Optogenetic feedback control of neural activity
Abstract
Optogenetic techniques enable precise excitation and inhibition of firing in specified neuronal populations and artifact-free recording of firing activity. Several studies have suggested that optical stimulation provides the precision and dynamic range requisite for closed-loop neuronal control, but no approach yet permits feedback control of neuronal firing. Here we present the ‘optoclamp’, a feedback control technology that provides continuous, real-time adjustments of bidirectional optical stimulation in order to lock spiking activity at specified targets over timescales ranging from seconds to days. We demonstrate how this system can be used to decouple neuronal firing levels from ongoing changes in network excitability due to multi-hour periods of glutamatergic or GABAergic neurotransmission blockade in vitro as well as impinging vibrissal sensory drive in vivo. This technology enables continuous, precise optical control of firing in neuronal populations in order to disentangle causally related variables of circuit activation in a physiologically and ethologically relevant manner.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.07192.001eLife digest
Cells called neurons use electrical signals to rapidly carry information around the body. When a neuron is activated, it generates (or ‘fires’) a short electrical impulse that travels along the cell to relay a message to other neurons, muscles or organs. Optogenetics is a technique that allows scientists to genetically modify neurons to produce proteins that make them light sensitive.
One of the most commonly used light-sensitive proteins is called channelrhodopsin-2. It is activated by blue light and increases the electrical activity of neurons. Another protein is called halorhodopsin, which responds to yellow light and inhibits the firing of neurons. By shining light of particular colors onto neurons that produce these and other light-sensitive proteins, it is possible to manipulate the activity of large populations of neurons.
Most previous optogenetic experiments have involved altering the activity of neurons and then observing the outcome at a later point in time. However, it would be very useful to be able to alter the amount of optical stimulation to achieve particular levels of neuron activity in real time. To achieve this, the level of neuron activity at any point in time would need to be quickly compared to the desired level, so that optogenetics could be used to increase or decrease the firing of neurons as appropriate.
Newman et al. have now developed an optogenetic system called ‘optoclamp’ that can control the activity of neurons in real time. In neurons grown in cell culture, the optoclamp is able to hold the level of neuron activity at particular values for periods of time ranging from 60 seconds to 24 hours. It can be used to restore and maintain the baseline level of neuron activity in the presence of drugs that would otherwise produce large increases or decreases in the firing of neurons. Moreover, in anaesthetized rats, the optoclamp can prevent some neurons from being activated even when the rats' whiskers move, which would normally change their firing level.
Newman et al.'s findings open the door to a new type of neuroscience experiment where it is possible to manipulate activity patterns as they are produced by the brain. This will help researchers to understand how particular patterns of brain activity are linked to learning, memory, and behavior.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.07192.002Introduction
Feedback is essential for controlling complicated systems. It can be used to define system dynamics and decouple causal interactions. Recently, a diverse set of specialized techniques that employ elements of feedback control have emerged for studying adaptation in neuronal micro-circuits (Ahrens et al., 2012), using electrical stimulation to control spike latency (Wallach et al., 2011) and firing levels (Wagenaar et al., 2005; Newman et al., 2013), improving brain-computer interfaces (Velliste et al., 2008; Cunningham et al., 2011), inducing motor plasticity (Jackson et al., 2006), and controlling intracellular firing rate (Miranda-Domínguez et al., 2010). True feedback control has been most broadly applied in neuroscience research using the voltage clamp, which is used to decouple the membrane potential from causally related voltage-dependent conductances. This approach has provided the foundation for our understanding intracellular electrochemical signaling and demands extension to other features of neural activity.
The neuronal firing rate is a basic feature of codes for motor action (Georgopoulos et al., 1988), vision (Steinmetz et al., 1987), and place (Zhang et al., 1998). Changes in thalamic firing tone can alter cortical receptive fields (Stoelzel et al., 2009) and the nature of temporal coding within the thalamocortical pathways (Wang et al., 2010). Long-term changes in network firing levels can trigger a multitude of homeostatic processes that regulate circuit excitability and stability (Turrigiano et al., 1998; Corner et al., 2002; Turrigiano, 2011). A system analogous to the voltage clamp, but capable of precise, bidirectional control of circuit firing levels could be used to identify the independent role of firing rate in downstream processes in spite of changes to causally-related variables. For instance, long-term changes in population firing have long been thought to initiate compensatory homeostatic mechanisms, but a causal link has remained elusive. Direct bidirectional control over population firing rates would allow us to test this hypothesis directly. Optogenetic tools are routinely used to provide genetically specified, millisecond time-scale stimulation or suppression of neural activity with light (Mattis et al., 2011) during simultaneous, artifact-free electrical recording. The ability to simultaneously perturb and measure neural activity form the basic elements of a feedback loop, which can be exploited to control firing. Although several studies have presented closed-loop optogenetic stimulation techniques (Leifer et al., 2011; Stirman et al., 2011; Paz et al., 2012; Krook-Magnuson et al., 2013; O'Connor et al., 2013; Siegle and Wilson, 2014), no approach yet permits feedback control of neuronal firing levels.
Here we describe and quantify optogenetic feedback control (‘optoclamping’), a method enabling continuous, bi-directional, closed-loop firing rate control both in vitro and in vivo. We show that the optoclamp allows precise control of population firing levels in dissociated cortical networks over timescales ranging from seconds to days. We characterize the effects of different control schemes, algorithm parameters, and optical waveforms on the precision of feedback control and higher-order statistics of population activity. We show that firing rate control can be achieved over many hours and used to restore pre-drug firing levels during chronic blockade of excitatory or inhibitory synaptic transmission. Using this approach, we decouple the effects of suppressed neurotransmission from the indirect effects on network firing, and find that changes in firing levels are not required to induce homeostatic alterations in network excitability. Finally, we show how optogenetic feedback can be used to control firing activity in vibrissal somatosensory thalamus of rats. We find that background thalamic activity levels can be controlled during ongoing sensory input without corrupting the fine-scale temporal structure of whisker-evoked spike trains. Together, our results demonstrate that the optoclamp is an effective general tool for decoupling neural firing from other variables that would normally affect network excitability.
Results
Characterizing bidirectional optical control signals
To characterize the range of evoked firing levels that could be achieved using multimodal optical stimulation in dissociated cortical networks, we stimulated excitatory cells expressing channelrhodopsin-2(H134R) (ChR2R) (Nagel et al., 2005) and enhanced halorhodopsin-3.0 (eNpHR3.0) (Gradinaru et al., 2008) while recording spiking activity using a 59-channel microelectrode array (MEA; Figure 1A) (Newman et al., 2013). For ChR2R activation, a single dimensionless excitatory control variable, UC, simultaneously modulated the pulse width, frequency, and intensity of homogeneous 465 nm stimuli (Equations 7–9, ‘Materials and methods’; Figure 1—figure supplement 1 and Figure 1—figure supplement 2). For eNpHR3.0 activation, we defined a second control variable, UH, proportional to the continuous intensity of a 590 nm LED (Equation 10). We applied UC and UH ranging from 0 to 1, for randomly interleaved, 60-s stimulation epochs (2 cultures, 50 trials/culture). Evoked population firing rates were positively correlated with UC and negatively correlated with UH (Figure 1B). UC-evoked firing levels saturated at approximately 12.5 Hz/unit corresponding to UC = 0.47 (freq. = 14.7 Hz, pulse-width: 2.4 ms, power at 465 nm: 6.9 mW·mm−2). Firing rate suppression saturated at 0.04 Hz/unit corresponding to UH = 0.15 (power at 590 nm: 1.8 mW mm−2). The monotonic relationships between UC and UH and network firing levels indicated their applicability as closed-loop control signals.
