Drosulfakinin signaling encodes early-life memory for adaptive social plasticity

  1. Jiwon Jeong
  2. Kujin Kwon
  3. Terezia Klaudia Geisseova
  4. Jongbin Lee
  5. Taejoon Kwon  Is a corresponding author
  6. Chunghun Lim  Is a corresponding author
  1. Department of Biological Sciences, Ulsan National Institute of Science and Technology, Republic of Korea
  2. Department of Biomedical Engineering, Ulsan National Institute of Science and Technology, Republic of Korea
  3. Research Center for Cellular Identity, Korea Advanced Institute of Science and Technology, Republic of Korea
  4. Center for Genomic Integrity, Institute for Basic Science, Republic of Korea
  5. Graduate School of Health Science and Technology, Ulsan National Institute of Science and Technology, Republic of Korea
  6. Department of Biological Sciences, Korea Advanced Institute of Science and Technology, Republic of Korea
  7. Graduate School of Stem Cell and Regenerative Biology, Korea Advanced Institute of Science and Technology, Republic of Korea

Abstract

Drosophila establishes social clusters in groups, yet the underlying principles remain poorly understood. Here, we performed a systemic analysis of social network behavior (SNB) that quantifies individual social distance (SD) in a group over time. The SNB assessment in 175 inbred strains from the Drosophila Genetics Reference Panel showed a tight association of short SD with long developmental time, low food intake, and hypoactivity. The developmental inferiority in short-SD individuals was compensated by their group culturing. By contrast, developmental isolation silenced the beneficial effects of social interactions in adults and blunted the plasticity of SNB under physiological challenges. Transcriptome analyses revealed genetic diversity for SD traits, whereas social isolation reprogrammed select genetic pathways, regardless of SD phenotypes. In particular, social deprivation suppressed the expression of the neuropeptide Drosulfakinin (Dsk) in three pairs of adult brain neurons. Male-specific DSK signaling to cholecystokinin-like receptor 17D1 mediated the SNB plasticity. In fact, transgenic manipulations of the DSK neuron activity were sufficient to imitate the state of social experience. Given the functional conservation of mammalian Dsk homologs, we propose that animals may have evolved a dedicated neural mechanism to encode early-life experience and transform group properties adaptively.

Editor's evaluation

This study presents important findings on the role of Drosulfakinin signaling in encoding early-life social memory in Drosophila, which influences adaptive social plasticity in adulthood. The research demonstrates the neurogenetic basis of social clustering and behavioral adaptation, advancing our understanding of social behavior's molecular and evolutionary bases. The evidence is solid, given the robust genetic, behavioral, and transcriptomic analyses.

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.103973.sa0

Introduction

Animals interact with other individuals in distinct social environments (Clutton-Brock, 2021; Jezovit et al., 2021; Sokolowski, 2010). For instance, a pair of animals display aggression or mating behaviors, whereas a group of individuals may show collective behaviors through intricate networks of social interactions. Such social network behaviors (SNBs) are believed to enhance group fitness and are conserved across many species, underscoring their evolutionary significance (Sokolowski, 2010; Blumstein et al., 2010). Additionally, social interactions influence various physiological activities in individuals, including feeding, sleep/circadian rhythms, aggression, stress response, and longevity (Levine et al., 2002; Ganguly-Fitzgerald et al., 2006; Arzate-Mejía et al., 2020; Li et al., 2021; Chen and Sokolowski, 2022; Vora et al., 2022; Xiong et al., 2023). Nonetheless, it remains elusive how group properties have evolved with other individual traits and how animals process social experiences to shape their behavior and physiology.

Drosophila has long been considered a solitary species. Yet a group of flies can display distinct SNB under specific experimental conditions (Schneider et al., 2012; Simon et al., 2012; Ramdya et al., 2015; Dombrovski et al., 2017; Sun et al., 2020; Burg et al., 2013), serving as an ideal genetic model to address our questions above (Jezovit et al., 2021). Previous studies have employed various biophysical parameters to quantify both individual and network behaviors in groups, establishing criteria for social interactions in Drosophila (Jezovit et al., 2021; Simon et al., 2012; Ramdya et al., 2015; Bentzur et al., 2021). While these measurements give a comprehensive view of group behaviors, our study focuses on the clustering property of social interactions within a group (Simon et al., 2012; Jiang et al., 2020). Social clustering is an intuitive measure that integrates diverse social interactions via multiple sensory cues (Schneider et al., 2012; Simon et al., 2012; Bentzur et al., 2021; Jiang et al., 2020), accompanying reductions in social distance (SD) among group members and their moving speed over time. We also reasoned that simple SD assessment could facilitate the alignment of large-scale datasets of group behaviors with physiological traits, differential gene expression, and neurogenetic manipulations.

These approaches lead to our demonstration that the group property for high social clustering (i.e., short SD) is closely associated with and compensates for inferior developmental traits in individuals. Moreover, Drosophila can adjust their social preferences according to physiological changes, indicating the adaptive plasticity of SNB. This social plasticity requires early-life social experiences that persist throughout development. We also define a specific neuropeptide signaling pathway that encodes social memory and supports SNB plasticity. Our findings thus provide new insights into the principles of SNB, offering a deeper understanding of the evolutionary and genetic bases of social behaviors in Drosophila and possibly other species.

Results

SNB is a quantitative trait in a natural Drosophila population

We employed the Drosophila Genetics Reference Panel (DGRP) to determine whether SNB has evolved with specific genetic factors and physiology. The DGRP consists of approximately 200 inbred wild-type strains and functions as a practical genetic library to explore the correlation between naturally occurring genetic variations and complex animal behaviors (Mackay et al., 2012; Mackay and Huang, 2018; Gardeux et al., 2024). We video-recorded a group of 16 male flies freely moving in an open arena for 10 min and quantitatively assessed their group properties over time (Figure 1A). These included SD between individual group members (Figure 1B), walking speed, and the centroid velocity of a given group. The DGRP lines displayed a range of distributions for the three parameters (Figure 1C, Figure 1—source data 1), and we found their significant correlations among 175 DGRP lines (Figure 1D, Figure 1—source data 1; Spearman correlation analysis). For instance, short-SD lines gradually reduced SD and walking speed over time to stay in the cluster, thereby exhibiting short travel distances, slow walking speeds, and low centroid velocities on average (e.g., DGRP73, DGRP 563, and DGRP370) (Figure 2A, Figure 2—figure supplement 1, Figure 2—source data 1, Video 1). By contrast, long-SD lines persistently explored the arena during video recording and sustained ‘social distancing’ to display long travel distances, fast walking speeds, and high centroid velocities (e.g., DGRP360, DGRP707, and DGRP317) (Figure 2B, Figure 2—figure supplement 1, Figure 2—source data 1, Video 2), although their locomotion was modestly slowed down over time. The locomotion trajectories of individual flies confirmed these characteristics (Figure 2C and D, Figure 2—source data 1), and long-SD individuals traveled much longer distances than short-SD individuals across the DGRP lines (Figure 2—figure supplement 1C, Figure 2—source data 1). Short-SD flies did not significantly change their locomotor activity over time when we placed a single fly in the same arena and assessed its behavior (Figure 2—figure supplement 2, Figure 2—source data 1). Thus, reduced activity in a group of short-SD flies is likely an effect of their clustering phenotypes but not necessarily the cause. Short- and long-SD flies retained their clustering properties even in a larger arena (Figure 2—figure supplement 3, Videos 3 and 4; 8.5 cm in diameter), indicating active social preferences that persist in different environments. Both types of DGRP lines displayed no chaining behaviors in the open arena, excluding the possible implication of male-to-male courtship in their SD phenotypes (Figure 2—figure supplement 4, Figure 2—source data 1). These results provide convincing evidence that SD is an inheritable group trait from natural Drosophila variants. Based on the average ranking of individual DGRP lines in the two group properties (i.e., SD and centroid velocity), we selected the top and bottom three DGRP lines representing short- and long-SD phenotypes, respectively, to elucidate the physiological significance of Drosophila SNB and its underlying mechanisms.

