Distributed task-specific processing of somatosensory feedback for voluntary motor control

  1. Mohsen Omrani
  2. Chantelle D Murnaghan
  3. J Andrew Pruszynski
  4. Stephen H Scott  Is a corresponding author
  1. Queen's Univertsity, Canada
  2. Rutgers Biomedical and Health Sciences, United States
  3. University of Western Ontario, Canada
  4. Queen's University, Canada
  5. Queen’s University, Canada

Peer review process

This article was accepted for publication as part of eLife's original publishing model.

History

  1. Version of Record published
  2. Accepted Manuscript published
  3. Accepted
  4. Received

Decision letter

  1. Rui M Costa
    Reviewing Editor; Fundação Champalimaud, Portugal

In the interests of transparency, eLife includes the editorial decision letter and accompanying author responses. A lightly edited version of the letter sent to the authors after peer review is shown, indicating the most substantive concerns; minor comments are not usually included.

Thank you for submitting your work entitled "Distributed task-specific processing of somatosensory feedback for voluntary motor control" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by two peer reviewers, and the evaluation has been overseen by a Reviewing Editor and David Van Essen as the Senior Editor.

The reviewers have discussed the reviews with one another and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this decision to help you prepare a revised submission.

The authors should highlight the novelty of the findings in relation to previous studies, as there was much debate about the advances that this study provides. The authors should also address the following points:

1) Trial-by-trial analysis correlation between the timing of the corrective responses and the timing of neuronal activity.

2) Determination if the cells had receptive fields on the shoulder/elbow.

3) Discussion of how the activation of different receptor types would influence the responses evoked in different areas.

4) Influence of early- versus long-latency responses in the conclusions of the study.

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.13141.011

Author response

The authors should highlight the novelty of the findings in relation to previous studies, as there was much debate about the advances that this study provides.

Our Introduction was perhaps too subtle with regards to articulating the novelty of our approach. We have re-written the Introduction to provide:

1) A more theory driven approach based on Optimal control and highlight the dichotomy of state estimation and control policy;

2) The value of using mechanical disturbances to probe voluntary control, and;

3) The use of multiple behaviors to identify if and how a brain region reflects different aspects of control.

We think this provides a more transparent presentation of our approach. We have also modified the Discussion a bit to align with the changes in the Introduction. While the Introduction now uses optimal control principles to introduce the motivation for the study, we believe the results are not necessarily bound within this framework.

The authors should also address the following points:

1) Trial-by-trial analysis correlation between the timing of the corrective responses and the timing of neuronal activity.

This is an interesting analysis. Unfortunately, we did not consider this type of analysis when designing our experiment as we only have 10 trials per load condition making it difficult for individual neurons to reach statistical significance. At the single cell level, there are some neurons that had significant correlations with joint reversal times (10% in M1). At the population level, M1 was the only cortical region that displayed a significant correlation. The low correlation level for the M1 population (r= -0.11) is not surprising as even arm muscle activity has rather low correlations (Crevecoeur et al., 2013). We have added this analysis to the Results section.

2) Determination if the cells had receptive fields on the shoulder/elbow.

We specifically targeted our recordings in areas of the cortex that correspond with shoulder/elbow movement or have sensory responses to shoulder/elbow manipulations (i.e. cutaneous or proprioceptive response). These areas were recognized in mapping sessions, which were performed following the recording chamber implantation and before behavioral recording sessions were performed. In these sessions, as a general rule, we first identified M1 and then explored other rostral-caudal areas at this same lateral distance from the midline. If the cells had responses to hand and wrist movement, we then moved our penetrations to more lateral sites to find shoulder/elbow cells. We also tested cell receptive fields more closely in the first few sessions of recording in a new area to make sure we were in the shoulder/elbow region for a given cortical area. We now explain this process in more detail in the Methods section. We have also included the penetration map for the monkey that was euthanized and in which we can relate cortical surface maps to the penetration sites (refer to supplemental material).

3) Discussion of how the activation of different receptor types would influence the responses evoked in different areas.

It is very difficult to differentiate the potential contribution of different receptor types. Reaching studies highlight that neurons with cutaneous receptive fields in area 3b are broadly tuned to the direction of hand motion like all other cortical areas, probably reflecting skin stretch in response to the perturbation (Cohen et al., 1994). Both cutaneous and muscle afferents will likely generate similar patterns of broad tuning to loads applied to the shoulder and elbow. Further, area 3b that is commonly assumed to be receive purely cutaneous information actually receives substantive input from muscle afferents (Heath et al., 1976). Muscle afferents tend to be a bit faster than cutaneous although the difference can be small (Heath et al., 1976). We have added a paragraph in the Discussion to raise the point that while muscle afferents likely play a dominant role providing feedback of limb motion from the applied loads, cutaneous afferents also likely contribute. We have also tried to explain why differentiating these modalities is not easy while the animal is in the robot. We have also tried to clarify in more detail the penetration sites within S1 in the Methods section.

4) Influence of early- versus long-latency responses in the conclusions of the study.

R1 (short) and R2 (long) muscle responses have clear differences, which we have articulated many times (and many others have as well). Notably, R1 responses are generated at the spinal level and are fairly fixed responses that do not reflect task-level corrective responses. In contrast, R2 responses are influenced by many different behavioral level factors and occur at a time point when cortical activity can contribute. These observations lay the groundwork for the present work to begin to unravel how cortex may be involved in these goal-directed motor corrections that begin in the R2 epoch. The comparisons across tasks allow us to identify when perturbation-related activity is task-independent (like R1) and task-dependent (like R2). We assume the issue is that we don’t link our task-dependent effects in cortex to the motor periphery. Thus, we have added some statements in the Discussion to more explicitly state that task-dependent changes observed in M1 for the IN/OUT task are appropriate for driving the subsequent R2 muscle responses.

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.13141.012

Download links

A two-part list of links to download the article, or parts of the article, in various formats.

Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)

Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)

Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)

  1. Mohsen Omrani
  2. Chantelle D Murnaghan
  3. J Andrew Pruszynski
  4. Stephen H Scott
(2016)
Distributed task-specific processing of somatosensory feedback for voluntary motor control
eLife 5:e13141.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.13141

Share this article

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.13141