We also tested the robustness of UC and UH for modulating firing levels during blockade of AMPAergic, NMDAergic, or GABAergic transmission using CNQX, AP5, or bicuculline, respectively (‘Materials and methods’). Synaptic blockade strongly affected mean spontaneous firing levels (CNQX: −74.2%, AP5: −66.6%, bicuculine: +357%) and network-level signal propagation (Figure 1C). In spite of this, firing in CNQX- and AP5-treated networks could be driven over the same dynamic range as the drug free condition (Figure 1B; CNQX: 0.057 to 13.3 Hz/unit, AP5: 0.088 to 12.6 Hz/unit), indicating that optogenetic input could compensate for depressed excitatory transmission. Bicuculline greatly reduced the dynamic range of evoked network activity indicating a loss of reliable activity modulation (Figure 1B; 0.095 to 5.1 Hz/unit).
Although time-averaged firing levels were monotonically related to UC and UH (Figure 1B), open-loop stimuli lost effectiveness throughout each 60-s trial causing significant second-to-second drift in evoked activity levels (Figure 1D,E). This effect was consistent across synaptic blocker conditions. Decreases in stimulus efficacy using ChR2R were likely due to network adaptation, rather than changes in ChR2R-mediated photocurrents (Mattis et al., 2011). Aside from network adaptation, deceased efficacy of eNpHR3.0-mediated firing suppression likely resulted from a loss of synaptic inhibition due to intracellular Cl− accumulation (Raimondo et al., 2012) and decreased outward photocurrents due to pump desensitization (Mattis et al., 2011).
Proportional-integral control of network firing
We developed a proportional-integral (PI) feedback controller to clamp network population activity in dissociated cortical networks in the face of uncontrolled fluctuations in neuronal excitability and opsin dynamics (Equations 1–6). The PI algorithm updated UC and UH in real-time in order to minimize the difference (‘error’) between the measured network firing rate and a target level (‘Materials and methods’). We tested the controller using 60-s, randomly-ordered targets ranging from 0–10 Hz/unit (Figure 2A; 7 cultures, 11 trials/culture), and successful control was achieved in over 90% of trials (Figure 2B; 71/77 trials; mean RMS tracking error 0.14 ± 0.091 Hz/unit). Tracking error increased with target rate, and occasionally the stimulator saturated before the trial was complete (e.g., Figure 2A, grey line). Control settling time varied across preparations and was not correlated with the target firing rate (mean ± SD, 7.83 ± 6.07 s; Figure 2—figure supplement 1). Although we generally used discrete steps in the target firing rate, the controller was also capable of tracking continuously varying targets (Figure 2—figure supplement 2).
We explored the parametic sensitivity of the controller by changing the value of either the proportional gain (K), the integral-error time constant (Ti), or the firing rate filter time constant (τ), while the remaining two parameters were held at their nominal values (K = 0.1, Ti = 1 s, τ = 2.5 s). Functional control was achieved at K < 1.0, 1.0 s < Ti <10 s, and 0.5 s <τ <10 s. Outside of these bounds, closed-loop dynamics were unstable and/or firing levels exhibited significant target offsets (Figure 2—figure supplement 3 through Figure 2—figure supplement 5).
The control signals, UC and UH, were highly variable across networks, even for the same target rate (Figure 2C). This variability likely reflects heterogeneous network excitability, opsin expression, synaptic connectivity, and developmental processes (Wagenaar et al., 2006) and suggests that the controller continuously adapts to ongoing changes in network excitability in order to precisely clamp firing levels. To test this possibility, we delivered open-loop replay of successful closed-loop control signals and found they were incapable of controlling firing (Figure 4, Figure 2 cultures). This demonstrates the necessity of real-time feedback to achieve precise control of neural firing, even for same target rate within single preparations.
Next, we used the PI controller to clamp network firing levels to targets between 0 and 10 Hz/unit during blockade of AMPAergic and NMDAergic transmission (Figure 2—figure supplement 6A; ‘Materials and methods’). Control performance was equivalent to the drug-free case and control failure was isolated to high target rates (9–10 Hz/unit) due to stimulus saturation (Figure 2—figure supplement 6B–E). We also tested PI control during blockade of GABAergic transmission and found that reliable control was not possible (Figure 2—figure supplement 6). This is likely due to the destabilizing effects of bicuculline, which caused the controller to oscillate. This result is consistent with studies indicating a general role of reduced inhibition in diseases of circuit instability such as temporal lobe epilepsy (Kobayashi and Buckmaster, 2003) and Dravet syndrome (Dutton et al., 2013).
To demonstrate PI control over more extended time periods, we clamped firing at a set of randomly selected, 5-min long target firing levels which switched without downtime (50 min. total clamp time; Figure 3). During each 5-min step, the controller made rapid, second-to-second adjustments in stimulus intensity to maintain the instantaneous target rate, while slower changes in stimulus intensity occurred over minutes. Minute-to-minute changes in the control signal intensity likely reflect short-term synaptic depression and changes in cellular excitability that accrued over each control epoch. Therefore, the control signal can be used as a readout of network excitability, analogous to how injected current from a voltage clamp amplifier can be used to asses cellular excitability.
To test how different stimulus waveforms affected network response correlations during PI control, we mapped UC onto triangular, sinusoidal, pseudorandom binary sequence (PRBS), and direct intensity modulation inputs (Tchumatchenko et al., 2013). Each permitted successful closed-loop control, but notably, the choice of stimulus waveform significantly affected the peak firing correlation (P = 6.5 × 10−24) and synchrony (P = 10−22) of the population response (Figure 4). The square pulse trains typically used for ChR2-based stimulation (Mattis et al., 2011) resulted in periodic, highly correlated population firing (Figure 4A). Compared to square pulses, sinusoidal stimuli decreased peak unit-to-unit firing correlations (−10.6%, p = 0.028), but did not affect synchrony. PRBS stimuli reduced peak correlations (−24.0%, p = 6.8 × 10−13) and firing synchrony (−17.9%, p = 4.6 × 10−8). Continuous light modulation increased both correlations (+21.3%, p = 4.3 × 10−5) and synchrony (+41.4%, p = 2.5 × 10−9). Triangular pulses did not affect correlation or synchrony. Importantly, while periodic input signals produced a periodic response, PBRS input and continuous intensity modulation resulted in non-periodic firing. Therefore, altering the temporal characteristics of excitatory stimulus waveforms resulted in remarkably different higher-order firing statistics while still enabling successful PI control. This emphasizes the fact that, in its current form, the optoclamp only controls population firing levels and leaves more complex features of neural activity unconstrained and subject to the influence of network connectivity, network dynamics, and the nature of the stimulus signal.
Multi-hour control of firing rates
Planar MEAs allow stable extracellular recordings over long timescales. Previous studies have used MEAs to monitor spiking activity over many hours and correlated recorded activity with homeostatic or developmental changes in network properties (Wagenaar et al., 2006; Minerbi et al., 2009). Compared to simply measuring spiking activity, controlling mean firing rates over long timescales would enable investigations of causal, rather than correlative, relationships between spiking and long-term, activity-dependent processes.
To this end, we developed an ‘on-off’ controller to clamp network activity across many hours. To clamp firing rates above spontaneous levels, this controller delivered a blue light pulse (5 ms, 13.4 mW mm−2, 465 nm) when the integral error exceeded zero (Equation 12). We tested on-off control by clamping network firing rates within a single culture to seven elevated setpoints (ranging from 0.75 to 6 Hz/unit) for 12-hr epochs (Figure 5). The on-off controller successfully held firing rates at six of these targets, saturating only at 6 Hz/unit after ∼7 hr (Figure 5A,B). Notably, the stimulation frequency required to maintain each target firing rate was better correlated to the difference between the target and the pre-clamp firing rate than the target rate alone (R2 = 0.63 vs 0.52; Figure 5—figure supplement 1). This indicates that alterations in network excitability across different experimental days were reflected in the intensity of the control inputs (Figure 5C,D; Figure 5—figure supplement 1A) and suggests that optogenetic feedback control can be used to study changes in neuronal network dynamics over developmental timescales.