Social network behavior (SNB) is a quantitative trait in a natural Drosophila population.

(A) The 10 min video recording of SNB in a group of 16 male flies. Representative snapshot images at each quarter time point (Q1, Q2, Q3, and Q4) were obtained from Drosophila strains with high (top, short social distance [SD]) or low clustering properties (bottom, long SD). (B) The definition of SD. SD was measured in each fly over the 10 min recording and averaged from a given group. Representative SD dynamics from short- (top) or long-SD strains (bottom) were shown. Dotted lines, group-averaged SD over the 10 min recordings. (C) Quantitative assessment of SNB by ranking SD, walking speed, and centroid velocity among 175 DGRP lines. Data represent means ± SEM (n = 5). (D) Significant correlation among SD, walking speed, and centroid velocity. ***p<0.001, as determined by Spearman correlation analysis. Red, representative short-SD lines; blue, representative long-SD lines.

Figure 1—source data 1

Correlation of SD, walking speed, and centroid velocity across 175 DGRP lines.

https://cdn.elifesciences.org/articles/103973/elife-103973-fig1-data1-v2.xlsx
Figure 2 with 4 supplements see all
Short- and long-social distance (SD) lines exhibit distinct social network behavior (SNB).

(A, B) SNB dynamics in short-SD (A) and long-SD lines (B). SD dynamics in a representative group of 16 male flies over the 10 min recordings (left, n = 16), quarter-averaged SD (middle, n = 8), and quarter-averaged walking speed (right, n = 8) were shown for each Drosophila Genetics Reference Panel (DGRP) line. Error bars indicate SEM. n.s., not significant; *p<0.05, **p<0.01, ***p<0.001 as determined by paired t-test (DGRP73, DGRP563, DGRP370, DGRP707, and DGRP317 for quarter-averaged SD; DGRP563, DGRP360, and DGRP317 for quarter-averaged walking speed) or Wilcoxon matched-pairs signed rank test (DGRP360 for quarter-averaged SD; DGRP73, DGRP370, and DGRP707 for quarter-averaged walking speed). (C) The 10 min locomotion trajectories of representative individual flies from short- (red) or long-SD lines (blue). (D) Cumulative travel distances of individual flies over the 10 min recording. Colored lines represent means (n = 128).

Figure 2—source data 1

Quantitative locomotor metrics in short- and long-SD DGRP lines.

https://cdn.elifesciences.org/articles/103973/elife-103973-fig2-data1-v2.xlsx
Video 1
Social network behavior (SNB) in DGRP73 (grp+ctrl).
Video 2
Social network behavior (SNB) in DGRP360 (grp+ctrl).
Video 3
Social network behavior (SNB) in DGRP73 (large arena).
Video 4
Social network behavior (SNB) in DGRP360 (large arena).

Social interactions compensate for developmental inferiority in short-SD larvae

Why do flies display SNB? One clue comes from the previous observation that Drosophila larvae collectively dig culture media and improve food accessibility, possibly facilitating their constitutive feeding during early development (Dombrovski et al., 2017). In fact, we found that the short-SD lines had higher numbers of larvae per cluster than the long-SD lines (Figure 3A, Figure 3—source data 1). These observations suggest that Drosophila express SNB traits from early development, and the sociality persists in adults. To better define the social-interaction effects on Drosophila physiology, we obtained socially enriched or deprived larvae from fertilized eggs (Figure 3B) and then compared their developmental phenotypes among DGRP lines. Social isolation substantially impaired larval activity and development in both short- and long-SD lines (Figure 3C–G, Figure 3—source data 1). We further found that the short-SD trait is tightly associated with inferior phenotypes, particularly in isolated larvae. For instance, socially isolated larvae from the short-SD lines displayed poor digging activity (Figure 3C, Figure 3—source data 1), low food intake (Figure 3D, Figure 3—source data 1), and long developmental time (Figure 3E, Figure 3—source data 1) compared to those from the long-SD lines. Short-SD larvae also had lower eclosion success (Figure 3F, Figure 3—source data 1) and a lower ratio of the adult male progeny (Figure 3G, Figure 3—source data 1) than long-SD larvae, although isolated cultures reduced the eclosion success regardless of the SD trait. Grouping of short-SD larvae blunted or partially rescued these phenotypes (i.e., digging activity, developmental time, eclosion success, and male progeny ratio). Our group culture was not a competitive environment for limited resources to delay developmental time (Horváth and Kalinka, 2016; Klepsatel et al., 2018), but it actually promoted food intake comparably in short- and long-SD larvae (Figure 3D, Figure 3—source data 1). The significant interaction effects of SD (i.e., short- vs. long-SD trait) and socialization (i.e., grouped vs. isolated cultures) on digging activity, developmental time, and male progeny ratio suggest that the clustering property of short-SD lines may have evolved as a compensation mechanism for the developmental inferiority in individuals. Considering that a low percentage of male likely limits mating choice, reproductive efficiency, and genetic diversity in a given group, social interactions may also contribute to the group fitness in short-SD lines across generations. We speculate that the feeding amount of isolated long-SD individuals is saturating for normal development (e.g., developmental time, male progeny ratio), possibly explaining the lack of interaction effects on food intake while displaying developmental inferiorities most evidently in isolated short-SD individuals.

Early-life social experience confers beneficial effects on Drosophila development.

(A) Larval clustering in short- (red) or long-social distance (SD) lines (blue). % clusters were calculated from 30 vials. Arrows indicate individual larvae. Scale bar = 0.5 mm. (B) Schematic for grouped (grp) vs. developmentally isolated (iso) culture conditions. (C, D) Grouped culture compensated for low food accessibility in the short-SD larvae. Aligned ranks transformation ANOVA detected significant effects of SD trait and social isolation on both digging depth (C, p<0.0001) and food intake (D, p<0.0001), and their significant interaction effects were detected only on digging depth (C, p=0.0338). Data represent means ± SEM (n = 36; 12 per line × 3 lines). ***p<0.001, as determined by Wilcoxon rank sum test. (E–G) Grouped culture rescued developmental delay and low male-progeny ratio in the short-SD larvae. Aligned ranks transformation ANOVA or ordinary two-way ANOVA detected significant effects of SD trait and social isolation on developmental time (E, p<0.0001), eclosion success (F, p=0.0116 for SD trait; p<0.0001 for social isolation), and male progeny ratio (G, p<0.0001). Significant interaction effects between SD trait and social isolation were also detected on developmental time (E, p<0.0001) and male progeny ratio (G, p<0.0001). Data represent means ± SEM (n = 24; 8 per line × 3 lines). n.s., not significant; *p<0.05, ***p<0.001, as determined by Wilcoxon rank sum test (developmental time and male progeny ratio) or Tukey’s multiple comparisons test (eclosion success).

