, CF behavioral responses were similar to those observed for alanine and serine, i.e., a decrease in back-and-forth swimming activity along the X-axis and a significant change in position towards the odor source, without changing swimming speed (not shown, see Table1). In sum, the different fish types show diverse responses to different concentrations of different amino acids.
Behavioral responses to lysine, histidine and leucine in Astyanax morphotypes
Finally, we choose to examine responses triggered by high concentrations (10-2M) of three other, less studied amino acids: lysine, histidine (both polar and positively charged) and leucine (aliphatic, like alanine) (Figure 6 and Suppl. Fig4).
For these three odors, CF responses were conspicuous and could include a shift of swim position towards the odor’s source, a decrease of back-and-forth swimming activity with decrease in swimming speed, and significant changes in swim patterns at population level (except for leucine) (Figure 6A-G and Suppl. Fig4; Suppl. Fig8B for Correspondence Analysis). Regarding SF, changes were restricted to increases in back and forth swimming activity for histidine, without change in swimming speed or position in the box (Figure 6A-D and Suppl. Fig4). These three amino acids did not elicit changes in swimming pattern in SF at population level (Figure 6E-G). Finally, F2 also showed specific qualitative (swimming patterns) and quantitative responses to each of these three amino acids. Together, these data suggested that CF, SF and F2 detected and responded in their own and specific way to high concentrations of lysine, histidine and leucine.
Table 1A provides a summary of the behavioral responses, averaged at population level, for the three fish types and the six amino acids tested. In the cases when significant behavioral responses were observed at population level, i.e., when responses are strongest and most stereotyped, different and opposed features emerged between CF and SF (and F2). As a “rule”, CF responses included a decrease in swimming activity (decrease in speed and back and forth swims along X- and Y-axes) together with an attraction to odor source and a change in swim pattern (Table 1A, red arrows). Conversely, SF responses mostly corresponded to an increase in swimming activity, with little or no attraction to odor and moderate change in swim pattern (Table 1A, blue arrows). In sum, a given sensory stimulus could trigger markedly different, morph-specific behavioral responses.
Behavioral responses to odors at individual level
Analyses as above performed at population level may mask or blur effects or phenotypes in the case when not all individuals respond in a stereotyped manner. As fish did express individual behavioral features in our experimental paradigm, we sought to perform further analyses at individual level, and to calculate individual response scores to the different odors. To do so, and to take into account the different components of the behavioral response, we summed the absolute values of indexes of speed, back and forth trips in X and Y, position and pattern changes (see Methods; Figure 7A). The threshold of score for which an individual fish was considered to respond significantly to a given stimulus was set at 1.5, from examination of the individual scores of fish in control conditions after perfusion of water (Suppl. Fig5). Visual inspection of the responses to amino acid odors for fish who had an individual score just above or just below this threshold confirmed that it was accurate.
For alanine 10-3M, the representation of individual behavioral responses shows that CF responded in a very stereotyped manner (all lines following the same “curve”) whereas SF responses were more diverse (lines cross) (Figure 7A, 1st column). Moreover, 73% of CF but only 19% of SF had an individual score above threshold - a marked difference that was visible on the distribution of scores (Figure 7AB). F2 fish on the other end had a bimodal distribution of their olfactory scores for alanine 10-3M (Figure 7B).
For other odors, the difference in olfactory scores of individual CF and SF was less obvious (Figure 7B and Suppl. Fig5), suggesting that taking into consideration individual’s variations of swimming behavior can unmask responses that are not visible when averaged at population level. For example, in response to cysteine 10-2M, SF (51% with score >1.5) and F2 (55% with score >1.5) had similar or slightly better scores than CF (39% with score >1.5) (Figure 7AB, 3rd column). Of note, these results had not shown up in Table 1A summarizing responses averaged at population level. Noteworthy, SF had mixed, diverse responses to all odors studied (lines crossing on all graphs). Conversely, CF showed non-homogenous responses to serine, histidine, leucine and lysine but highly stereotyped responses to both alanine and histidine (Figure 7A and Suppl. Fig5). A summary of individual response scores to the different odors is given in Table 1B.
Individual personality and individual behavioral response
The results presented above led us to test whether the “personality” of each fish, represented by their specific baseline swimming behavior, could influence their ability to respond to odors. To this end, we plotted individual olfactory scores as a function of individual baseline swimming patterns (R, WF, TX, TY, or C). We also examined possible correlations between basal swimming speed or round trip activity and individual olfactory score.
