Spatial tuning and brain state account for dorsal hippocampal CA1 activity in a non-spatial learning task
Abstract
The hippocampus is a brain area crucial for episodic memory in humans. In contrast, studies in rodents have highlighted its role in spatial learning, supported by the discovery of place cells. Efforts to reconcile these views have found neurons in the rodent hippocampus that respond to non-spatial events but have not unequivocally dissociated the spatial and non-spatial influences on these cells. To disentangle these influences, we trained freely moving rats in trace eyeblink conditioning, a hippocampally dependent task in which the animal learns to blink in response to a tone. We show that dorsal CA1 pyramidal neurons are all place cells, and do not respond to the tone when the animal is moving. When the animal is inactive, the apparent tone-evoked responses reflect an arousal-mediated resumption of place-specific firing. These results suggest that one of the main output stages of the hippocampus transmits only spatial information, even in this non-spatial task.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.14321.001Introduction
The hippocampus is a brain structure that is known to play a critical role in memory formation (Squire, 1992), but the scope and nature of hippocampal memory processing remains elusive. The discovery of place cells in rodents led to the hypothesis that the hippocampus forms a 'cognitive map' that is essential for spatial learning (O’Keefe and Nadel, 1978). In contrast, human studies have indicated a more general role of the hippocampus in the formation of episodic memories (Scoville and Milner, 1957). In support of the latter view, electrophysiological studies in rodents have found that pyramidal neurons in the dorsal CA1 area of the hippocampus can change their firing in response to arbitrary stimuli, both spatial and non-spatial (Berger et al., 1976; Wood et al., 1999; Moita et al., 2003; Komorowski et al., 2009). However, the functional significance of these observations remains controversial because many non-spatial tasks are studied in restrained animals or under circumstances where spatial and non-spatial influences on hippocampal firing cannot be unequivocally dissociated (O’Keefe, 1999).
To overcome this challenge, we selected a simple learning task—trace eyeblink conditioning—that can be performed independently of spatial location. In this task, a neutral conditioned stimulus (CS, a tone) is followed, after a short delay, by an unconditioned stimulus (US) that evokes an involuntary blink (Figure 1A). With repeated presentations, the subject learns that the CS predicts the US and begins to blink in anticipation (Figure 1C–D). Learning the CS-US association has been shown to be hippocampally dependent in many species, including humans and rodents (Clark and Squire, 1998; Kim et al., 1995; Takehara et al., 2003). Previous electrophysiological studies have found that dorsal CA1 pyramidal neurons change their firing following the CS onset (Berger et al., 1976, 1983; Weiss et al., 1996; Weible et al., 2006), suggesting that these cells encode the non-spatial stimulus. However, these experiments did not characterize the spatial tuning properties of the recorded neurons.
Here, we disentangle the spatial and non-spatial influences on hippocampal firing by studying trace eyeblink conditioning in freely moving rats. Consistent with previous studies, we find that the CS can evoke significant changes in the firing rate of dorsal CA1 neurons. Additionally, we find that these apparent CS responses are spatially modulated, but surprisingly, they are absent when the animal is moving. These CS-evoked changes occur only when the animal is inactive, produce firing rates that match normal place cell activity, and are accompanied by a cessation of ripples. These observations are more consistent with an arousal-mediated resumption of place cell firing rather than a genuine encoding of the non-spatial stimuli. These results suggest that neurons in dorsal CA1, a major output stage of the hippocampus, transmit exclusively spatial signals even in this hippocampally dependent non-spatial task.
Results
To simultaneously map the spatial and non-spatial influences on hippocampal firing, we trained adult rats in trace eyeblink conditioning while they traversed a linear track (Figure 1B), an environment in which the spatial coding of hippocampal neurons has been well-characterized (O’Keefe, 2007). We presented the eyeblink trials at randomized locations throughout the environment, which enables us to dissociate spatial and non-spatial influences on neuronal activity.
Using chronically implanted tetrode arrays, we recorded from pyramidal cells in the dorsal CA1 region of the hippocampus (an average of 36 cells per training session for a total of 1264). Consistent with previous studies, we found that a subset of cells significantly changed their firing rate following the CS onset (p<0.01 for 10% of cells, two-sided Wilcoxon signed-rank test). However, an analysis of single-trial responses reveals that these cell-level statistics belie a high trial-to-trial variability, and none of the recorded cells respond consistently to the CS onset (Figure 1E–F).
What is the source of this trial-to-trial variability? Does spatial tuning play a role? To address this question, we mapped each cell’s spatial tuning by measuring its average firing rate as a function of the animal’s location while moving around the environment. Cells that were active in the environment had directional place fields distributed throughout the linear track (Figure 2A). The remaining cells had low firing rates everywhere, consistent with place cells that lack a place field in this environment.