Figure 3—source data 1

Quantitative analysis of larval SNB and developmental metrics.

https://cdn.elifesciences.org/articles/103973/elife-103973-fig3-data1-v2.xlsx

Early-life experience is necessary for social benefits on adult physiology and adaptive social plasticity

We further asked whether social interactions also benefit adult physiology in the short-SD lines. To this end, we designed a maze experiment where a group of flies were placed in a novel arena to determine how fast they could reach a food resource in the presence or absence of pretrained flies (Figure 4A). Our prediction was that social interactions between naïve and trained flies might facilitate their food-seeking, possibly mimicking social foraging in other species (Giraldeau and Caraco, 2000). We first confirmed that both short- and long-SD lines significantly shortened latency to the arrival of 75% of flies on food by iterative exposures to the maze (Figure 4B, Figure 4—figure supplement 1, Figure 4—source data 1). Representative plasticity mutants of the rutabaga (rut) gene did not significantly shorten the arrival latency after three consecutive training sessions in our maze paradigm (Figure 4—figure supplement 2A, Figure 4—source data 1), implicating rut-dependent learning and memory in this process (Levin et al., 1992). The short-SD lines displayed poor performance in the maze assay as assessed by longer latency to the arrival of 75% naïve flies on food than the long-SD lines (Figure 4B, Figure 4—figure supplement 1, Figure 4—source data 1). Combining the trained ‘pioneers’ with a group of naïve flies significantly improved the group property of food-seeking behaviors in both short- and long-SD lines (Figure 4B, Figure 4—figure supplement 1, Figure 4—source data 1). The pioneer effects disappeared when the naïve group consisted of socially isolated individuals from egg development. These results support that social foraging in our maze paradigm is unlikely a simple collective response in adults, but it specifically requires early-life social experience. Of note, social deprivation effects on pioneer-free group foraging somewhat varied across the long SD lines (Figure 4B, Figure 4—figure supplement 1, Figure 4—source data 1). We reason that the hyperactivity of individual long-SD flies facilitates their food-seeking behaviors in the maze, weakening the pioneer effects or even overriding the group property.

Figure 4 with 5 supplements see all
Early-life social experience confers beneficial effects on social foraging in adult Drosophila.

(A) Experimental scheme for assessing social interactions in a maze assay. Representative images were shown for a group of Drosophila strains with high (short latency) or low social foraging (long latency). (B) Pioneer groups of flies from either grouped (grp) or isolated cultures (iso) were effectively trained in the maze assay, but social isolation of both short- (red, DGRP73) and long-SD flies (blue, DGRP360) blunted the pioneer effects on food-seeking behaviors in a group of naive flies. Two-way repeated measures ANOVA detected significant effects of training (p<0.0001 for DGRP73 and DGRP360) but not social isolation on latency during the training session of pioneer groups. Ordinary two-way ANOVA also detected significant interaction effects of pioneer and social isolation on latency during the maze test (p=0.0131 for DGRP73; p=0.0310 for DGRP360). Data represent means ± SEM (n = 6‒16). n.s., not significant; *p<0.05, **p<0.01, ***p<0.001, as determined by Sidak’s multiple comparisons test (training session) or Tukey’s multiple comparisons test (maze test). (C) Experimental scheme for assessing injury-induced social network behavior (SNB) plasticity. GTI, grouped-to-isolated culture transition; ITG, isolated-to-grouped culture transition. (D) Physical injury induced clustering behaviors in group-cultured but not developmentally isolated flies. Data represent means ± SEM (n = 8). n.s., not significant; *p<0.05, **p<0.01, ***p<0.001, as determined by one-way ANOVA with Tukey’s multiple comparisons test.

Figure 4—source data 1

Quantitative analysis of adult SNB and social plasticity.

https://cdn.elifesciences.org/articles/103973/elife-103973-fig4-data1-v2.xlsx

The long-SD lines outcompeted the short-SD lines in both larval and adult assays, and the beneficial effects of their social experience were barely detectable. SNB in the long-SD adults was also insensitive to early-life experience, whereas the grouped culture promoted SNB in the short-SD lines (Bentzur et al., 2021; Figure 2—figure supplement 1B, Figure 2—source data 1). We hypothesized that superior traits in long-SD individuals led to their genetic selection toward the degeneration of social interaction effects. Alternatively, long-SD genomes may still encode genetic programs for social activity, but they only express social traits under physiologically challenging conditions. Given the positive correlation between locomotion activity and SD trait among DGRP lines (Figure 1D, Figure 1—source data 1), we reasoned that modest injury could serve as a physiological cue to reduce locomotor activity in individuals, facilitate their interactions in a group, and induce SNB even in the long-SD flies. We indeed discovered that mechanical injury shortened SD in both sociality types of DGRP lines (Figure 4C and D, Figure 4—figure supplement 3A, Figure 4—source data 1; Videos 5 and 6;) while reducing walking speed and centroid velocity only in the long-SD lines (Figure 4—figure supplement 4, Figure 4—source data 1). The injury effects were transient because 1 week of recovery was sufficient to restore the original SD traits (Figure 4D, Figure 4—figure supplement 3A, Figure 4—source data 1). The injury-induced plasticity of SNB and locomotor activity was not detectable in a group of socially isolated flies (Figure 4D, Figure 4—figure supplements 3A and 4, Figure 4—source data 1, Videos 710). We thus reason that the mechanical injury does not severely impair general locomotion per se to abolish or overestimate SNB under our experimental conditions, but low activity in grouped long-SD flies is likely a consequence of their injury-induced clustering. The injury-induced SNB plasticity required no receptor potential A (norpA) among sensory pathway genes tested (Figure 4—figure supplement 5, Figure 4—source data 1), indicating a crucial role of norpA-dependent visual sensing. It is consistent with the previous finding that vision is required for larval clustering behaviors in Drosophila (Dombrovski et al., 2017). We further found that an adult-specific grouping of developmentally isolated flies was insufficient to support the social plasticity (Figure 4D, Figure 4—figure supplement 3A, Figure 4—source data 1). Finally, the rut-dependent memory pathway seems dispensable for developmental ‘social memory’ since rut mutants displayed injury-induced social plasticity comparable to control flies (Figure 4—figure supplement 2B, Figure 4—source data 1). These results suggest that individual Drosophila strains differentially display social traits in adults; however, the potency of adaptive social plasticity is acquired through their early-life experience of social interactions.

Video 5
Social network behavior (SNB) in DGRP73 (grp+inj).
Video 6
Social network behavior (SNB) in DGRP360 (grp+inj).
Video 7
Social network behavior (SNB) in DGRP73 (iso+ctrl).
Video 8
Social network behavior (SNB) in DGRP360 (iso+ctrl).
Video 9
Social network behavior (SNB) in DGRP73 (iso+inj).
Video 10
Social network behavior (SNB) in DGRP360 (iso+inj).

Unraveling the genetic basis of SNB and its plasticity

How is early-life social experience encoded in individual larvae to persist throughout development? To define the molecular signatures of social interaction phenotypes and their plasticity, we profiled differentially expressed genes (DEGs) in adult fly heads among distinct contexts of genetic and environmental sociality. The transcriptome analyses revealed significantly upregulated genes in the short- and long-SD lines (n genes = 191 and 199, respectively), as well as in grouped and socially isolated flies (n genes = 159 and 755, respectively) (Figure 5A and B, Figure 5—source data 1,Figure 5—source data 2). Genes upregulated in socially isolated DGRP lines overlapped substantially with those identified in previous studies using independent wild-type strains (Li et al., 2021; Wang et al., 2008; Agrawal et al., 2020; Figure 5—figure supplement 1A, Figure 5—source data 3), whereas upregulated genes in short- or long-SD lines barely overlapped with DEGs between grouped and socially isolated flies (Figure 5—figure supplement 1B, Figure 5—source data 3). Gene Ontology (GO) analyses showed no significant enrichment of specific GO terms in DEGs between the short- and long-SD lines. We thus concluded that diverse genetic pathways shape baseline SD phenotypes, as suggested previously (Schneider et al., 2012). It is also consistent with the lack of the phylogenetic correlation between Drosophila species and their social network phenotypes (Jezovit et al., 2020). By contrast, select metabolic pathways were upregulated in both types of DGRP lines by social isolation (Figure 5C and D, Figure 5—source data 2;Figure 5—source data 4). These observations were consistent with previous findings that chronic social isolation acts as a hunger cue to suppress sleep, induce feeding, and alter metabolic gene expression, including those involved in lipid metabolism (Li et al., 2021; Liu et al., 2018). Considering that the number of commonly upregulated genes in group-cultured flies was very limited, we speculate that Drosophila has evolved a genetic reprogram where social isolation elevates metabolic gene expression to adaptively induce a metabolic shift for energy storage and fitness.