In SF, individual olfactory scores were similar whatever the basal swimming pattern of the fish (Figure 8A). However, for several odors, those fish exhibiting lower baseline swimming speed and less round trips in X had better olfactory scores (Figure 8B and Suppl. Fig6A; negative correlation). By contrast, in CF, fish exhibiting WF or C as baseline pattern had significantly better individual scores and those swimming randomly (R) had poor scores (Figure 8A) – but scores were not correlated whatsoever to baseline swimming speed (Figure 8B). Moreover, for alanine 10-3M and histidine 10-2M, the two odors for which more than 70% of CF showed a significant response (Figure 7A), a high number of round trips in X in baseline behavior was also associated to good olfactory scores (in agreement with WF being associated to significant individual response)(Suppl. Fig6A). Finally, F2 that exhibited a WF baseline pattern had better scores than others (like CF) (Figure 8A), and swimming speed was poorly correlated to their olfactory performances (not shown). Therefore, swimming patterns of CF and swimming speed of SF seem correlated to their behavioral response scores.
To further assess whether fish baseline swimming parameters are predictive of the behavioral output when presented with an odor, we next calculated how much of the odor response score variation can be explained by “personality”. We found that the PVE (Percentage of Variation Explained) on the individual olfactory score varies significantly with the baseline swimming pattern in CF (but not in SF) (Figure 8C) and with the swimming speed in SF (but not in CF) (Figure 8D). Altogether, these analyses strongly suggest that (1) the fish swimming personality has an influence on its response to olfactory stimulation and can be predictive of the response, and (2) the personality parameter that is important to predict a good individual response is not the same in the two Astyanax morphs: in SF speed matters, while in CF swimming pattern matters.
Discussion
We have developed a high throughput, specific olfaction test, together with a pipeline of analysis allowing assessing qualitatively and quantitatively individual’s and population’s behaviors, in order to describe and compare responses of blind and sighted Astyanax to odorant cues. We discovered that cavefish, surface fish and F2 hybrids display different odor preferences and sensitivities and show individual, distinctive, diverse and specific responses to varying concentrations of the six amino acids tested. Using this novel setup where fish were tested in solo in a rectangular box and during one hour (as opposed to testing in groups in a U- shaped box and during 8 minutes in previous studies (Blin et al., 2020; Blin et al., 2018; Hinaux et al., 2016a), we established that cavefish are bona fide “alanine specialists” and we analyzed in depth their behavioral responses.
A setup to probe olfaction in fish with markedly different sensory apparatuses and internal states
There is an inherent difficulty to compare sensory-driven behaviors in cave and surface morphs of Astyanax: all their sensory systems have evolved in one way or another. Cavefish have no eyes, but they possess enhanced mechanosensory lateral line and chemosensory gustatory and olfactory organs. To decipher behavioral responses driven by a single of these senses, one needs to control carefully the potential influence of the other sensory modalities. Here, we have recorded unimodal behavioral responses driven exclusively by olfaction by performing experiments in the dark to abrogate vision in sighted fish, and by designing a setup where the delivery of the olfactory stimulus is vibration-free. Moreover, we know from previous studies that gustatory taste buds do not participate in responses to amino acids at the concentrations used because the lesion of the olfactory epithelium abolishes the attraction to high concentrations of alanine, in both SF and CF (Hinaux et al., 2016a).
Due to a mutation in their Monoamine Oxidase enzyme that interferes with the metabolism of brain monoamines (Elipot et al., 2014), Pachón cavefish have lower basal cortisol levels, hence lower basal anxiety than surface fish, when they are long habituated in their home tank (Pierre et al., 2020). However, the mutation confers cavefish with a much higher stressability after environmental change, such as the transfer in a novel tank (Pierre et al., 2020). Consequently, in order to record relevant behavioral responses to olfactory stimuli in unstressed fish, we used long acclimation times (72 hours) and we tested long habituation times, either 1 hour or 24 hours – as compared to 10 minutes as usually done in most fish studies including zebrafish or Astyanax. We concluded that 1 hour is too short for proper habituation, as the swimming speed and patterns were affected for both SF, CF and F2 as compared to 24 hours. Importantly, such long, 24 hours habituation periods are susceptible to reveal unperturbed and even novel behaviors: recently applied in a study of social behaviors in cavefish, 3 days of habituation allowed the analysis of behaviors in a familiar environment and could unmask social interactions in the so-called ‘asocial’ cavefish (Iwashita and Yoshizawa, 2021).