To clamp firing rates below spontaneous levels, the on-off controller delivered continuous yellow light (∼11 mW mm−2, 590 nm) when the integral error signal fell below zero (Equation 13; ‘Materials and methods’). Because chronic activation of eNpHR-3.0 produced relatively weak photocurrents and induced a depolarizing shift in the chloride reversal potential (Raimondo et al., 2012), we found that it was inadequate for long-term control. For this reason, archaerhodopsin-3.0 (Arch-3.0) was used for multi-hour firing rate suppression (Chow et al., 2010; Mattis et al., 2011) (‘Materials and methods’). To validate this strategy, we clamped a culture's spiking activity to ∼60% of its spontaneous firing rate (from 1.23 to 0.75 Hz/unit) over a 3-hr epoch (Figure 5—figure supplement 2).
The on-off and PI control schemes have distinct advantages and disadvantages. PI control provided rapid response times and low RMS error over small time windows, but imposed strong, short timescale (∼50 ms) response correlations between units (Figure 5C; Figure 4). This correlation structure contrasts the aperiodic, network bursting activity that is a common feature of developing neural circuits in vivo and in vitro (O'Donovan et al., 1998; Feller, 1999; Wagenaar et al., 2006). We found that both excitatory and inhibitory on-off control were better able to preserve spontaneous activity correlations than PI control (Figure 5E; Figure 5—figure supplement 2C). However, for targets that required higher excitatory stimulation rates during on-off control, a periodic correlation structure emerged (Figure 5E).
Using on-off control to reveal the direct role of neurotransmission in regulation of network excitability
Previous in vitro studies have shown that chronic elevation in network activity using GABAergic transmission blockers leads to a homeostatic reduction in firing rate following relief from blockade (Turrigiano et al., 1998). Conversely, chronic reductions in activity using glutamatergic blockers elicit homeostatic increases in firing rate (Corner et al., 2002). Interestingly, we did not observe homeostatic changes in firing rate following prolonged increases or decreases in network spiking activity during on-off feedback control (Figure 5; Figure 5—figure supplement 1C and Figure 5—figure supplement 2B). This suggests that altered synaptic transmission and altered spiking activity may have distinct effects on homeostatic regulation of network excitability.
To test this possibility, we used on-off control to decouple the effects of prolonged glutamatergic or GABAergic blockade from changes in firing rate. We treated networks with CNQX (2 cultures), AP5 (2 cultures), or bicuculline (1 culture), and then used on-off control to restore pre-drug firing rates during 24-hr (CNQX or AP5) or 3-hr (bicuculline) periods. In all cases, the controller effectively clamped firing rates to pre-drug levels (Figure 6A.v,B.v,C.v). 10 min of activity were recorded in the presence of each drug just before and after each clamp period. As expected, application of CNQX or AP5 caused marked reductions in network spiking activity compared to the pre-drug levels and these reduced activity levels were sustained upon relief from on-off control (Figure 6A.v,B.v). Meanwhile, bicuculline greatly increased firing rate, both before and after the clamp period (Figure 6C.v).
The mean stimulation frequency remained low during excitatory on-off control in the presence of CNQX or AP5 (Figure 6A.iv,B.iv); CNQX: 0.72 and 0.19 Hz, AP5: 0.21 and 0.71 Hz). For CNQX, pre-drug network activity correlations and burst shape were largely maintained during on-off control (Figure 6A.vi–vii). For AP5, the unit-to-unit correlation time and burst duration were shorter than pre-drug levels (Figure 6B.vi–vii). This is likely due to the prominent role of NMDA receptors in signal propagation in dissociated cortical networks (Nakanishi and Kukita, 1998). Following drug removal, spontaneous firing was elevated compared to pre-drug levels (CNQX: +86.4 and +21.1%, AP5: +157.4 and +273.3%).
Inhibitory on-off control using Arch3.0 was used to clamp firing to pre-drug levels during bicuculline treatment (‘Materials and methods’). Bicuculline greatly increased network firing correlations and burst duration compared to spontaneous activity levels. During the clamp epoch, ‘on’ to ‘off’ control transitions reliably triggered large rebound bursts. A rapid closed-loop response truncated rebound bursts via reactivation of Arch-3.0 (Figure 6C.vi–vii). After washing bicuculline, firing was reduced by 47.2% compared to pre-drug levels.
Notably, homeostatic changes in spiking levels were observed following prolonged glutamatergic or GABAergic blockade even though network firing rates were maintained at pre-drug levels during the treatment windows. This indicates that changes in firing rate were not required to drive compensatory alterations in network excitability. Instead, homeostatic alterations of network excitability were triggered directly by suppressed synaptic activity. In line with this result, on-off control has recently been used to show that upward synaptic scaling, the most widely studied form of homeostatic plasticity, is directly induced via reductions in AMPA receptor activation (Fong et al., 2015).
Control of single unit activity during ongoing sensory perturbations, in vivo
We next evaluated the functionality of optogenetic feedback control in the intact rodent brain. The rat vibrissal pathway is a widely studied model of sensory information transduction due to its well defined discrete feed-forward anatomy. Recent findings have revealed the importance of network activity state for gating sensory information in thalamic networks (Halassa et al., 2014). We used optogenetic feedback to control background firing state in single units of the ventral posteromedial nucleus (VPm) in anesthetized rats during ongoing vibrissa stimulation. Extracellular recordings of ChR2-expressing thalamocortical units (TCUs) were used to update an integral controller (Equation 15) (parameters: Ti = 1 s, τ = 0.8 s), which determined the continuous intensity of 470 nm light delivered to VPm using an optical fiber (Figure 7A; ‘Materials and methods’).
We used optogenetic feedback to clamp firing rates in TCUs at increasing target levels for 30-s epochs (Figure 7B). For > 75% of TCUs, the controller effectively clamped firing over a range of target rates, which varied across cells (e.g., 4–40 Hz vs 18–22 Hz; Figure 7C). As in our in vitro PI experiments, the RMS tracking error trended upward with increasing target rates (Figure 7D). Because firing rate estimates were derived from single cells instead of population activity, the RMS tracking error was larger than for in vitro control (mean ± SD, 2.0 ± 0.8 Hz). Controller settling time was not correlated to the target firing rate and was shorter than for in vitro control (mean ± SD, 3.3 ± 3.2 s).
The optical power required for successful control varied greatly from cell to cell (Figure 7E). For instance, the mean optical intensity required for successful control at 16 Hz, the most widely achieved target in our sample, spanned nearly two orders of magnitude (0.66–22.92 mW mm−2; Figure 7E). This suggests that open-loop optical stimuli would not result in consistent firing rates. Indeed, we found that linear increases in optical intensity resulted in extremely variable and temporally non-stationary firing over trials and units (Figure 7—figure supplement 1). Further, we examined whether successful control could be achieved by locking the stimulator at a static optical power once the controller stabilized. Halfway through each 30-s trial we locked the light signal either at the last output taken by the controller or the average control signal during first half of the trial (6 TCUs; Figure 7—figure supplement 2). Locking optical power at the mid-trial or trial-averaged level significantly increased RMS tracking error (+204 ± 227% and +238 ± 145%, respectively, p < 10−14 for both). This indicates that continuous stimulus updates are required to exert precise control over neural activity in vivo.