Figure 5 with 2 supplements see all
Social experience shapes gene expression profiles in Drosophila heads.

(A) Heatmaps for differentially expressed genes (DEGs, more than twofold difference with adjusted p<0.05) in short- vs. long-social distance (SD) lines (left); in grouped (grp) vs. isolated (iso) condition (right). Fly heads were harvested from individual Drosophila Genetics Reference Panel (DGRP) lines in grouped or isolated cultures and their gene expression profiles were analyzed by RNA sequencing. Averaged counts per million were converted to z-score for visualization. (B) Volcano plots for differentially expressed genes (DEGs) in short- vs. long-SD lines (top); in grp vs. iso flies (bottom). Social interactions evidently upregulated the neuropeptide Drosulfakinin (Dsk) expression. (C) Overlapping DEGs in grp vs. iso conditions across DGRP lines. Dsk was identified as a commonly upregulated gene by social interactions in short- (top, red) and long-SD lines (middle, blue). (D) Gene Ontology analysis reveals upregulation of select metabolic pathways upon social isolation. False discovery rate (FDR) < 0.05, as determined by Fisher’s exact test (grp vs. iso). (E) Significant phenotypic correlation of SNB to food intake, starvation-induced activity, and mean aggressive encounters among DGRP lines. Raw data for food intake (n = 52 DGRP lines), starvation-induced activity (n = 60 DGRP lines), and aggression (n = 57 DGRP lines) were obtained from previous studies (Garlapow et al., 2015; Shorter et al., 2015; Chi et al., 2021) and then aligned to SD, centroid velocity, and walking speed in the corresponding DGRP lines that were measured by our SNB analyses. *p<0.05, **p<0.01, ***p<0.001, as determined by Spearman correlation analysis. (F) Expression heatmap for genes implicated in feeding, adult locomotor behavior, and aggression. Downregulation of Dsk and its two receptors (CCKLR-17D1 and CCKLR-17D3) by social isolation was visualized in relevant gene categories.

Figure 5—source data 1

Normalized gene expression in individual DGRP lines under grouped vs. isolated culture conditions.

https://cdn.elifesciences.org/articles/103973/elife-103973-fig5-data1-v2.xlsx
Figure 5—source data 2

DEG analyses between distinct social groups (short vs. long SD; or grouped vs. isolated).

https://cdn.elifesciences.org/articles/103973/elife-103973-fig5-data2-v2.xlsx
Figure 5—source data 3

Comparative analyses of social group-specific DEGs from independent studies.

https://cdn.elifesciences.org/articles/103973/elife-103973-fig5-data3-v2.xlsx
Figure 5—source data 4

DEG analyses among individual DGRP lines.

https://cdn.elifesciences.org/articles/103973/elife-103973-fig5-data4-v2.xlsx
Figure 5—source data 5

Correlation of SNB to food intake, starvation-induced activity, and aggression among DGRP lines.

https://cdn.elifesciences.org/articles/103973/elife-103973-fig5-data5-v2.xlsx

Interestingly, the phenotypic alignment of DGRP lines revealed significant correlations of their social interaction behaviors to food intake, starvation-induced activity, and aggression, among others (Garlapow et al., 2015; Shorter et al., 2015; Chi et al., 2021; Figure 5E, Figure 5—source data 5). We validated that the long-SD lines showed more lunges than the short-SD lines, indicative of high aggression behaviors (Figure 5—figure supplement 2, Figure 5—source data 5, Videos 11 and 12). The association of multiple behaviors raised the possibility of their overlapping evolution of regulatory genes and mechanisms. The expression heatmaps of relevant gene categories visualized a subset of genes that indeed displayed social experience-dependent expression across the DGRP lines analyzed (Figure 5F, Figure 5—source data 1). These included Drosulfakinin (Dsk), a neuropeptide implicated in aggression, food intake, satiety, and sexual behaviors (Agrawal et al., 2020; Wu et al., 2019; Wu et al., 2020; Guo et al., 2021; Wang et al., 2022). The mammalian Dsk homolog cholecystokinin (CCK) has also been shown to play a similar role in relevant physiology, suggesting the possible conservation of Dsk function (Nichols et al., 1988; Staljanssens et al., 2011; Nässel and Wu, 2022). In fact, Dsk was the only overlapping gene that was downregulated upon social isolation across independent studies (Li et al., 2021; Wang et al., 2008; Agrawal et al., 2020; Figure 5—figure supplement 1A, Figure 5—source data 3). We also found that social isolation of the DGRP lines significantly downregulated the expression of Dsk and the two CCK-like receptors (i.e., CCKLR-17D1 and CCKLR-17D3), although Dsk receptor gene expression showed less than twofold change (Figure 5F, Figure 5—source data 2). These observations prompted us to ask whether DSK signaling contributes to the plasticity of social behaviors through early-life experience.

Video 11
Aggression behaviors in DGRP73.
Video 12
Aggression behaviors in DGRP360.

DSK neuron activity encodes early-life experience for SNB plasticity

Immunostaining of whole-mount brains identified three groups of DSK-expressing neurons with distinct neuroanatomical morphology (i.e., MP1a, MP1b, and MP3) (Wu et al., 2019; Wu et al., 2020; Figure 6A). The long-SD lines displayed relatively high DSK signals in the cell bodies compared to the short-SD lines (Figure 6B, Figure 6—source data 1). However, DSK levels in the neural projections were comparable between the two groups, raising the possibility that axonal transport or processing of the neuropeptide was limiting under the group-culture condition. Social isolation lowered DSK levels irrespective of the SD phenotype (Figure 6A and B, Figure 6—source data 1), consistent with our DEG analysis above. The social experience effects on DSK levels were most evident in the MP1a neuron projections. Live-brain imaging of the genetically encoded Ca2+ indicator GCaMP showed that DSK neuron activity correlated with social experience and plasticity in a wild-type background. Social deprivation reduced relative Ca2+ levels in DSK neurons, whereas mechanical injury generally elevated DSK neuron activity (Figure 6C, Figure 6—source data 1). The two conditions, however, acted independently on the GCaMP signals in DSK neurons since their interaction effects were not significantly detected. Nonetheless, MP1a neuron activity did not respond to injury when transgenic flies were socially deprived during development, which may contribute to the lack of social behavior plasticity upon isolation (Figure 6C, Figure 6—figure supplement 1, Figure 6—source data 1). These observations are unlikely due to transgenic Dsk-Gal4 activity per se given that social isolation did not comparably affect the Gal4-dependent expression of a dendritic marker transgene in the DSK neurons (Figure 6—figure supplement 1, Figure 6—source data 1). We also confirmed that GCaMP levels in other neuropeptide-expressing neurons (i.e., pigment-dispersing factor) were insensitive to social isolation or mechanical injury (Figure 6—figure supplement 2, Figure 6—source data 1).

Figure 6 with 2 supplements see all
Drosulfakinin (Dsk) neuron activity encodes social experience.