Neurophysiological and molecular considerations
In all vertebrates including fish, odorant molecules are recognized by olfactory receptors expressed at the surface of olfactory sensory neurons, which project onto the olfactory glomeruli in the olfactory bulb with the one receptor : one glomerulus rule (Axel, 1995; Braubach et al., 2012; Buck, 2000; Kermen et al., 2013; Koide et al., 2009; Li et al., 2005; Yoshihara, 2008). Moreover, parallel neural pathways in the olfactory circuitry process different types of odorants. In zebrafish, the perception of amino acids is mediated via OlfC/V2R receptors on microvillous sensory neurons that innervate lateral and ventro-medial glomeruli in the bulb. From the periphery to the brain, Astyanax surface and cave morphs display some variations in this amino acid signal processing circuitry. SF have 43 and CF have 41 V2R/OlfC receptor genes in their genomes (Policarpo et al., 2022), a minor difference that is unlikely to underlie their differences in olfactory capacities and preferences. CF larvae have higher proportions of microvillous neurons than SF, which together with the larger size of their olfactory epithelium and olfactory bulbs may influence their olfactory sensibility (Blin et al., 2018). Astyanax glomerular organization is unknown.
Interestingly, here we have found that CF are strongly attracted and respond to alanine and histidine, two amino acids which, albeit probably not recognized by the same receptor(s), are processed in the same or very close glomeruli in zebrafish larval olfactory bulbs (Li et al., 2005). SF on the other hand seem to show a preference for cysteine (sulfur), the amino acid that is the most potent to evoke electrical responses in the olfactory bulbs of sea breams (Hubbard et al., 2011) or hammerhead sharks (Tricas et al., 2009), and that elicits a strong aversive behavioral response in larval zebrafish (Vitebsky et al., 2005). Therefore, odor preferences have evolved between cavefish and surface fish, as well as between zebrafish and Astyanax. In the same line, previously we had reported that chondroitin is a strong attractant for Astyanax (Blin et al., 2020), whereas it induces freezing and fear behavior in zebrafish (Mathuru et al., 2012). Such significant variations in odor preferences or value may be adaptive and relate to the differences in the environmental and ecological conditions in which these different animals live. Of note, we have not found an odor that would be repulsive for Astyanax so far, and this may relate to their opportunist, omnivorous and detritivore regime (Espinasa et al., 2017; Marandel et al., 2020). However, the reason why Pachón cavefish have become “alanine specialists” remains a mystery and prompts analysis of the chemical ecology of their natural habitat. Alternatively, specialization for alanine may not need to be specific for an olfactory cue present only, or frequently, or in high amounts in caves. Bat guano for example, which is probably the main source of food in the Pachón cave, must contain and release many amino acids. Enhanced recognition of only one of them - in the present case alanine but evolution may have randomly acted for enhanced recognition of another amino acid – should suffice to confer cavefish with augmented sensitivity to their main source of nutriment.
Cavefish have also evolved regarding olfactory sensitivity. They are able to detect very low concentrations of alanine (aliphatic), hence they have a lower detection threshold than SF. Injection of low concentration alanine 10-4M/10-5M (thus 10-6M/10-7M in the odorant third of the arena) elicits strong behavioral responses in CF, whereas even higher concentrations of 10- 3M and 10-2M evoke modest responses in SF (zero response at population level, 19% responders at individual level with alanine 10-3M). We have performed dose-response experiments for 3 out of the 6 amino acids tested and for both morphs. With these, CF also appear capable of detecting low concentrations of cysteine (sulfur). Moreover, they seem to detect better histidine 10-2M (polar) as well (69.2% of CF responders, versus 42.1% of SF), and CF but not SF detected lysine (polar) at the relatively high concentration of 10-2M. From these observations, we can predict that the difference between SF and CF probably does not lie in the molecular evolution of their olfactory receptor repertoire, because it is unlikely that all the receptors recognizing diverse types of amino acids have evolved all at once. Rather, we can hypothesize that evolution occurred at the level of the general regulation of odorant receptor genes expression, or at the level of olfactory processing and computation in the bulbs (Friedrich et al., 2009), or in higher order brain regions.
A variety of behavioral responses to olfactory stimulation
Our study suggests that behavioral responses to a unimodal olfactory sensory stimulus are complex and have evolved in cavefish (summary on Table 1). When they detect the odor stimulus, cavefish globally decrease their scanning activity, which might help them to compute and swim up towards the higher values of concentration in the odor plume. This is associated to a change in swimming pattern whereby WF and T are eliminated at the expense of R and C, presumably to facilitate searching. This hypothesis is further supported by their systematic change of position in the box, meaning that they locate efficiently the odor source. Strikingly, surface fish display opposite behavioral responses after odor detection: they rather increase locomotor activity, which corresponds to intense foraging but does not seem optimal to find the odor source, which is confirmed by their lack of change in position of the box (i.e., they may not locate efficiently the odor source). This is also consistent with the fact that at individual level the best SF responders are slow swimmers. Such poor ability to find the odor source may result from testing in the dark. Indeed, SF behaviors are mostly visually driven (but see Simon et al., 2019) and they might need multimodal visual + olfactory integration to find food efficiently. These interpretations are consistent with early work showing that, when competing in the dark and with limited amounts of food, SF starve and CF thrive (Hüppop, 1987). In sum, CF foraging strategy has evolved in response to the serious challenge of finding food in the dark. Future experiments including functional imaging of brain activity in live animals may reveal the changes in olfactory-driven motor circuits that allowed the evolution of behavioral outputs in cavefish.