Next we examined how changes to the firing rate filter time-constant, τ, or the integral time-constant, Ti affected control performance (Figure 7—figure supplement 3). Increasing τ (from 0.8 to 1.6 s) introduced lag into the control loop, causing overshoot, decreased stability, and a significant increase in RMS tracking error (+57%, p = 1.4 × 10−6). Conversely, lowering τ (from 0.8 to 0.16 s) decreased overshoot and significantly reduced RMS tracking error (−46.9%, p = 1.2 × 10−10). Increasing Ti (from 1 to 10 s) did not significantly affect RMS tracking error (p = 0.088), but caused over-damping and increased controller settling time. Reducing Ti (from 1 to 0.1 s) significantly decreased RMS tracking error (−18%, p = 0.011) and introduced overshoot during control onset. Together, these results indicate that low-latency feedback and a short integral time constant improve the performance of firing rate control in vivo. In this experiment, τ = 0.16 s and Ti = 1 s gave the best performance in terms of RMS error.
In the awake animal, sensory thalamic spike trains tend to exhibit irregular firing with interval statistics close to those of a Poisson process (Poggio and Viernstein, 1964). Drugs used for anesthesia have profound effects on evoked and background firing (Simons et al., 1992), receptive field properties (Friedberg et al., 1999), and subthreshold voltage statistics (Constantinople and Bruno, 2011) in the vibrissal pathway. We calculated the coefficient of variation of the interspike interval (ISI) distribution (CVISI) for each TCU. Across target rates and units, we found a CVISI close to 1, indicating a Poisson-like spiking process (mean ± SD, 1.31 ± 0.33 across targets and units; Figure 7F). In comparison, the CVISI of spontaneous unit activity was significantly elevated (Figure 7F, inset) mean ± SD, 1.72 ± 0.43; p = 0.043), likely due to an increased propensity for burst firing in sensory thalamus during anesthesia and non-alert states (Stoelzel et al., 2009). This suggests that optogenetic feedback control using continuously modulated input can be used to mimic alert, Poisson-like spiking statistics in anesthetized animals.
Finally, we tested whether the controller could clamp firing during ongoing sensory drive. We recorded from TCUs that were responsive to punctate deflections of the corresponding primary vibrissa (5 TCUs, 8° at ∼1600 deg. s−1, 10 Hz; Figure 8—figure supplement 1; ‘Materials and methods’). Vibrissa deflections evoked stimulus-locked spike trains both in the presence and absence of closed-loop control (Figure 8, Figure 8—figure supplement 1). However, whisker stimuli resulted in little or no performance degradation of firing rate control in terms of mean TCU firing rate (Figure 8B; p = 0.09) or RMS tracking error (Figure 8C; p = 0.73) compared to control without whisker input. To maintain control during sensory stimulation, the controller automatically decreased LED stimulus intensity to accommodate sensory drive (Figure 8D; mean ± SD, 8.4 ± 5.34 to 4.9 ± 4.8 mW mm−2, p = 0.024).
Precise temporal spiking patterns carry information in the vibrissal sensory pathway (Curtis and Kleinfeld, 2009; Wang et al., 2010; Bruno, 2011). To characterize the timescales over which the optical controller and whisker stimuli affected firing, we calculated the spike-triggered average (STA) of optical power and whisker position (Figure 8E,F). During concurrent whisker stimulation and closed-loop control, the full-width at half-maximum (FWHM) of the STA for optical power and whisker position differed by more than an order of magnitude, indicating that sensory input and optical control signals affected firing on distinct timescales (Figure 8G; mean ± SD FWHM of STA: optical power, 0.46 ± 0.2 s vs whisker position, 18.3 ± 5.5 ms, p = 0.0079). Therefore, optogenetic feedback control using continuously modulated input provides a means to control baseline firing state without distorting the fine-scale temporal structure of sensory-evoked spike trains. Punctate vibrissa stimuli rearrange spike times instead of introducing additional spikes, allowing the temporal correlations in the firing rate to dictate the downstream response rather than the magnitude of the firing rate. This effect is reminiscent of firing modulation by free air whisking in vibrissa primary sensory cortex (Curtis and Kleinfeld, 2009) and contrasts the effect of pulsed optogenetic stimulation, which transiently overrides the influence of sensory input on the temporal structure of spike trains.
Discussion
All previous methods involving closed-loop or activity-guided optogenetic stimulation have used a physiological measurement to trigger static optical stimuli without corrective action or modification of stimulus parameters following stimulus onset (Leifer et al., 2011; Stirman et al., 2011; Paz et al., 2012; Krook-Magnuson et al., 2013; O'Connor et al., 2013; Siegle and Wilson, 2014). In contrast, the optoclamp continuously updates stimulus intensity and frequency in real-time to enable precise control of neural firing in cultured networks and single cells in vivo and therefore provides a foundation for techniques aiming to achieved true closed-loop optical control of neural activity (Grosenick et al., 2015). We have shown that this form of optogenetic feedback control is capable of clamping network firing rates using different stimulation protocols (Figure 4) and control algorithms (Equations 4, 12, 13, 14) over a wide range of controller parameters (Figure 2—figure supplement 3 through Figure 2—figure supplement 5, Figure 7—figure supplement 3) and timescales (Figure 2 vs Figure 5 through Figure 6). The optoclamp's ability to control firing levels even during various synaptic (Figure 6; Figure 2—figure supplement 6) and sensory (Figure 8) perturbations demonstrates its robustness and provides evidence of the method's suitability in a range of experiments. Finally, we found that even when using the same controller and target firing rate, successful control often required vastly different intensities of optical stimulation across experiments and experimental preperations (Figure 9). This suggests that closed-loop regulation of optical stimulation intensity may be a requirment, rather than a benefit, for evoking consistent activity levels across experiments.
Like the optoclamp, the voltage clamp relies on a continuously updated feedback loop to control neural activity. Although the scale of neural activity controlled by these two methods differs greatly (population firing rate vs membrane potential), comparing the optoclamp to the voltage clamp is useful for illustrating the power, potential uses, and shortcomings of optogenetic feedback control in its current form, and to highlight how the technique might be extended to allow control over features of neural activity other than firing levels.
Two capabilities of the voltage clamp technique have made it a ubiquitous tool for characterizing neuronal excitability and synaptic dynamics. First, by continuously servoing the current injected into a cell in order to maintain a target membrane potential, the injected current mirrors underlying synaptic or intrinsic conductances that are difficult to measure directly. Second, because the voltage clamp can precisely control the membrane potential, it can decouple strong causal relationships that exist between the membrane voltage and other variables. For example, measuring the open-loop relationship between the membrane voltage and voltage-dependent conductances is impossible in the unclamped cell since these variables feed back onto one another. The voltage clamp breaks this circular dependence in order to systematically examine the strength of conductances at fixed voltage levels.
The optoclamp affords analogous capabilities for examining network dynamics. For instance, population-level excitability in an unclamped network must be inferred through measures of firing activity, which in turn affects network excitability. By continuously updating the intensity of optical stimulation to clamp network firing at set levels, information about network excitability and afferent input can be read directly from optical control signals (Figure 3C). One shortcoming of this feature is that it is difficult to identify the origin of changes in network excitability that appear in optical control signals since they result from the aggregate effect of many different processes. However, parallel limitations also exist for the voltage clamp. For example, when using voltage clamp, it can be difficult to distinguish contributions from different neurotransmitter systems and intrinsic conductances or to deduce the relationship between synaptic currents measured at the recording site vs at the site of receptor binding. Another issue with both optical clamping and the voltage clamp is that the control signal can be contaminated by non-ideal aspects of the recording or stimulation setup. For instance, opsin desensitization could affect the intensity of light required for the optoclamp to maintain firing levels. Likewise, space clamp and seal stability issues that arise during voltage clamp influence the amount of current required to hold a target membrane potential.