(A, B) Social experience elevates DSK levels in the Drosophila brains. Whole-mount brains from each Drosophila Genetics Reference Panel (DGRP) line in grouped (grp) or socially isolated cultures (iso) were co-immunostained with anti-DSK (green) and anti-BRUCHPILOT antibodies (BRP, a synaptic protein; magenta). The fluorescent DSK signals from confocal images were quantified using ImageJ. Ordinary two-way ANOVA or aligned ranks transformation ANOVA detected significant effects of social isolation on DSK levels in MP1a/MP1b/MP3 cell bodies and their projections (p<0.0001). The social distance (SD) trait effects (i.e., short vs. long SD) were more evident on DSK levels in cell bodies (p<0.0001). Data represent means ± SEM (n = 13). n.s., not significant; **p<0.01, ***p<0.001, as determined by Tukey’s multiple comparisons test (MP1a/MP1b cell bodies and MP1a projections) or Wilcoxon rank sum test (MP3 cell bodies and projections). Scale bar = 40 µm. (C) Social experience and physical injury elevate DSK neuron activity. Social isolation masked an injury-induced Ca2+ increase in the MP1a neurons among other DSK neurons as assessed by the genetically encoded Ca2+ sensor GCaMP in live-brain imaging. Two-way ANOVA detected significant effects of social isolation (p<0.0001) and injury (inj, p<0.01) on GCaMP levels in DSK neurons. Data represent means ± SEM (n = 10). n.s., not significant; *p<0.05, **p<0.01, ***p<0.001, as determined by Tukey’s multiple comparisons test. Scale bar = 5 µm. (D–F) Social experience strengthens postsynaptic signaling of DSK neurons only in males. Whole-mount brains were dissected from group-cultured (grp) or isolated (iso) flies and immunostained with anti-DSK antibody (white). Postsynaptic partner of DSK neurons was visualized by the heterologous ligand-receptor signaling embedded in the transsynaptic mapping transgene (Dsk>trans-Tango, magenta) while DSK neurons were further labeled by the presynaptic marker of the trans-Tango (green). The fluorescent trans-Tango signals and anti-DSK staining intensities from confocal images were quantified using ImageJ. Aligned ranks transformation ANOVA or ordinary 2-way ANOVA detected significant interaction effects of social isolation and gender on trans-Tango signals (E, p<0.0001) and DSK levels in MP1a projection (F, p<0.0001). Data represent means ± SEM (n = 13). n.s., not significant; ***p<0.001, as determined by Wilcoxon rank sum test (trans-Tango signals) or Tukey’s multiple comparisons test (DSK levels). Scale bar = 40 µm.

Figure 6—source data 1

Quantitative analysis of DSK neuron activities under distinct social contexts.

https://cdn.elifesciences.org/articles/103973/elife-103973-fig6-data1-v2.xlsx

We used the transsynaptic mapping transgene trans-Tango to visualize the postsynaptic partner of DSK neurons via heterologous ligand-receptor signaling (Talay et al., 2017; Sorkaç et al., 2023). Socially enriched, but not socially isolated, male flies displayed strong postsynaptic signals of the trans-Tango-expressing DSK neurons in the lateral protocerebrum where transgenic signals of DSK neuron axons (Dsk>SytGFP) were readily detectable (Figure 6D and E, Figure 6—figure supplement 1B, Figure 6—source data 1). Previous studies also demonstrated that MP1a neuron projections are specifically enriched in this brain region (Wu et al., 2019; Wu et al., 2020). DSK neurons have gender-specific presynaptic partners, which may mediate distinct signaling for sexual behaviors (Wu et al., 2019; Wu et al., 2020; Wang et al., 2022). To our surprise, socially enriched female brains did not show trans-Tango signals as evidently as male brains (Figure 6D and E, Figure 6—source data 1). Moreover, social deprivation downregulated DSK levels in the MP1a projections less potently in females than in males (Figure 6D and F, Figure 6—source data 1). These observations suggest sexual dimorphism in social behavior plasticity. Indeed, female flies from select DGRP lines showed baseline SD phenotypes consistent with their male counterparts, yet mechanical injury did not significantly affect female SD (Figure 4—figure supplement 3B, Figure 4—source data 1; also see Figure 7G, Figure 7—source data 1). Considering that DSK-expressing MP1 neurons originate from the larval brain (Oikawa et al., 2023), we hypothesized that early-life experience is developmentally encoded in DSK neuron activity via the male-specific neural pathway and adaptively expressed for social plasticity in adults. We further reason that low food intake upon social isolation may not directly implicate DSK expression or DSK neuron activity since DSK signaling has been shown to suppress feeding behavior as a satiety cue (Wu et al., 2020; Guo et al., 2021; Söderberg et al., 2012; Williams et al., 2014).

Figure 7 with 3 supplements see all
Genetic manipulations of Drosulfakinin (DSK) signaling imitate social experience.

(A) DskattP mutant brain expresses barely detectable DSK peptides. Whole-mount brains from Canton S (CS, a wild-type control) and DskattP mutant flies were co-immunostained with anti-DSK (green) and anti-BRUCHPILOT antibodies (BRP, a synaptic protein; magenta). Scale bar = 40 µm. (B, C) Genetic silencing of DSK-CCKLR-17D1 signaling by genomic deletions (DskattP or CCKLR-17D1attP) or DSK depletion (Dsk >DskRNAi) blocks injury-induced clustering behaviors. Data represent means ± SEM (n = 8). n.s., not significant; **p<0.01, ***p<0.001, as determined by two-way ANOVA with Tukey’s multiple comparisons test. (D, E) Conditional blockade of DSK neuron transmission at larval stage is sufficient to blunt social experience-dependent plasticity of social network behavior (SNB). Transgenic crosses for DSK-specific expression of the temperature-sensitive shibire allele (Dsk >shits) were kept at either restrictive (29°C) or permissive temperature (18°C) for synaptic transmission until the end of larval stage. Two-way ANOVA detected significant interaction effects of injury (inj) and temperature on social distance (SD) in Dsk>shits flies (p=0.0169) but not in heterozygous controls (p=0.7549 for Dsk-Gal4/+; p=0.2030 for shits/+). Data represent means ± SEM (n = 6). n.s., not significant; *p<0.05, **p<0.01, ***p<0.001, as determined by Tukey’s multiple comparisons test. (F) CCKLR-17D1 neurons display sexually dimorphic dendrites around MP1 projections. Transgenic male or female brains (CCKLR-17D1>DenMark + SytGFP) were immunostained with anti-DSK antibody to visualize DSK neuron projections (white) along with dendrites (DenMark, magenta) and axons (SytGFP, green) of neurons expressing CCKLR-17D1-Gal4 knock-in. Scale bar = 40 µm. (G) Transgenic excitation of CCKLR-17D1 neurons confers injury-induced plasticity of SNB in socially isolated males but not females. Two-way ANOVA detected significant interaction effects of injury (inj) and social isolation on SD in male heterozygous controls (p=0.0040 for 17D1-Gal4/+; p=0.0007 for NaChBac/+) but not in all the other genotypes. Data represent means ± SEM (n = 8). n.s., not significant; *p<0.05, **p<0.01, as determined by Tukey’s multiple comparisons test.

Figure 7—source data 1

Quantitative analysis of SNB plasticity in genetic and transgenic Drosophila models for DSK-DSK receptor signaling.

https://cdn.elifesciences.org/articles/103973/elife-103973-fig7-data1-v2.xlsx

Male-specific DSK-CCKLR-17D1 signaling mediates SNB plasticity

To determine whether DSK signaling actually contributes to social behavior plasticity, we first examined the loss-of-function effects of relevant genes. Genomic deletion of the Dsk locus (DskattP) or DSK depletion by RNA interference (Dsk>DskRNAi) abolished injury-induced clustering behaviors (Figure 7A–C, Figure 7—figure supplement 1, Figure 7—source data 1). Furthermore, genomic deletions of the Dsk receptor CCKLR-17D1 (CCKLR-17D1attP and CCKLR-17D1Δ) but not CCKLR-17D3 (CCKLR-17D3attP and CCKLR-17D3Δ) comparably masked injury-induced social interactions (Figure 7B, Figure 7—figure supplement 2, Figure 7—source data 1). Genetic effects of Dsk deletion and DSK depletion were not consistent on baseline SD in grouped cultures. These observations suggest that Dsk is not crucial for shaping the SD traits per se, but genetic backgrounds may substantially contribute to it. Nonetheless, genetic evidence from independent alleles and transgenic RNAi convincingly supports specific implication of the DSK-CCKLR-17D1 pathway in experience-dependent social plasticity.