Finally, at population level, F2 fish shared some behavioral response traits with both parental morphs, and they were often closer to CF. At individual level their responses were also variable and non-stereotyped (including for alanine 10-3M and histidine 10-2M, the conditions for which CF showed highly stereotyped responses). Olfactory scores behave as a quantitative trait. The tools we have developed here will allow the future determination of the genetic underpinnings of the evolution of olfactory-driven cavefish behaviors and capabilities.
A “personality” for each fish?
Our recordings of several hundred (n=489 total) well-habituated, individual Astyanax larvae highlight an often overlooked aspect of fish behavioral analyses: fish may have a “personality” or “temperament”, which we characterized at the level of their baseline swimming patterns. This hypothesis is strongly supported by the consistency over time, one day after the other, of the favorite swimming pattern or combination of patterns and of the swimming kinetics expressed by individuals when freely swimming without stimulus (most fish were recorded on 4 days distributed over 2 weeks). To our knowledge, this is the first time individual temperament is taken into consideration in Astyanax behavioral studies.
In the context of ecology and evolution, the personality in non-human animals was proposed to be organized along five primary axes: sociability, boldness, aggressiveness, exploration and activity (Réale et al., 2007). In fish, studies applying an animal personality approach have focused to resolve variations in physiological and molecular parameters, suggesting a link between phenotype and genotype, or between behavior and transcriptome regulation eg (Rey et al., 2021). Here, we propose that the different temperaments expressed by individual larvae/juveniles could correspond to the genetic diversity present in the natural populations of Astyanax, a diversity that we have maintained intentionally along generations in captivity in our fish facility.
Further, we have assessed whether swimming temperaments could influence the way and the extent to which fish respond to an olfactory sensory stimulus. Strikingly, we discovered that baseline swimming patterns and swimming speed do influence fish olfactory responses. In addition, important personality traits that confer positive behavioral responses are not the same in SF and CF. For cavefish, WF and round trips in X (but not speed) are the key parameters. Wall following behavior has been reported in Astyanax and other blind cavefish species (Chen et al., 2022; Patton et al., 2010; Sharma et al., 2009) and was hypothesized to confer foraging advantages, although this had never been directly tested. Here, it seems to be the case for 6- week old juveniles in a rectangular box - but the link may be more elusive when considering a fish swimming in a natural, complex environment. We propose that WF and round trips in X are together the expression of an intense exploratory behavior, which allows CF to better cover their swimming space and thus have a higher probability to detect odorant cues - even at very low concentrations because their detection capacities for some amino acids are excellent. By contrast, a slow swimming speed (but not swimming pattern) is the most critical personality parameter for SF. As SF olfactory concentration detection thresholds are higher than CF, one could imagine that they need more integration time inside the odorant plume to trigger a neuronal and a behavioral response. In any case and importantly, personality parameters that matter in CF and SF to confer good olfactory response scores are distinct. This suggests that the modulation of neuronal circuits underlying both the control of baseline behaviors and olfactory processing has evolved in cavefish.
Conclusion
Previous studies had shown that, when tested in groups, cavefish larvae as well as adults smell better than their surface fish conspecifics (Blin et al., 2020; Blin et al., 2018; Hinaux et al., 2016a). Here, we have established that it is also true for fish in solo, ruling out the possibility that when in group, one “good smeller” individual could drive others to respond through (unknown) communication mode. Using individual behavioral tests, we have discovered that both olfactory sensitivity, olfactory preferences, olfactory behavioral responses and key personality parameters have evolved in cavefish, conferring them with outstanding skills to forage in the darkness of caves.
Methods
Fish samples
Laboratory stocks of A. mexicanus surface fish (SF, origin: San Salomon spring, Texas, USA) and cavefish (CF, Pachón population) were obtained in 2004 from the Jeffery laboratory at the University of Maryland, College Park, MD, USA. Fish were maintained at 22°C with a 12h:12h light:dark cycle, fed twice daily with dry and live food. SF and CF brood stock were induced to spawn once every two weeks by thermal shock at 26°C. A F1 hybrid family was generated by crossing a SF female with a CF Pachón male. Second-generation hybrids (F2) were generated by crossing two F1 individuals. Embryos and larvae were reared at 24°C in water produced by our animal house’s water production system. All fish used (CF, SF and F2) were exactly six weeks old on the first day of the experiment. SR’s authorization for use of Astyanax mexicanus in research is 91-116. Animals were treated according to the French and European regulations for animal testing in research (authorization n°APAFIS#8604).