In the case of the voltage clamp, these issues are partially compensated for by using secondary measurements of seal quality and by employing the voltage clamp in concert with drugs or genetic manipulations that help to isolate particular synaptic and intrinsic conductances. Different neural circuits have distinct population-level dynamics, connectivity, neurotransmitter systems, cellular constituents, and opsin expression properties. Therefore, analogous to the voltage clamp, the optoclamp should be used in combination with auxiliary techniques that can compensate for imperfections in the control system and allow the mechanistic underpinnings of changes in excitability that occur during clamping to be isolated. For instance, the effects of opsin desensitization and bandwidth can be factored out of optical control signals such that the control signals accurately reflect changes in network excitability during clamping (Tchumatchenko et al., 2013). Further, the optoclamp can be combined with existing pharmacological, genetic, or electrophysiological tools to isolate specific mechanisms that influence network excitability following clamping (Fong et al., 2015).
In addition to providing a means to quantify network excitability, like the voltage clamp, optogenetic feedback control is capable of decoupling causally related variables of circuit activation. Using two case studies, operating on very different time-scales, we have demonstrated that the optoclamp is capable of decoupling thalamocortical cell baseline state from fine-scale sensory-evoked firing patterns in vivo (Figure 8) and decoupling network firing levels from various forms of neurotransmission (Figure 6). The former is especially relevant given recent focus on the state-dependent nature of thalamic coding (Halassa et al., 2014), and the need for stimulation technologies capable of controlling non-stationary neural dynamics in order to inject meaningful sensory information into damaged circuits (Stanley, 2013). Decoupling firing levels from variables to which they are normally causally intertwined can also help clarify causal relationships between firing and other factors that influence network excitability. For instance, determining the independent roles of neurotransmission and spiking on various forms of plasticity has proven challenging since manipulation of neurotransmission invariably affects spiking levels, and vice versa. To overcome this, Fong et al. recently used the optoclamp to decouple network firing levels from long-term AMPAergic neurotransmission blockade to show that upward synaptic scaling is directly triggered by reduced AMPAergic transmission without relying on changes in spiking activity (Fong et al., 2015). This provides a plasticity mechanism to explain the increases in network firing levels we witnessed following long-term CNQX treatment even when firing was clamped to pre-drug levels for the duration of drug application (Figure 6A).
The control algorithms and technologies presented here are simple, straightforward, and widely available. Our implementations of the optoclamp are quite reliable for controlling neural activity in cultured networks and thalamic cells in vivo and the technique's simplicity lowers the barrier for its adoption. However, there are several avenues for further development of the method we have presented. For instance, we used a pre-sorting procedure to identify neurons used for real-time firing rate estimation in subsequent clamping periods. During periods of optical stimulation, neurons that were silent during the pre-sorting routine may be activated. If these cells had systematically different firing characteristics the sorted units used for firing rate estimation, then our procedure would lead to a biased estimate of network firing levels. Although we found no evidence that this was the case in our networks, the situation might differ for other neural preparations or brain regions. If this issue were to arise, spike sorting could be removed entirely and firing could be normalized by the number of recording channels, provided that the channel count is sufficiently high.
Further, the incorporation of more sophisticated activity measurement techniques, stimulation technologies, and control algorithms will enable improvements in control performance and broaden the applicability of optogenetic feedback control to different experimental contexts (Grosenick et al., 2015). For example, the incorporation of spatial light modulation would allow optical inputs to be steered towards the spike initiation zones of individual cells in order to minimize light exposure (Figure 9) and abnormal conductances, and potentially enable complex system identification alogrithms to be introduced into the feedback loop (Grosenick et al., 2015). Additionally, decreased feedback latency or the addition of predictive elements to the feedback loop may enable control over rapid sensory or motor events. There are two options to obtain accurate firing rate measures over very short timescales (e.g., that of individual whisker perturbations). The first option is to sample a very large population of neurons such that small time bins will have an adequate number of spikes for accurate estimation of population firing levels. This large population measure would need to be combined with a sub-millisecond feedback loop and opsins with extremely fast kinetics. Alternatively, if an accurate model of feedforward network dynamics could be incorporated into the controller, reliable control over fast events might be possible with a modestly sized population of cells, a slower feedback loop, and standard opsin variants. This is a viable approach in circuits for which predictive models of feedforward network dynamics are available, such as early visual, auditory, and vibrissal pathways (Wu et al., 2006; Millard et al., 2013), or, for which accurate input/output relationships can be deduced in situ using real-time system identification (Grosenick et al., 2015). Additionally, control algorithms that incorporate models of feed-forward neural dynamics will be more capable of stabilizing firing in unstable circuits, such as epileptic networks, without total cessation of ongoing activity.
We demonstrated that optical waveforms with very different temporal characteristics could be used to successfully control population firing rate in vitro while having markedly different effects on spiking correlation and synchrony across individual units (Figure 4). However, in the optoclamp's current form, higher-order temporal characteristics, such as the unit-to-unit firing correlation, are not actively controlled features of neural activity. Therefore, during clamping periods, these features of population firing will be dependent on network architecture, the identity and percentage the cells expressing opsins, and opsin dynamics. Improvements on our technique might treat higher order features of network activity, such as unit-to-unit correlation and synchrony, as secondary control targets. In this case, the controller would use real-time measures of higher order firing statistics to adjust the spatial and temporal characteristics of optical stimuli in order to enforce a particular firing structure, such as the heterogeneity of activity levels across cells and/or temporal variance of firing activity (e.g., regular firing vs bursting). Optogenetic feedback control could also be incorporated into more complex experimental contexts. For example, firing rate control could be made contingent on specific behaviors or complex spatiotemporal activity patterns associated with specific behaviors (Zhang et al., 1998), in order to introduce fictive reward or neuromodulatory signals to influence learning or alleviate pathological activity. In particular, optoclamping cortical activity to replace lost neuromodulatory tone is a potentially exciting future avenue for treating Parkinson's disease (Beuter et al., 2014).
Recently, several ‘all-optical’ electrophysiology techniques have been introduced to simultaneously measure neural activity via genetically encoded calcium sensors and optogenetically inject currents at single cell resolution (Packer et al., 2014; Rickgauer et al., 2014). If combined with real-time control, these techniques could offer the ability to optically clamp activity levels in specified subnetworks with far greater specificity than is afforded using electrodes. Perhaps most exciting, recent improvements in microbial rhodopsins for simultaneous voltage indication and optogenetic stimulation provide a means for all-optical measurement and perturbation of the membrane voltage at subcellular resolution and millisecond timescales (Flytzanis et al., 2014; Hochbaum et al., 2014). These tools even allow simultaneous sensing and actuation using a single opsin. Using opsins to both measure and actuate voltage within a feedback control loop will open the possibility of voltage clamping arbitrary populations of cells without puncturing their cell bodies. This would enable an unprecedented improvement of fine-scale control and measurement of neural circuit activation in vitro and, with specialized optics, in vivo.
In summary, we have performed a systematic and extensive investigation of how optogenetic feedback control can be used to precisely control neuronal firing levels during perturbations that strongly affect network excitability, across time scales ranging from seconds to days, both in vitro and in vivo. The functionality of our technique across control parameters, algorithms, preparations, and firing rate measures (network vs single units) indicates the robustness and general applicability of the technique to different experimental contexts. When combined with secondary genetic, pharmacological, or behavioral manipulations and tailored to particular experimental contexts using suitable control algorithms, we envision the use of optogenetic feedback control in a multitude of experimental and clinical contexts requiring precise control of neuronal activity. For these reasons, we believe that the optoclamp is a powerful addition to the expanding optogenetic toolbox and will improve and accelerate the study of neural control of motor action, sensory encoding and adaptation, neuromodulation, and activity homeostasis.
Materials and methods
Statistics
All statistical analyses were performed using MATLAB (MathWorks, Natick, MA). For tests between two groups, we first used a Lilliefors test (α = 0.05) to determine if sample distributions were normally distributed. If the null hypothesis of normality was rejected for one or both sample distribution(s), we performed a Wilcoxon signed-rank test (α = 0.05). Otherwise we used a paired t-test (α = 0.05). We used paired tests because our samples were ‘matched’ (i.e., the same culture or cell was examined in two different experimental conditions).