We further validated if neuronal activity or synaptic transmission for DSK-CCLKR-17D1 signaling controls injury-induced SNB plasticity. To this end, a temperature-sensitive allele of Drosophila dynamin (shibirets) (Kitamoto, 2001) was transgenically expressed in DSK neurons to block their synaptic transmission only at restrictive temperatures (Figure 7D). The conditional manipulation of larval DSK neurons was sufficient to suppress injury-induced clustering in group-cultured adults (Figure 7E, Figure 7—source data 1). CCKLR-17D1-expressing neurons displayed a gender-specific distribution in the adult brain when their dendrites and axons were visualized using specific transgenic markers (i.e., DenMark and SytGFP, respectively) (Figure 7F). In particular, male brains expressing the CCKLR-17D1 knock-in transgene showed more signals for cell bodies and dendrites around MP1a axon projections than females (Wu et al., 2020). This was consistent with the high levels of axonal DSK expression and trans-Tango signals in the same region of male brains (Figure 6D–F, Figure 6—source data 1). We hypothesized that transgenic activation of DSK-CCKLR-17D1 signaling genetically mimics social experience in developmentally isolated flies. Supporting this idea, transgenic excitation of CCKLR-17D1 neurons was sufficient to confer injury-induced social interactions to isolated male flies (Figure 7G, Figure 7—source data 1). The same transgenic manipulation of CCKLR-17D1 neurons did not induce social plasticity in females, irrespective of their early-life experience. On the other hand, blocking synaptic transmission in CCKLR-17D1 neurons suppressed injury-induced clustering in group-cultured male flies (Figure 7—figure supplement 3, Figure 7—source data 1). These findings convincingly provide a neuroanatomical basis for early-life social memory and male-specific social plasticity.

Discussion

Our study demonstrated that conspecific individuals show a wide range of preferences for social distancing, and it has likely co-evolved with their inferior traits as a compensatory mechanism (Figure 8). Physiological challenges (e.g., mechanical injury) may adaptively modify the clustering property of a given group. However, the plasticity of group behaviors requires social experience during development. We propose that a subset of the Drosophila brain neurons expressing the neuropeptide DSK serves as a neural substrate for social memory, as supported by the intimate coupling of early-life social experience and SNB plasticity to DSK expression, DSK neuron activity, and postsynaptic signaling.

A working model for early-life social memory and the experience-dependent plasticity of social network behavior (SNB).

Social distancing is an inheritable trait in Drosophila. Diverse genetic pathways contribute to active social preferences while the clustering property may have evolved to compensate for inferior traits in individuals and confer their group fitness. Nonetheless, Drosophila can tune their social distance depending on physiological states (e.g., mechanical injury) and this feature of SNB is defined as ‘social plasticity’. In fact, the social plasticity requires early-life social experience or ‘social memory’ during development. Group culturing elevates DSK expression and DSK neuron activity to reinforce its postsynaptic signals likely to the cognate receptor CCKLR-17D1. The activation of DSK-CCKLR-17D1 pathway thus encodes early-life social experience in developing brains to support social plasticity in adults.

Distinct social behaviors in Drosophila (i.e., aggression and mating) have been commonly mapped to specific pairs of DSK neurons (Wu et al., 2019; Wu et al., 2020; Wang et al., 2022). Presynaptic partners of DSK neurons are sexually dimorphic, while their postsynaptic effects are differentially mediated via the two DSK receptor pathways (i.e., CCKLR-17D1 for aggression and SNB plasticity; CCKLR-17D3 for sexual behaviors). Intriguingly, this neural architecture for balancing aggression and mating behaviors has been proposed to be analogously conserved between flies and mammals (Anderson, 2016). Furthermore, DSK neuron activity correlates with social dominance (i.e., winner effects from aggression) (Wu et al., 2020) and group housing (Wu et al., 2019), but not with mating status (Wang et al., 2022). Accordingly, the DSK-CCKLR-17D1 pathway meets the necessary criteria for male-specific SNB plasticity.

The phenotypic correlates of aggression and SNB in natural populations (i.e., DGRP lines) are consistent with their common neural locus. However, these findings do not necessarily imply that DSK neurons control the two social behaviors in a similar manner. For instance, DSK excitation promotes aggression in both males and females (Wu et al., 2020), whereas DSK signaling is unlikely to trigger grouping behaviors per se. What remains to be clarified is how DSK signaling gates SNB plasticity and why this process is missing in female flies. Considering that social hierarchy is established in male fights only (Nilsen et al., 2004; Simon and Heberlein, 2020), we speculate that male-specific DSK pathways may include extra circuit modalities for contextual processing of social environments and adaptive social structures. The development of neuroanatomical differences between male and female brains may coincide with the acquisition of gender-specified demands for innate behaviors and physiology during evolution (e.g., a reproductive advantage of male clustering under physiologically challenging conditions).

The mammalian DSK homolog CCK shows sexual dimorphism in brain expression and mating behavior response (Bloch et al., 1987; Bloch et al., 1988; Micevych et al., 1988). Moreover, CCK activation is implicated in aggression and exploratory behaviors (Raud et al., 2005; Li et al., 2007). It would thus be interesting to determine whether DSK/CCK signaling indeed represents an ancestral mechanism for social memory, sexually dimorphic social behaviors, and their plasticity.

Materials and methods

Fly stocks

Request a detailed protocol

Flies were raised on standard cornmeal-yeast-agar food at 25°C and 40–50% humidity under 12 hr light:12 hr dark cycles. Behavioral experiments were primarily conducted between Zeitgeber time (ZT) 4 and 8 (lights-on at ZT0; lights-off at ZT12). DGRP lines, Canton-S (BL64349), rut1 (BL9404), rut2080 (BL9405), DskattP (BL84497), Dsk2A-Gal4 (BL84630), UAS-DskRNAi (BL25869), CCKLR-17D1attP (BL84462), CCKLR-17D12A-Gal4 (BL84605), CCKLR-17D3attP (BL84463), Orco1 (BL23129), UAS-myrGFP.QUAS-mtdTomato-3xHA; trans-Tango (BL77124), 20XUAS-IVS-jGCaMP7f (BL80906), and UAS-DenMark, UAS-syt.eGFP (BL33065) were obtained from Bloomington Drosophila Stock Center. CCKLR-17D1Δ1 (119026), CCKLR-17D1Δ2 (119027), CCKLR-17D3Δ1 (119029), CCKLR-17D3Δ2 (119030), iav1 (101174), norpA7 (108362), and Poxn68 (119155) were obtained from Kyoto Drosophila Stock Center. UAS-shits, UAS-NaChBac, and UAS-TNT have been described previously (Kitamoto, 2001; Nitabach et al., 2006).

SNB analysis

Request a detailed protocol

A Petri dish (5.5 cm [d] × 1.5 cm [h]) was filled with 2% agar media (1.3 cm [h]) to prepare a circular arena for SNB analysis. This setup minimizes side-wall walking or z-stacking of individual flies that interferes with tracing group behaviors over time (Simon and Dickinson, 2010). Three-to-five-day-old flies from standard cultures (n = 16) were briefly cold-anesthetized (<15 s) and then transferred to the circular arena. Each arena was video-recorded for 10 min using a cellular phone (Samsung Galaxy Note 8 or Samsung Galaxy Note 20). Time-series coordinates of each fly’s position in the arena were extracted from raw video data using an in-house Python code (https://github.com/taejoonlab/tracking-fly, copy archived at Jeong et al., 2021a). SD was calculated from the position coordinates of individual flies at a given time and averaged over time. The average walking speed of individual flies and the interquartile ranges of the positional centroid in a given group of flies were also calculated over time (https://github.com/KJKwon/2023_FlyBehavior, copy archived at Jeong et al., 2023). The centroid velocity was determined from the fourth quarter of video-recording data, given more evident clustering phenotypes at the later period. Single-fly recordings in the circular arena were analyzed using a MATLAB-based fly_tracker code (https://github.com/jstaf/fly_tracker, copy archived at Stafford, 2016).