Experimental set-up
The test consisted of filming individual larvae for 30 minutes before olfactory stimulation and for a further 30 minutes afterwards (see Supplemental movies). The behavior laboratory is a soundproofed room maintained at a constant temperature of 24° C. To ensure that eyed fish (SFs and F2 with eyes) could not use the visual modality during the test (unlike CFs and some F2), all experiments (except one) were carried out in the dark. The experimental device was composed of 2 infrared (IR) tables (60x60cm each) over which 32 test boxes were placed. A custom-built wooden holder supported 8 IR cameras (DIGITNOW!), located 40 cm above the boxes. Each camera filmed 4 boxes simultaneously (Figure 1).
The individual test boxes (11.5x8.5x4.3 cm, multiroir #45103BOILAB03) were made of crystal-clear plastic. Odorant solutions (and/or water) were manually injected at the two opposite sides of the box using 1ml syringes. In order to stop the dispersion wave of injection, and to assure that we always injected at the same right place, we added 2 injection guides in each box: we glued two plastic tubes (4 cm long and 1 cm in diameter) at the center of the two opposite short sides (Y), 0.2 cm from the bottom. In addition, a net (6.5x6.5 cm) was attached around each tube and reached the bottom of the box, to prevent fish from passing under the tube during the experiment, thus escaping from the subsequent tracking (Figure 1).
Behavior tests
Larvae were placed individually in a box containing 150 ml of water (clean rearing water), fed with micro-worms (three drops of concentrated Panagrellus redivivus solution placed in the center of the box) and subjected to a 12:12h day:night cycle for 72 hours acclimation. Then, the water in each box was changed, the boxes were placed on IR tables for 24 hours, and the larvae were fasted (=habituation). The day of testing, for reasons of reproducibility, all experiments started at 11 a.m. The light was switched off, the cameras were switched on and the experimenter left the room for a 30-minute baseline behavior recording period. At t=30 min, the experimenter entered the room and switched on the inactinic red lamps. Solutions were manually injected bilaterally (500 µl of odor/ 500 µl of water) into the injection guides of each box. The total time needed to perform injections of 32 boxes was 5 minutes. Then, the experimenter switched off the inactinic lamps and left the room for a 30-minute response behavior recording period. At the end of recording, the water was changed and each box was placed back exactly at the same location on IR table for 24 hours and not moved again until the following day of testing (the larvae were not fed). For the series of experiments with a habituation period of 1 hour, the water in the boxes was changed every morning, 1 hour before the start of the test. The typical timeline was as follows: [72 h acclimation and feeding], Week1 [habituation 24h, test day 1, habituation, test day 2, rest and feeding for three days], Week2 [habituation, test day 3, habituation, test day 4]. As the larvae were tested on several consecutive days, to ensure that the fish were not learning/associating one side with a scent cue, the odor injection side was reversed each day. The control experiments (unilateral or bilateral injection of 500 µl rearing water or no injection) were always carried out the day1 when the larvae were naive. For the “light vs dark” control experiment, SF were filmed 30 minutes in normal light and 30 minutes in the dark.
Odorant solutions
The odorant solutions were prepared by dilution of amino acids in clean rearing water (L- Alanine #A7627; L-Serine #S4500; L-Lysine #L5501; L-Histidine #H8000; L-Leucine #L8000 and L-Cysteine #168149; all from Sigma-Aldrich). A volume of 500µl (either water or odor) was injected with a 1ml syringe (Sigma-Aldrich #Z683531) fitted with a needle (0.8x50mm, 21G; B Braun #4665503). Depending of the test, concentration of solutions injected were 10-2 M or 10-3 M or 10-4 M. In a total water volume of 150ml, with a box virtually separated into three, the volume of water in the odor injection zone is 50 ml. By injecting 500 µl of solution into this zone, we expected to obtain a dilution factor of 100. For example, for a concentration of [10-3 M] in the syringe, the concentration in the odor injection zone will therefore be ∼[10-5 M]. All concentrations given in the article indicate the concentration inside the syringe.
When consecutive injections of an odor or different odors at different concentrations were tested on the same fish, they were performed from the least concentrated to the most concentrated.
Video editing and tracking
Videos were saved on SD card in AVI format (1208x720 px, 30 fps), then edited using Adobe Premiere Pro V14.0 to be calibrated at 1 hour long without the 5 minute-injection time. They were then exported into MPG2 format. The tracking software used was TheRealFishTracker V.0.4.0 (https://www.dgp.toronto.edu/~mccrae/projects/FishTracker/), developed and freely available from the University of Toronto. In our hands, it was the only software able to detect properly transparent CF larvae. The parameters used were Confidence Threshold = 10; Mean Filter Size = 1; signed Image = dark object and all other parameters were the default ones. The x/y-scales were drawn and given in cm. The output data file provided 30 X/Y coordinates (in pixels and in cm) per seconds for a 1-hour movie.