For tests involving multiple comparisons across three or more groups, we first used a Lilliefors test (α = 0.05) to determine if the sample distributions were normally distributed. If the null hypothesis of normality was rejected for one or more sample distribution(s), we performed a Kruskal–Wallis one-way analysis of variance. Otherwise, we performed standard one-way ANOVA. Post-hoc hypothesis testing was performed using the Bonferroni correction to control the familywise error rate in order to determine which pairs were significantly different. We used t-tests if sample distributions were Gaussian and Mann–Whitney U tests otherwise. Adjusted p-values are reported in figure captions and the text.
In vitro procedures
Cell culture
Request a detailed protocolWhole neocortex was isolated from embryonic day 18 (E18) rats in accordance with the National Research Council's Guide for the care and use of laboratory animals using a protocol approved by the Georgia Tech IACUC. Cortical tissue was digested in 20 U ml−1 papain (Sigma-Aldrich, St. Louis, MO) diluted in a culturing medium described in (Jimbo et al., 1999), but without antibiotics or antimycotics. Following enzymatic digestion, cells were dissociated mechanically using 3 to 5 passes through a 1 ml conical pipette tip, and diluted to 2500 cells μl−1. MEAs with 200 μm electrode spacing, 30 μm electrode diameter (Multichannel Systems, Reutlingen, Germany) were sterilized using 70% ethanol and exposure to UV light, and precoated with laminin. Approximately 50,000 cells in a 20 μl drop were plated onto a ∼2 mm diameter area over the array, resulting in ∼2500 cells mm−2 on the culturing surface. The culturing well of each MEA was sealed with a fluorinated ethylene-propylene membrane (Potter and DeMarse, 2001). Experiments and culture storage were carried out in an incubator regulated to 35°C, 5% CO2, 65% relative humidity. The details of our culturing methods are described in (Hales et al., 2010). All experiments were carried out on cultures that were 3–4 weeks old.
Viral transfections
Request a detailed protocolConcentrated aliquots of AAV2-CaMKllα-hChR2(H134R)-mCherry, AAV2-CaMKllα-eNpHR3.0-eYFP, and AAV2-hSyn-eArch3.0-eYFP were produced by the University of Carolina Chapel Hill Vector Core using DNA provided by Karl Deisseroth (Stanford University). When cultures reached 1 to 5 days in vitro (DIV), viral aliquots were diluted to 1×1012 c.f.u. ml−1 and 1 μl was added to 1 ml culturing medium. Infected cultures were incubated for 3 days with the viral solution before the culturing medium was exchanged. To evaluate this protocol, we monitored the fluorescent signal of hChR2(H134R)-mCherry's reporter protein in 3 sister cultures over the days post infection using an LSM510 confocal microscope (Carl Zeiss AG, Oberkochen, Germany). Identical laser power and imaging settings were used for each imaging session. The fluorescent signal increased monotonically before plateauing at ∼3 weeks in vitro (Figure 1—figure supplement 2A,B). Additionally, the functional reactivity of the cultures to 465 nm and 590 nm optical stimuli was probed in the weeks following infection (Figure 1—figure supplement 2C). The ability of ChR2R, eNpHR3.0, and Arch3.0 to affect network firing levels mirrored the expression time course of the marker proteins.
Multichannel electrophysiology
Request a detailed protocolMicroelectrode voltages were amplified and bandpass filtered between 1 Hz and 5 kHz using a 60 channel MEA60 analog amplifier (Multichannel Systems, Reutlingen, Germany). When stored in a 35°C incubator, the temperature of the amplifier exceeded 37°C. Therefore, the culture was regulated to 35°C using a servo-controlled (Modular One Technology, Parker, TX) custom solid state Peltier cooler mounted below the recording amplifier (Figure 1A). Analog signals were digitized and processed by the NeuroRighter multichannel electrophysiology platform (https://sites.google.com/site/neurorighter/) (Newman et al., 2013). Amplified electrode voltages were digitally filtered using a third order Butterworth filter with a passband of 300–5000 Hz. Extracellular action potentials were detected using a voltage threshold of 5 times the RMS noise on each electrode. A spike classifier was trained for each channel by collecting a set of spike waveforms, projecting them into their first two principal components, and fitting a mixture of K Gaussians to the resulting 2D sample distribution using expectation maximization. K was deduced automatically using a minimum description length cost function. Following training, spikes were classified online with a maximal latency of ∼5 ms. The details of NeuroRighter's spike detection/sorting algorithms are presented elsewhere (Newman et al., 2013). We note that, given the simplicity of our controllers, they could be easily implemented on any multichannel electrophysiology system capable of rapid, real-time feedback (e.g., open-ephys, http://open-ephys.org/ or Tucker–Davis Technologies bioprocessors, http://www.tdt.com/).
Optical stimulator
Request a detailed protocolTo deliver optical stimuli, we used a custom LED driver (http://www.open-ephys.org/cyclops/) to control a single blue LED (LZ4-00B200, LEDEngin, San Jose, CA) and 3 amber LEDs wired in series (LZ4-00A100, LEDEngin). LEDs were butt-coupled to a 4-to-1 randomized fiber bundle (Schott AG, Mainz, Germany), which then fed light into a custom Köhler illumination train mounted beneath the MEA amplifier (Figure 1A and Figure 1—figure supplement 1). We confirmed the spatial homogeneity of light at the culturing surface using a BC106-VIS CCD-based beam profiler (Thorlabs, Newton, NJ; Figure 1—figure supplement 1B). Because the blue LED was used to deliver complex temporal waveforms (Figure 4), we used optical feedback to completely linearize the relationship between the control signal and the LED's radiant intensity (Tchumatchenko et al., 2013). Scattered 465 nm light was sampled using a PDA36 amplified photodiode (Thorlabs) fitted with a FB450-40 450 ± 22 nm FWHM optical bandpass filter (Thorlabs) mounted on the Kohler illuminator optical cage (Figure 1A). Amplified light power measurements were fed back to the LED driver to linearize the relationship between the control voltage, provided by NeuroRighter, and optical power. Yellow LEDs were driven using a second driver in a constant-current configuration. The static control signal to irradiance relationship for both light sources are shown in Figure 1—figure supplement 1A,B. A full design specification for the LED driver is available online (https://github.com/jonnew/cyclops).
Pharmacology
Request a detailed protocolEach MEA culturing well contained 1.5 ml of culturing medium. To administer synaptic blockers, 100 μl of culturing medium was transferred from the well to a 0.5 ml centrifuge tube and mixed with either 3 μl of 10 mM CNQX (6-cyano-7-nitroquinoxaline-2,3-dione), 3 μl of 25 mM AP5 (amino-5-phosphonovaleric acid), or 3 μl of 10 mM bicuculline. The resulting mixture was returned to the culturing well and pipetted up and down 5 times to arrive at a final concentration of 20 μM CNQX, 50 μM AP5, or 20 μM bicuculline.
Functional expression
Request a detailed protocolTo characterize the ability of ChR2R to increase population activity, we scanned three parameters of ChR2R excitation in open-loop: 0.1–5 ms pulse width, 1–40 Hz stimulation frequency, and 0.1–1.5 Amps through a 465 ± 11 nm FWHM LED, which corresponds to 1.6–13.4 mW mm−2 at the culturing surface in our configuration (Figure 1—figure supplement 1A). We found that all three parameters provided smooth, positive, monotonic relationship with the average population firing rate at any point approximately 1 week after viral transduction, and that the functional ability of ChR2R co-varied with its expression time-course (Figure 1—figure supplement 2C). Therefore, we used a single control variable, called UC, to simultaneously modulate the pulse-width, stimulation frequency, and optical power of 465 nm stimulation (Equations 7–9).