Fly manipulations

Request a detailed protocol

Drosophila eggs were collected on a plate filled with grape juice media (https://cshprotocols.cshlp.org/content/2007/9/pdb.rec11113). Each egg was gently transferred to an isolation chamber containing 300 ul of cornmeal-yeast-agar food for social isolation. The chamber was sealed using parafilm with a tiny hole for air circulation and kept at 25°C before relevant experiments. To generate grouped-to-isolated (GTI) flies, 3-day-old flies from standard culture vials were individually transferred to each isolation chamber and further incubated for a week. To generate isolated-to-grouped (ITG) flies, 3-day-old flies eclosed from isolated eggs were collected into a standard food vial for group-culturing (~20 isolated flies per vial) and further incubated for a week. To assess SNB in isolated or GTI flies, each fly was reared in the isolation chamber before collectively transferring into the SNB arena. Flies were briefly cold-anesthetized during GTI/ITG transitions or before transferring to the SNB arena. For physical injury, the mesothoracic segment of 3-day-old flies was pierced (<1 mm depth) using a sterilized needle.

Larval behavior and developmental analyses

Request a detailed protocol

For clustering assay, third-instar larvae were obtained from grouped-egg cultures (50 eggs per culture). A group of the third-instar larvae (n = 20) were then loaded onto a cylinder vial (2.3 cm [d] × 9.5 cm [h]) containing standard cornmeal-yeast-agar food. The food vial was divided into four sectors. The maximum number of clustering larvae per sector was scored from each vial, and the percentage of clusters with given larvae numbers was calculated from 30 vials. For digging assay, a 3D arena (2 cm [w] × 0.5 cm [d] × 4 cm [h]) was filled up to 2 cm with standard cornmeal-yeast-agar food. Either a group of third-instar larvae (n = 20) from standard culture vials or an isolated third-instar larva from single-egg cultures (one egg per culture) was transferred to the digging-assay arena and then allowed to explore it for 12 hr before measuring digging depth from the surface (Dombrovski et al., 2017). For food intake assay, a wider 3D arena (8 cm [w] × 0.5 cm [d] × 4 cm [h]) was filled up to 2 cm with standard cornmeal-yeast-agar media containing 1% brilliant blue FCF (JUNSEI, 64350-0410). A group of third-instar larvae (n = 20) from standard culture vials or an isolated third-instar larva from single-egg cultures was transferred to the food intake arena and then allowed to explore it for 12 hr. Each larva was gently homogenized in 50 ul of distilled water, and the absorbance of individual larval extracts was measured at 627 nm using a microplate reader (Tecan, Infinite M200). Developmental time was measured by the first eclosion day in a grouped-egg culture (50 eggs per culture) vs. a set of 81 single-egg cultures per experiment. The percentage of eclosed flies was scored from a grouped-egg culture (100 eggs per culture) vs. a set of 81 single-egg cultures per experiment, and the ratio of males to total flies was then calculated accordingly.

Maze assay

Request a detailed protocol

Yeast paste was placed at the corner of a 14 cm × 14 cm transparent maze, and the maze was put on a white-light box to avoid any phototatic effects. A group of four flies was starved for 6 hr and then transferred to the maze by an aspirator. A training session was completed when three or more pioneer flies reached the food, and the trained flies were transferred to an empty vial containing water only. The pioneer training was repeated three times. For the maze test, a group of 16 flies (16 naive or 12 naive + 4 pioneer flies) was briefly cold-anesthetized (<15 s) and then placed at the opposite corner of the maze to the yeast paste. The 75% arrival time was recorded when 12 or more flies reached the food.

Aggression assay

Request a detailed protocol

Quantitative assessment of aggression behaviors was performed as described previously with minor modifications (Chen et al., 2002). Three-to-five-day-old male flies were separated from females and starved for 6 hr. A pair of male flies with the same genotype were then transferred by gentle aspiration into a cylinder arena (1.4 cm [d] × 0.5 cm [h]), where a thin droplet of yeast paste with a decapitated female carcass was placed in the center. The arena was video-recorded for 10 min, and the number of lunges was counted manually.

Courtship chaining behavior assay

Request a detailed protocol

Male–male courtship was quantified by the courtship chaining index as described previously with minor modifications (Kitamoto, 2002). A group of 3-to-5-day-old male flies (n = 16) from standard cultures were briefly cold-anesthetized (<15 s), transferred to the circular arena for SNB analysis, and then video-recorded for 10 min. Courtship chain was scored when three or more male flies were engaged in chaining with courtship behavior (Kitamoto, 2002). The chaining index was calculated by the percentage of courtship chaining duration over the 10 min recording.

Transcriptome analysis

Request a detailed protocol

Three-to-five-day-old flies were harvested at ZT4-6. Total RNAs were extracted from 35 fly heads and purified using the PureLink RNA mini kit according to the manufacturer’s instructions (Invitrogen). RNA quality was assessed by Bioanalyzer using the Agilent RNA 6000 pico kit (Agilent Technologies). RNA-seq libraries were constructed using the NEBNext Ultra Directional RNA Library Prep Kit for Illumina (New England Biolabs), together with NEBNext Poly(A) mRNA Magnetic Isolation Module (New England Biolabs) and subsequently sequenced by Illumina NovaSeq 6000 or Illumina NextSeq 500/550 (LabGenomics, Republic of Korea). RNA-seq reads were processed using trimmomatic (Bolger et al., 2014) (version 0.39) with the default option to remove bases with low-quality scores or from sequencing adapters. The trimmed reads were then mapped to the Drosophila melanogaster reference genome R6.46 using STAR (Dobin et al., 2013) (version 2.7.10b). DEGs were determined using DEseq2 (Love et al., 2014) (version 1.40.0; more than twofold change with adjusted p<0.05). Transcripts undetectable in more than half of the RNA-seq libraries were excluded from the analysis. Overrepresented GO terms were identified by Fisher’s exact test (false discovery rate < 0.05) with PANTHER (Thomas et al., 2022) (version 17.0).

Quantitative brain imaging

Request a detailed protocol

Whole-mount brain imaging was performed as described previously (Jeong et al., 2021b). The primary antibodies used in immunostaining included rabbit anti-DSK (Boster Bio, DZ41371; diluted at 0.5 ug/ml) and mouse anti-BRP antibodies (Developmental Studies Hybridoma Bank, nc82; diluted at 1:1,000). The GCaMP signals from live brains were recorded at room temperature using an FV1000 (Olympus) or A1 confocal microscope (Nikon). Fluorescence intensities from regions of interest in confocal images were quantified by background normalization [(S-B)/B] using ImageJ software.

Statistical analysis

Request a detailed protocol

Statistical analyses were performed using GraphPad Prism or R (version 4.2.3). Shapiro–Wilk test was followed by F-test (two samples) or Brown–Forsythe test (multiple samples) to check normality (p<0.05) and equality of variances (p<0.05), respectively. For two-sample comparisons, parametric datasets with equal variance were analyzed by unpaired t-test. For multiple-sample comparisons, (1) parametric datasets with equal variance were analyzed by ordinary ANOVA with Tukey’s multiple comparisons test; (2) parametric datasets with unequal variance were analyzed by Welch’s ANOVA with Dunnett’s T3 multiple comparisons test (one-way) or by aligned ranks transformation ANOVA with Wilcoxon rank sum test (two-way); and (3) nonparametric datasets with equal variance were analyzed by Kruskal–Wallis test with Dunn’s multiple comparisons test (one-way) or by aligned ranks transformation ANOVA with Wilcoxon rank sum test (two-way). For comparisons between repeatedly measured samples, datasets were analyzed by paired t-test (two samples) or two-way repeated measures ANOVA with Sidak’s multiple comparisons test (multiple samples). The significance of the correlation among SD, walking speed, centroid velocity, food intake, starvation-induced activity, and aggression in DGRP strains was determined by Spearman correlation analysis. Sample sizes and p values obtained from individual statistical analyses were all summarized in each source data and indicated in the figure legends accordingly.