Graphical representations, quantifications and statistics
Statistical analyses were carried out with R-3.4.2 software (R Development Core Team, 2016) using the stats and rstatix libraries, and all graphical representations were designed using the ggplot2, ggrepel, gghighlight and RGraphics libraries.
Quantitative parameters
From the output data set, pixel coordinates Xpi and Ypi were first recoded using the box center as 0, Xcmax = 1 on the odor side and Xcmin = -1 on the water side. The same was applied to the Y coordinate with Ycmax = 0.74 and Ycmin = -0.74. Fish position in the box along the X-axis during test was calculated using Xc and Yc at each time point (30/s). Speed between two time points (5-second time step) was calculated thanks to pythagoras’s theorem using X/Y in cm. Round trip number is the total number of time the fish crosses the 0.5 and -0.5 position (along X-axis) divided by two. Idem for round trips in Y-axis. Quantitative parameter means were all calculated over a 15-minute period, from minute 10 to minute 25 for the ‘before’ period, and from minute 37 to minute 52 for the ‘after’ period. Means are represented by box plots showing the distribution of individual values of each fish (black dots), the median, the 25th- 75th percentile and outliers indicated by fish ID number. Paired Mann-Whitney two-tailed tests were performed to compare means of position, speed, or round trips before and after odor injection.
Non-paired Mann-Whitney two-tailed tests were performed to compare means of speed between 1h and 24h habituation periods represented in Figure 2C by box plots.
Swimming patterns
The swimming activity of the fish in the test box was systematically represented under 3 different forms, which allow grasping the details and different aspects of the behaviors (see Figure 2A). Xc /t coordinates were used with the geom_line() function for the ’Position (X-axis)’ graph, (Xc,Yc) /t coordinates were used with the geom_path() function for ’2D’ top view, and the gg3D package for ’2D + time’ view.
The determination of baseline swimming patterns and swimming patterns after odor injection was performed manually based on graphical representations such as in Figure 2A or Figure 3A. Four distinctive baseline behaviors clearly emerged. (1) Random swim (R; defined as haphazard swimming with no clear pattern, covering entirely or partly the surface of the arena). (2) Wall following (WF; defined as the fish continuously following along the 4 sides of the box and turning around it, in a clockwise or counterclockwise fashion). (3) Large or small circles (C; self-explanatory). (4) Thigmotactism (T, along the X- or the Y-axis of the box; defined as the fish swimming back and forth along one of the 4 sides of the box). On graphical representations of swimming pattern distributions, we used the following color code: R in blue, WF in red, C in green, T in yellow. Of note, many fish swam according to combination(s) of these four elementary swimming patterns (see descriptions in the legends of Supplemental figures showing many examples). To fully represent the diversity and the combinations of swimming patterns used by individual fish, we used an additional color code derived from the “basic” color code described above and where, for example R+WF is purple. The complete combinatorial color code is shown in Suppl. Fig7. Of note, in all figures, the swimming pattern color code does not relate whatsoever with the time color code used in the 2D plus time representation of swimming tracks such as in Figure 2A. Swimming patterns (i.e., qualitative variables; compositional dataset) in different conditions were analyzed using Fisher’s exact test comparing the quantity and distribution of different swimming patterns between SF/CF/F2 at day1, or over day1 to day4, or between 1h vs 24 h habituation period, or before vs after odor injection for each morphotype.
To reinforce our conclusions, swimming patterns in different conditions were also compared using CA (Correspondence Analysis), an appropriate method to analyze compositional data (Greenacre, 2021). The results are plotted in Suppl. Fig8. The distribution of swimming patterns in SF/CF/F2 in control experiments, or before vs after odor injection for each morphotype confirm the changes (or absence of changes) in behavioral patterns suggested by the colored bar plots in main Figures, with confidence ellipses overlapping or not overlapping, depending on cases. Of note, CA analysis cannot provide statistical support through calculation of p values.
Index and score
For quantitative swimming parameters: indexes of position, speed and number of round trips changes were calculated using the equation (mean after – mean before) / (mean after + mean before) commonly used in olfactory tests in fish (Choi et al., 2021; Koide et al., 2009; Wakisaka et al., 2017). The index can vary from zero to 1. An index close to zero indicates that the values of the considered parameter before or after odor injection are close. The higher the absolute value of the index, the greater the difference. When the parameter increases after injection, the index is positive; when the parameter decreases after injection, the index is negative. The absolute value of each index will be used for score calculation below.