To characterize the ability of eNpHR3.0 to decrease population firing, we delivered 30 s long stimulus pulses ranging from 0 to 1 Amp to three 590 ± 10 nm FWHM LEDs wired in series, throughout development. These LED currents corresponded to ∼1.3–10.8 mW mm−2 in our configuration (Figure 1—figure supplement 1B). We observed a negative, monotonic relationship between the optical power of the LED and population firing throughout development. Therefore, we defined a control input UeNpHR as the forward diode current of the 590 nm LED (Equation 10).
Feedback controllers
Request a detailed protocolOptogenetic feedback control was implemented using the NeuroRighter plug-in interface, which allows on-the-fly access to NeuroRighter's data streams to user written plug-in code (Newman et al., 2013). Every dt = 4 ms, the average network firing rate, f[t] was calculated using action potentials produced by sorted units and passed through a first-order averaging filter,
where is the instantaneous firing rate during the 4 ms bin, averaged across all detected units and the weighting factor,
is defined using a τ = 2.5 s time constant. The firing rate was then compared to a desired firing rate, f* (setpoint), and the error between the two,
was used to generate stimulus signals using either a PI or on-off control scheme. The PI controller was defined in a recursive form as,
where K = 0.1 is the proportional gain, Ti = 1 s is the integral time constant, and Ts = 0.01 s is the period of the control loop. u[t] was then transformed into optical stimulus signals according to,
Δ1,2 = 0.25 determine the degree of overlap in ChR2R and eNpHR3.0 activation, respectively. UC, the control variable for ChR2R, was transformed into pulses of blue light according to,
UH, the control variable for eNpHR3.0, was transformed into continuously modulated yellow light according to,
UC and UH were bounded between 0 and 1 to prevent integral windup and unreasonably high stimulation intensities. 20 s prior to the start of each 60-s control epoch a 10-s train of UC = 1.0 stimuli was applied, which we found increased control stability by preventing oscillations at the start of the control epoch. This conditioning stimulus train is referred to as a ‘pre-pulse’ in Figure 3. If oscillations persisted, we increased Ti from its nominal value of 1 s until the controller stabilized (Figure 2—figure supplement 4). The largest value of Ti required for to achieve stable control was 10 s.
The excitatory on-off controller was defined as,
where and If[t] is the integrated error signal and a maximal stimulation frequency of 10 Hz was enforced. The inhibitory on-off controller was defined as,
where 1.0 A was delivered to the 590 nm LEDs during the ‘on’ phase, producing ∼11.8 mW mm−2 at the MEA.
In vivo procedures
Experimental preparation
Request a detailed protocolAll procedures were approved by the Georgia Institute of Technology Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee and followed guidelines established by the National Institutes of Health. Female sprague-dawley rats (250–300 g) underwent an initial survival surgery, during which the viral vector (AAV2-CaMKllα-hChR2(H134R)-mCherry, UNC Viral Vector Core, Chapel Hill, NC) was delivered to the left thalamus using stereotactic coordinates targeting the ventral posteriomedial nucleus. The viral syringe (Neuros Syringe, Hamilton Laboratory Products, Reno, NV) was slowly lowered to depth (approximately 2 mm/min) where 1 μl of viral vector solution was delivered at 0.2 μl/min. The injection was allowed to sit for 5 min before slowly retracting the syringe to prevent movement of the virus away from the target location. The animals recovered for 3–4 weeks, providing time for ChR2R expression to reach functional levels.
In a second acute surgery, the rodents were initially anesthetized with 4% isoflurane before transitioning to intravenous administration of fentanyl cocktail (5 μg/kg fentanyl, 150 μg/kg dexmedotodomine, 2 mg/kg midazolam) through the tail vein. Throughout the experiment, measurements of the heart rate, respiratory rate, oxygen saturation, and response to toe pinch stimuli were used to monitor and titrate the depth of anesthesia. Body temperature was maintained at 37°C by a servo-controlled heating blanket (FHC, Bowdoinham, ME). Animals were mounted in a stereotactic frame and a craniotomy was performed over the left parietal cortex to allow access to the ventral postero-medial (VPm) region of the thalamus (coordinates: 2–4 mm posterior to bregma, 2.5–3.5 mm lateral to midline, 4.5–5.5 mm below cortical surface).
Electrophysiology
Request a detailed protocolThe ‘optrode’ consisted of a multimode optical fiber (105 μm core diameter, 125 μm coating diameter, 0.22 NA, Thorlabs, Newton, NJ) and one tungsten microelectrode (75 μm diameter, FHC). The microelectrodes had an impedance of 1–2 MΩ at 1 kHz. The optical fiber was ground to a fine point, producing a spherical, rather than conical, pattern of light delivery. The optrode was advanced to the ventral posterio-medial (VPm) region of the thalamus using a precision microdrive (Knopf Instruments, Tujunga, CA). Single and multi-unit activity were band-pass filtered between 300 and 5000 Hz and digitized at 24.414 kHz using an RZ2 multichannel bioacquisition system (Tucker Davis Technologies, Alachua, FL). The principal vibrissa was determined by manually deflecting individual whiskers and observing resultant multi-unit activity.
Whisker stimulation
Request a detailed protocolWhiskers were trimmed at approximately 12 mm from the face, and were inserted into a custom attachment of a feedback-controlled galvo motor (range of motion, ± 20°; bandwidth, 250 Hz; Cambridge Technology) positioned 10 mm from the vibrissa pad. Vibrissae were always deflected in the rostral–caudal plane. Punctate deflections consisted of exponential rising and falling phases (total excursion, 8°; 99% rise and fall times, 5 ms; average angular velocity, 1600° s−1).
Closed-loop optical stimulation
Request a detailed protocolWe drove a 470 nm high power LED (LBW5SN, OSRAM Opto Semiconductors GmbH, Regensburg, Germany) using our custom LED driver in optical feedback mode. The LED was butt-coupled to a 125 μM diameter optical fiber (Thorlabs) which was used to deliver light to the VPm thalamus and stimulate the ChR2R-expressing cells. Because we used optical feedback to drive the LED, the control output, u[t], was converted directly into a continuously varying light intensity,
where G is a gain parameter that accounts for loss in the coupling from the LED die to the fiber and tuning of the driver circuit. G was measured for each fiber used (Figure 1—figure supplement 1D). The controller was implemented on the Tucker–Davis RZ2's digital signal processors using the RPvdsEx graphical programming language. The in-vivo control loop was equivalent to Equations 1–4 with the exception that τ = 0.8 s and the proportional component was set to zero,
References
-
Closed-loop cortical neuromodulation in Parkinson's disease: an alternative to deep brain stimulation?Clinical Neurophysiology 125:874–885.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2014.01.006
-
Synchrony in sensationCurrent Opinion in Neurobiology 21:701–708.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2011.06.003
-
Physiological effects of sustained blockade of excitatory synaptic transmission on spontaneously active developing neuronal networks—an inquiry into the reciprocal linkage between intrinsic biorhythms and neuroplasticity in early ontogenyNeuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 26:127–185.https://doi.org/10.1016/S0149-7634(01)00062-8
-
A closed-loop human simulator for investigating the role of feedback control in brain-machine interfacesJournal of Neurophysiology 105:1932–1949.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00503.2010
-
Preferential inactivation of Scn1a in parvalbumin interneurons increases seizure susceptibilityNeurobiology of Disease 49:211–220.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nbd.2012.08.012
-
Upward synaptic scaling is dependent on neurotransmission rather than spikingNature Communications 6:6339.https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms7339
-
Modulation of receptive field properties of thalamic somatosensory neurons by the depth of anesthesiaJournal of Neurophysiology 81:2243–2252.