These spatial firing rate maps—which were constructed with eyeblink trials omitted—reveal that a cell’s spatial tuning exerts a strong influence on its response to non-spatial stimuli. For example, the intermittently CS-responsive neuron shown in Figure 1E had a place field in the right endbox, and the trials in which it fired corresponded to the trials presented within this place field (Figure 2B). To visualize the influence of this spatial tuning, we ordered the trials by the place field intensity at the trial delivery location (Figure 2B, bottom panel). Overall, we find that the CS evokes an increase in firing when delivered within a cell’s place field, and silences the cell when delivered outside its place field (Figure 2C–E). This correlation between CS-evoked firing and place field intensity also holds at the single-cell level (Figure 2F). These observations indicate that there are no CA1 neurons that respond consistently to the CS in a space-invariant fashion. Instead, the non-spatial stimulus modulates the firing of place cells.
What is the nature of this modulation? If the CS amplifies ongoing place cell activity, as Figure 2E suggests, then we would expect that CS responses would be most prominent when place cells are actively engaged, such as during running. Surprisingly, we find the opposite: CS-evoked changes in firing rate are absent when the animal is running (Figure 3A) and occur only when the animal is inactive (Figure 3B). Importantly, the lack of CS-evoked changes while running is not accompanied by an impairment of task performance (Figure 3—figure supplement 1). When the animal is sitting, the CS-evoked change in firing actually reflects the fact that the pre-trial firing rates differ from the spatially predicted rates (Figure 3C, grey markers). After the CS onset, firing rates re-align with the place field intensity at the present location (Figure 3C, blue markers). The US evokes a response that briefly exceeds the spatially predicted rates (Figure 3C, red markers), but analysis of this phenomenon is potentially confounded by head movement associated with the unconditioned response.
Why do pre-trial firing rates in sitting animals differ from the place field intensity? During periods of inactivity, animals may enter a state of quiet wakefulness, in which the firing of place cells is not exclusively governed by the animal’s present location. Instead, the hippocampus exhibits other patterns of firing, such as synchronous bursts (ripples) that may engage neurons with distant place fields (Foster and Wilson, 2006; Csicsvari et al., 2007; Diba and Buzsáki, 2007). We find that when the animal is sitting, ripples spontaneously occur during the pre-trial period, and abruptly cease following the CS onset (Figure 4A). Furthermore, CS-evoked changes in firing rate are more pronounced in trials with detected ripples (Figure 4B) and the firing of these cells outside their place fields is closely associated with the ripple events (Figure 4C). These observations suggest that the animal is indeed in a state of quiet wakefulness during 'sitting' trials, and that the CS triggers an arousal response that leads to a resumption of place cell firing.
Finally, we investigated the effect of the CS on hippocampal theta oscillations. Previous studies have reported that the CS can reset the theta rhythm, aligning the phase of theta to the CS onset (Moita et al., 2003; Nokia et al., 2010; Darling et al., 2011). To distinguish a genuine phase reset from stimulus-evoked initiation of theta oscillations, it is important to restrict our analysis to trials with ongoing theta activity prior to the CS onset. The prominent theta oscillations during running provide the ideal conditions for this analysis (Figure 5A). Under these conditions, we found that both the CS and US can evoke a sustained alignment of theta phase, but only in one out of three animals (Figure 5D). Even when theta phase alignment did occur, the new phase remained highly correlated with the pre-stimulus phase (Figure 5E), indicating that the observed alignment is due to a subtle phase shift rather than a reset of the theta rhythm. These results confirm that it is possible for the eyeblink stimuli to perturb ongoing theta oscillations, but the absence of this perturbation in two out of three animals shows that it is not necessary to perform the task.
Discussion
These findings require a re-evaluation of how dorsal CA1 pyramidal neurons may encode non-spatial information. The simplest form of non-spatial coding would be a purely non-spatial CS cell (Figure 6A) that exhibits a consistent change in firing to the CS independent of the animal’s location. None of the 1264 pyramidal cells responded in this purely non-spatial manner.
Previous studies that found spatial correlates in hippocampal responses to non-spatial stimuli have hypothesized that these cells encode the conjunction of a specific non-spatial stimulus with a spatial context (Wood et al., 1999; Moita et al., 2003). A conjunction CS-place cell (Figure 6B) would change its firing in a location-dependent manner. At first glance, our data appear to fit this model—the CS responses of place cells are gated by their place fields (Figure 2)—but our key observation is that CS-evoked changes in firing rate disappear when the animal is running (Figure 3). This new finding rejects the conjunctive model because these cells can no longer encode the CS-place conjunction in this behavioral state.
A model that only relies on the known properties of place cell firing provides a more compelling explanation of the experimental observations (Figure 6C). In particular, place cells exhibit place-specific firing only when the animal is moving or alert, and exhibit a different pattern of firing when the animal is in a state of quiet wakefulness. If the CS were able to trigger an arousal response, this would manifest as an increase in firing within a cell’s place field (due to activation of place-specific firing) and a decrease in firing outside the place field (due to cessation of ripple-related firing). Consistent with this hypothesis, we find that in sitting animals, the CS indeed activates place-specific firing, silences out-of-field firing, and abolishes ripples. When the animal is already alert, such as during running, we observe no additional modulation beyond normal place field firing.