For qualitative swimming parameter: the index of pattern change was calculated as the number of new patterns observed after stimulation, divided by the total number of patterns observed after odor injection. An index equal to zero indicates no pattern change after stimulation. The higher the index is, the greater the difference of pattern is.
For each fish, the individual olfactory score for a given odor and concentration was the sum of the absolute values of the four indexes: individual score = |position index| + |speed index| +|round trips in X+Y index| +|pattern index|. On Figure 7A and Suppl. Fig5, the 4 parameter’s indexes and the total individual fish olfactory scores are represented by dots connected by a colored line for each individual. Those with a score > 1.5 are highlighted and considered to display a significant behavioral response to the stimulus. The 1.5 threshold value was arbitrarily determined after the examination of individual olfactory scores in control experiments (Suppl. Fig5) and the observation that the vast majority of fish have individual scores below this value in the absence of specific olfactory stimulus.
On Figure 8A and 8C, to assess the relationship between individual olfactory score and baseline swimming pattern, we pooled results of odors for which more than 40% of individuals have an olfactory score >1.5. So for SF, alanine [10-2M] / cysteine [10-2M] / histidine [10-2M] / serine [10-2M] have been used; for CF, alanine [10-2M] / alanine [10-3M] / alanine [10-4M]/ cysteine [10-2M] / cysteine [10-3M] / histidine [10-2M] / lysine [10-2M]; and for F2, alanine [10-3M] / cysteine [10-2M] / histidine [10-2M] (see Table 1B). Mann-Whitney two-tailed with Bonferroni post hoc tests were performed on each morphotype.
Slopes in linear regressions show relationships between mean speed or number of round trips before injection and olfactory score. Linear correlations were calculated with Spearman’s rank correlation test followed by student test for the p.values.
Supplementary information and data availability
The R script used to recode raw data into corrected coordinates, to calculate means and to draw 2D and 2D + time graphs is available on GitHub (https://github.com/Maryline-Blin/Astyanax-swimming-pattern).
F1 to F3 Files correspond to source data files (.txt) containing the raw data presented and analyzed in this paper. For each type, the files provide the ID numbers of the fish, condition (day1 to day4 or odor or control type), means of position, speed, X and Y round-trips and behavior for periods 10-25 minutes and 37-52 minutes, as well as the calculated individual olfactory score.
Acknowledgements
We thank Camille Lejeune for her participation in the generation of the SF x CF F1 hybrid line, Maxime Policarpo for preliminary testing of tracking softwares and Rose Tatarsky for interesting discussions. Zootechnical care of our Astyanax fish facility was taken in charge by the TEFOR Unit. We are indebted to Joël Attia for invaluable help with statistical analyses during the revision of our manuscript. This work was supported by Equipe FRM (Fondation pour la Recherche Médicale) grant EQU202003010144 to SR.
References
- A 3D molecular map of the cavefish neural plate illuminates eye-field organization and its borders in vertebratesDevelopment 149
- The molecular logic of smellSci Am 273:154–9
- Diversity of Olfactory Responses and Skills in Astyanax Mexicanus Cavefish Populations Inhabiting different CavesDiversity 12:1–21
- Developmental evolution and developmental plasticity of the olfactory epithelium and olfactory skills in Mexican cavefishDev Biol 441:242–251
- Distribution and functional organization of glomeruli in the olfactory bulbs of zebrafish (Danio rerio)J Comp Neurol 520:2317–39
- The molecular architecture of odor and pheromone sensing in mammalsCell 100:611–8
- Expanded olfactory system in ray- finned fishes capable of terrestrial explorationBMC Biol 21
- Sensory evolution in a cavefish radiation: patterns of neuromast distribution and associated behaviour in Sinocyclocheilus (Cypriniformes: Cyprinidae)Proc Biol Sci 289
- Specialized neurons in the right habenula mediate response to aversive olfactory cuesElife 10
- The exceptional diversity of visual adaptations in deep-sea teleost fishesSemin Cell Dev Biol 106:20–30
- Investigating the evolution of sleep in the Mexican cavefishBiology and Evolution of the Mexican Cavefish. Chapter 15 Elsevier, Academic Press :335–359
- A Mutation in the Enzyme MonoAmine Oxidase Explains Part of the Astyanax Cavefish Behavioral SyndromeNature Communications Apr 10
- Contrasting feeding habits of post-larval and adult Astyanax cavefishSubterranean Biology 21:1–17
- Processing of odor representations by neuronal circuits in the olfactory bulbAnn N Y Acad Sci 1170:293–7
- Compositional Data AnalysisAnnual Review of Statistics and Its Application 8:271–299
- Olfaction and gustation in fish: an overviewActa Physiol Scand 152:207–17