Data availability

The datasets generated and analyzed during the current study are included in source data files or available in the European Nucleotide Archive repository (accession number PRJEB61423). The python scripts that support the findings of this study are available from the author's GitHub webpage under the links https://github.com/taejoonlab/tracking-fly, (copy archived at Jeong et al., 2021a) and https://github.com/KJKwon/2023_FlyBehavior, (copy archived at Jeong et al., 2023).

The following data sets were generated
    1. Lim C
    (2023) EBI European Nucleotide Archive
    ID PRJEB61423. Gene expression analysis of 6 DGRP lines based on clustering behavior and housing.

References

  1. Book
    1. Giraldeau LA
    2. Caraco T
    (2000)
    Social Foraging Theory, Monographs in Behavior and Ecology
    Princeton University Press.
    1. Nichols R
    2. Schneuwly SA
    3. Dixon JE
    (1988)
    Identification and characterization of a Drosophila homologue to the vertebrate neuropeptide cholecystokinin
    The Journal of Biological Chemistry 263:12167–12170.

Article and author information

Author details

  1. Jiwon Jeong

    Department of Biological Sciences, Ulsan National Institute of Science and Technology, Ulsan, Republic of Korea
    Contribution
    Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Validation, Investigation, Visualization, Methodology, Writing – original draft
    Contributed equally with
    Kujin Kwon
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0009-0006-3111-2656
  2. Kujin Kwon

    Department of Biomedical Engineering, Ulsan National Institute of Science and Technology, Ulsan, Republic of Korea
    Contribution
    Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Validation, Investigation, Visualization, Methodology, Writing – original draft
    Contributed equally with
    Jiwon Jeong
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0009-0004-6642-222X
  3. Terezia Klaudia Geisseova

    Department of Biological Sciences, Ulsan National Institute of Science and Technology, Ulsan, Republic of Korea
    Contribution
    Investigation
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
  4. Jongbin Lee

    Research Center for Cellular Identity, Korea Advanced Institute of Science and Technology, Daejeon, Republic of Korea
    Contribution
    Investigation
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0002-5868-7437
  5. Taejoon Kwon

    1. Department of Biomedical Engineering, Ulsan National Institute of Science and Technology, Ulsan, Republic of Korea
    2. Center for Genomic Integrity, Institute for Basic Science, Ulsan, Republic of Korea
    3. Graduate School of Health Science and Technology, Ulsan National Institute of Science and Technology, Ulsan, Republic of Korea
    Contribution
    Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review and editing
    For correspondence
    tkwon@unist.ac.kr
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0002-9794-6112
  6. Chunghun Lim

    1. Research Center for Cellular Identity, Korea Advanced Institute of Science and Technology, Daejeon, Republic of Korea
    2. Department of Biological Sciences, Korea Advanced Institute of Science and Technology, Daejeon, Republic of Korea
    3. Graduate School of Stem Cell and Regenerative Biology, Korea Advanced Institute of Science and Technology, Daejeon, Republic of Korea
    Contribution
    Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review and editing
    For correspondence
    clim@kaist.ac.kr
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0001-8473-9272

Funding

Suh Kyungbae Foundation (SUHF-17020101)

  • Chunghun Lim

National Research Foundation of Korea (NRF-2021M3A9G8022960)

  • Chunghun Lim

National Research Foundation of Korea (NRF-2018R1A5A1024261)

  • Chunghun Lim

National Research Foundation of Korea (NRF-2023R1A2C100627511)

  • Taejoon Kwon

National Research Foundation of Korea (NRF-2018R1A6A1A03025810)

  • Taejoon Kwon

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Acknowledgements

We thank Bloomington Drosophila Stock Center, Developmental Studies Hybridoma Bank, Korea Drosophila Resource Center, and Kyoto Drosophila Stock Center for reagents; Kenneth Wilson and Pankaj Kapahi for raw data from their phenotypic DGRP screens. This work was supported by grants from the Suh Kyungbae Foundation (SUHF-17020101[CL]); from the National Research Foundation funded by the Ministry of Science and Information & Communication Technology (MSIT), Republic of Korea (NRF-2021M3A9G8022960 [CL]; NRF-2018R1A5A1024261 [CL]; NRF-2023R1A2C100627511 [TK]); from Basic Science Research Program through the National Research Foundation funded by Ministry of Education (NRF-2018R1A6A1A03025810 [TK]).

Copyright

© 2024, Jeong, Kwon et al.

This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.

Metrics

  • 321
    views
  • 64
    downloads
  • 0
    citations

Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.

Download links

A two-part list of links to download the article, or parts of the article, in various formats.

Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)

Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)

Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)

  1. Jiwon Jeong
  2. Kujin Kwon
  3. Terezia Klaudia Geisseova
  4. Jongbin Lee
  5. Taejoon Kwon
  6. Chunghun Lim
(2024)
Drosulfakinin signaling encodes early-life memory for adaptive social plasticity
eLife 13:e103973.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.103973

Share this article

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.103973

Further reading

    1. Neuroscience
    Matthew A Churgin, Danylo O Lavrentovich ... Benjamin L de Bivort
    Research Article

    Behavior varies even among genetically identical animals raised in the same environment. However, little is known about the circuit or anatomical origins of this individuality. Here, we demonstrate a neural correlate of Drosophila odor preference behavior in the olfactory sensory periphery. Namely, idiosyncratic calcium responses in projection neuron (PN) dendrites and densities of the presynaptic protein Bruchpilot in olfactory receptor neuron (ORN) axon terminals correlate with individual preferences in a choice between two aversive odorants. The ORN-PN synapse appears to be a locus of individuality where microscale variation gives rise to idiosyncratic behavior. Simulating microscale stochasticity in ORN-PN synapses of a 3062 neuron model of the antennal lobe recapitulates patterns of variation in PN calcium responses matching experiments. Conversely, stochasticity in other compartments of this circuit does not recapitulate those patterns. Our results demonstrate how physiological and microscale structural circuit variations can give rise to individual behavior, even when genetics and environment are held constant.

    1. Neuroscience
    Tanja Fuchsberger, Imogen Stockwell ... Ole Paulsen
    Research Advance

    The reward and novelty-related neuromodulator dopamine plays an important role in hippocampal long-term memory, which is thought to involve protein-synthesis-dependent synaptic plasticity. However, the direct effects of dopamine on protein synthesis, and the functional implications of newly synthesised proteins for synaptic plasticity, have not yet been investigated. We have previously reported that timing-dependent synaptic depression (t-LTD) can be converted into potentiation by dopamine application during synaptic stimulation (Brzosko et al., 2015) or postsynaptic burst activation (Fuchsberger et al., 2022). Here, we show that dopamine increases protein synthesis in mouse hippocampal CA1 neurons, enabling dopamine-dependent long-term potentiation (DA-LTP), which is mediated via the Ca2+-sensitive adenylate cyclase (AC) subtypes 1/8, cAMP, and cAMP-dependent protein kinase (PKA). We found that neuronal activity is required for the dopamine-induced increase in protein synthesis. Furthermore, dopamine induced a protein-synthesis-dependent increase in the AMPA receptor subunit GluA1, but not GluA2. We found that DA-LTP is absent in GluA1 knock-out mice and that it requires calcium-permeable AMPA receptors. Taken together, our results suggest that dopamine together with neuronal activity controls synthesis of plasticity-related proteins, including GluA1, which enable DA-LTP via a signalling pathway distinct from that of conventional LTP.