-
Primate motor cortex and free arm movements to visual targets in three-dimensional space. II. Coding of the direction of movement by a neuronal populationThe Journal of Neuroscience 8:2928–2937.
-
eNpHR: a Natronomonas halorhodopsin enhanced for optogenetic applicationsBrain Cell Biology 36:129–139.https://doi.org/10.1007/s11068-008-9027-6
-
How to culture, record and stimulate neuronal networks on micro-electrode arraysJournal of Visualized Experiments 39:e2056.https://doi.org/10.3791/2056
-
Reduced inhibition of dentate granule cells in a model of temporal lobe epilepsyThe Journal of Neuroscience 23:2440–2452.
-
On-demand optogenetic control of spontaneous seizures in temporal lobe epilepsyNature Communications 4:1376.https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms2376
-
Firing rate control of a neuron using a linear proportional-integral controllerJournal of Neural Engineering 7:066004.https://doi.org/10.1088/1741-2560/7/6/066004
-
Neural coding during active somatosensation revealed using illusory touchNature Neuroscience 16:958–965.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3419
-
Time series analysis of impulse sequences of thalamic somatic sensory neuronsJournal of Neurophysiology 27:517–545.
-
A new approach to neural cell culture for long-term studiesJournal of Neuroscience Methods 110:17–24.https://doi.org/10.1016/S0165-0270(01)00412-5
-
Optogenetic silencing strategies differ in their effects on inhibitory synaptic transmissionNature Neuroscience 15:1102–1104.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3143
-
Simultaneous cellular-resolution optical perturbation and imaging of place cell firing fieldsNature Neuroscience 17:1816–1824.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3866
-
Responses of barrel cortex neurons in awake rats and effects of urethane anesthesiaExperimental Brain Research 91:259–272.https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00231659
-
Functional properties of parietal visual neurons: radial organization of directionalities within the visual fieldThe Journal of Neuroscience 7:177–191.
-
Stability of thalamocortical synaptic transmission across awake brain statesThe Journal of Neuroscience 29:6851–6859.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5983-08.2009
-
Delivery of continuously-varying stimuli using ChR2Frontiers in Neural Circuits 7:184.https://doi.org/10.3389/fncir.2013.00184
-
Too many cooks? Intrinsic and synaptic homeostatic mechanisms in cortical circuit refinementAnnual Review of Neuroscience 34:89–103.https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-neuro-060909-153238
-
Controlling bursting in cortical cultures with closed-loop multi-electrode stimulationThe Journal of Neuroscience 25:680–688.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4209-04.2005
-
Thalamic synchrony and the adaptive gating of information flow to cortexNature Neuroscience 13:1534–1541.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.2670
-
Complete functional characterization of sensory neurons by system identificationAnnual Review of Neuroscience 29:477–505.https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.29.051605.113024
-
Interpreting neuronal population activity by reconstruction: unified framework with application to hippocampal place cellsJournal of Neurophysiology 1044:1017–1044.
Article and author information
Author details
Funding
National Science Foundation (NSF) (Integrative Graduate Education and Research Traineeship)
- Jonathan P Newman
- Ming-fai Fong
National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (NINDS) (Kirschstein National Research Service Award)
- Clarissa J Whitmire
National Science Foundation (NSF) (Collaborative Research in Com- putational Neuroscience grant IOS-1131948)
- Garrett B Stanley
National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (NINDS) (2R01NS048285)
- Garrett B Stanley
National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (NINDS) (R01NS085447)
- Garrett B Stanley
National Science Foundation (NSF) (Emerging Frontiers in Research and Innovation grant 1238097)
- Steve M Potter
National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (NINDS) (1R01NS079757-01)
- Steve M Potter
National Science Foundation (NSF) (Graduate Research Fellowship)
- Jonathan P Newman
- Ming-fai Fong
- Daniel C Millard
National Institutes of Health (NIH) (Computational Neuroscience Training Grant (1T90DA032466))
- Clarissa J Whitmire
The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.
Acknowledgements
We thank M LaPlaca for providing tissue and JT Shoemaker for performing tissue harvests. JPN, MF, and DCM were each supported by a US National Science Foundation Graduate Research Fellowship. JPN and MF were supported by a US National Science Foundation Integrative Graduate Education and Research Traineeship. CJW is supported by the National Institutes of Health Ruth L Kirschstein National Research Service Award (F31NS089412) and was supported by the National Institute of Health computational neuroscience training grant (1T90DA032466). GBS was supported by US National Science Foundation Collaborative Research in Computational Neuroscience grant IOS-1131948 and US National Institutes of Health National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke grant 2R01NS048285. SMP was supported by US National Science Foundation Emerging Frontiers in Research and Innovation grant 1238097 and US National Institutes of Health National Institute of Neurological Disorders grant 1R01NS079757-01.
Ethics
Animal experimentation: This study was performed in strict accordance with the National Research Council's Guide for the care and use of laboratory animals using protocol A12012 approved by the Georgia Tech IACUC.
Copyright
© 2015, Newman et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
-
- 5,412
- views
-
- 1,057
- downloads
-
- 106
- citations
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.
Download links
Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)
Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)
Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)
Further reading
-
- Cell Biology
- Neuroscience
Blood–brain barrier (BBB) prevents neurotoxins from entering central nervous system. We aimed to establish and characterize an in vitro triple co-culture BBB model consisting of brain endothelial cells hCMEC/D3, astrocytoma U251 cells, and neuroblastoma SH-SY5Y cells. Co-culture of SH-SY5Y and U251 cells markedly enhanced claudin-5 and VE-cadherin expression in hCMEC/D3 cells, accompanied by increased transendothelial electrical resistance and decreased permeability. Conditioned medium (CM) from SH-SY5Y cells (S-CM), U251 cells (U-CM), and co-culture of SH-SY5Y and U251 cells (US-CM) also promoted claudin-5 and VE-cadherin expression. Glial cell line-derived neurotrophic factor (GDNF) levels in S-CM and US-CM were significantly higher than CMs from hCMEC/D3 and U-CM. Both GDNF and US-CM upregulated claudin-5 and VE-cadherin expression, which were attenuated by anti-GDNF antibody and GDNF signaling inhibitors. GDNF increased claudin-5 expression via the PI3K/AKT/FOXO1 and MAPK/ERK pathways. Meanwhile, GDNF promoted VE-cadherin expression by activating PI3K/AKT/ETS1 and MAPK/ERK/ETS1 signaling. The roles of GDNF in BBB integrity were validated using brain-specific Gdnf silencing mice. The developed triple co-culture BBB model was successfully applied to predict BBB permeability. In conclusion, neurons enhance BBB integrity by upregulating claudin-5 and VE-cadherin expression through GDNF secretion and established triple co-culture BBB model may be used to predict drugs’ BBB permeability.
-
- Neuroscience
Relevance-based selectivity and high energy cost are two distinct features of long-term memory (LTM) formation that warrant its default inhibition. Spaced repetition of learning is a highly conserved cognitive mechanism that can lift this inhibition. Here, we questioned how the spacing effect integrates experience selection and energy efficiency at the cellular and molecular levels. We showed in Drosophila that spaced training triggers LTM formation by extending over several hours an increased mitochondrial metabolic activity in neurons of the associative memory center, the mushroom bodies (MBs). We found that this effect is mediated by PKCδ, a member of the so-called ‘novel PKC’ family of enzymes, which uncovers the critical function of PKCδ in neurons as a regulator of mitochondrial metabolism for LTM. Additionally, PKCδ activation and translocation to mitochondria result from LTM-specific dopamine signaling on MB neurons. By bridging experience-dependent neuronal circuit activity with metabolic modulation of memory-encoding neurons, PKCδ signaling binds the cognitive and metabolic constraints underlying LTM formation into a unified gating mechanism.