- The diversity of fishes. Biology, Evolution, and EcologyWiley-Blackwell
- Sensory evolution in blind cavefish is driven by early events during gastrulation and neurulationDevelopment 143:4521–4532
- Social behavior and aggressiveness in AstyanaxBiology and Evolution of the Mexican Cavefish. Chapter 17 Elsevier, Academic Press :335–359
- Olfactory sensitivity to amino acids in the blackspot sea bream (Pagellus bogaraveo): a comparison between olfactory receptor recording techniques in seawaterJ Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 197:839–49
- Food-finding ability in cave fish (Astyanax fasciatus)Int. J. Speleol 16:59–66
- Social-like responses are inducible in asocial Mexican cavefish despite the exhibition of strong repetitive behaviorElife 10
- Towards a comprehensive catalog of zebrafish behavior 1.0 and beyondZebrafish 10:70–86
- Neural circuits mediating olfactory-driven behavior in fishFront Neural Circuits 7
- The ’goatee’ of goatfish: innervation of taste buds in the barbels and their representation in the brainProc Biol Sci 269:1773–80
- Olfactory neural circuitry for attraction to amino acids revealed by transposon-mediated gene trap approach in zebrafishProc Natl Acad Sci U S A 106:9884–9
- Utilizing the blind cavefish Astyanax mexicanus to understand the genetic basis of behavioral evolutionJ Exp Biol 223
- All rodents are not the same: a modern synthesis of cortical organizationBrain Behav Evol 78:51–93
- Early development of functional spatial maps in the zebrafish olfactory bulbJ Neurosci 25:5784–95
- Evolutionary convergence of a neural mechanism in the cavefish lateral line systemElife 11
- Nutritional regulation of glucose metabolism-related genes in the emerging teleost model Mexican tetra surface fish: a first explorationR Soc Open Sci 7
- Chondroitin fragments are odorants that trigger fear behavior in fishCurr Biol 22:538–44
- Active wall following by Mexican blind cavefish (Astyanax mexicanus)J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 196:853–67
- A mutation in monoamine oxidase (MAO) affects the evolution of stress behavior in the blind cavefish Astyanax mexicanusJ Exp Biol 223
- Coevolution of the olfactory organ and its receptor repertoire in ray- finned fishesBMC Biol 20
- Restoring eye size in Astyanax mexicanus blind cavefish embryos through modulation of the Shh and Fgf8 forebrain organising centresDevelopment 138:2467–76
- R: a language and environment for statistical computing
- Integrating animal temperament within ecology and evolutionBiol Rev Camb Philos Soc 82:291–318
- Analysis across diverse fish species highlights no conserved transcriptome signature for proactive behaviourBMC Genomics 22
- Vergleichende Untersuchungen an den Hautsinnesorganen ober- and unter-irdisch lebender Astyanax-ForamenZ Morph Tiere 61:255–316
- The function of wall-following behaviors in the Mexican blind cavefish and a sighted relative, the Mexican tetra (Astyanax)J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 195:225–40
- Breeding behavior in the blind Mexican cavefish and its river-dwelling conspecificPLoS One 14
- Brain diversity evolves via differences in patterningProc Natl Acad Sci U S A 107:9718–23
- Maternally regulated gastrulation as a source of variation contributing to cavefish forebrain evolutionElife 8
- Response of the hammerhead shark olfactory epithelium to amino acid stimuliJ Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 195:947–54
- Taste bud development and patterning in sighted and blind morphs of Astyanax mexicanusDev Dyn 238:3056–64
- Isolation and characterization of the laure olfactory behavioral mutant in the zebrafish, Danio rerioDev Dyn 234:229–42
- An Adenosine Receptor for Olfaction in FishCurr Biol 27:1437–1447
- Pleiotropic functions of embryonic sonic hedgehog expression link jaw and taste bud amplification with eye loss during cavefish evolutionDev Biol 330:200–11
- Molecular Genetic Dissection of the Zebrafish Olfactory SystemResults Probl Cell Differ
- The evolution of sensory adaptation in Astyanax mexicanusBiology and Evolution of the Mexican Cavefish. Chapter 13 Elsevier, Academic Press :335–359
- Evolution of a behavioral shift mediated by superficial neuromasts helps cavefish find food in darknessCurr Biol 20:1631–6
- Evolutionary tuning of an adaptive behavior requires enhancement of the neuromast sensory systemCommunicative and Integrative Biology 4:89–91
- The sensitivity of lateral line receptors and their role in the behavior of Mexican blind cavefish (Astyanax mexicanus)J Exp Biol 217:886–95
Article and author information
Author information
Version history
- Preprint posted:
- Sent for peer review:
- Reviewed Preprint version 1:
- Reviewed Preprint version 2:
- Version of Record published:
Copyright
© 2023, Blin et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
- views
- 1,050
- downloads
- 65
- citation
- 1
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.