Hence the most parsimonious explanation of the experimental data is that dorsal CA1 neurons are all place cells and the apparent responses to the non-spatial stimuli are due to an arousal-mediated resumption of place-specific firing.
These observations clarify the nature of previously-reported patterns of hippocampal activity in eyeblink conditioning. Studies of inactive subjects in a fixed location, such as those with restrained animals (Berger et al., 1983; Weiss et al., 1996; Weible et al., 2006), would find that a subset of cells—those with a place field overlapping the experiment location—consistently increase their firing following the stimulus presentation. Most cells would not have place fields overlapping this location and would be silenced following the stimulus presentation.
The arousal-mediated nature of the hippocampal response can also account for a variety of learning-related phenomena. In particular, any salient stimulus should be able to evoke these hippocampal responses, regardless of whether the task is hippocampally dependent (Berger et al., 1983; Moita et al., 2003; Abe et al., 2014). The salience of the stimulus, and hence its ability to trigger alertness, may change as the subject learns the predictive power of the stimulus, producing a stereotyped evolution of hippocampal activity across learning (McEchron and Disterhoft, 1997). On the other hand, manipulations that decrease the salience of the CS, such as habituation to the unpaired stimuli presented to control animals, would reduce the apparent response (Berger et al., 1976; Weiss et al., 1996).
Previous studies have also reported that the CS can reset the phase of hippocampal theta oscillations (Nokia et al., 2010; Darling et al., 2011). Given the lack of CS-evoked firing rate changes while running, could the CS onset be encoded by this phase reset instead? Analogous to previous studies, we find evidence for an alignment of theta phase to the CS onset. However, a direct comparison of pre- and post-stimulus phase reveals that this effect is due to a slight perturbation of the ongoing theta phase (Figure 5). In particular, this analysis rejects the notion of theta reset to a specific phase. Furthermore, this phase perturbation is present in only one out of three animals, indicating that it is not necessary for performing the task. Moreover, we did not find evidence that theta phase precession was affected by the CS onset (Figure 5—figure supplement 4). Note, however, that this analysis does not rule out the possibility that the CS may modulate the degree of theta phase-locking across brain areas.
Finally, extensive lesion studies have demonstrated the critical importance of the hippocampus in trace eyeblink conditioning (Kim et al., 1995; Takehara et al., 2003). How do our observations fit with this literature? We found that CS-evoked changes in dorsal CA1 firing rates and theta phase are not necessary for task performance—these responses are absent during running, but performance is not impaired—yet dorsally restricted lesions still produce learning deficits (Takehara et al., 2003). Several possible explanations may account for this apparent discrepancy: (1) Dorsal hippocampal lesions may disrupt activity in other hippocampal areas (e.g. ventral CA1), which may contain genuinely CS-responsive neurons. (2) The encoding of the CS may be more subtle than changes in firing rate, but still detectable by postsynaptic targets. (3) The critical engagement of the hippocampus may not occur only during the trial itself. Memory consolidation is hippocampally dependent and proceeds without additional training (Takehara et al., 2003; Takehara-Nishiuchi and McNaughton, 2008), indicating that some eyeblink-related activity occurs well after the stimulus presentation. (4) Even though spatial information may appear irrelevant in this task, the presence of ongoing place-specific firing may still be necessary for learning space-invariant responses. For example, awareness of the context may be necessary for the animal to attend to the appropriate stimuli, or learning a context-dependent association may be a necessary stepping-stone to developing a space-invariant association (Nadel and Willner, 1980).
These findings suggest that the dorsal hippocampus transmits exclusively spatial signals, even in a hippocampally dependent non-spatial task, and constrain the ways in which it may contribute to memory processing. These experiments also highlight the importance of accounting for brain state in studies where alertness changes may confound the observed selectivity to experimental stimuli.
Materials and methods
Electrophysiological recordings
Request a detailed protocolWe recorded from three male Long-Evans rats, 3–5 months old at the time of surgery. We chronically implanted each animal with a differential pair of EMG recording wires in the left eyelid muscle, a pair of bipolar stimulation electrodes in the same area, and a microdrive array that allowed independent adjustment of 24 tetrodes. In two animals (A and B), these tetrodes were targeted to the right dorsal hippocampus; animal C received a bilateral implant targeting the dorsal hippocampus in both hemispheres. We lowered these tetrodes to their targets in the dorsal CA1 cell layer over the course of several days.
Electrophysiological signals were buffered on the headstage, amplified, and digitally recorded for offline analysis. This data acquisition system also received timestamps from the video recording system and the stimulus delivery system to allow these data streams to be synchronized for analysis. All data analysis was performed in MATLAB using the DataJoint data processing chain toolbox (Yatsenko et al., 2015).
At the end of the experiment, we marked the final electrode locations using electrolytic lesions. Animals were then euthanized and intracardially perfused with 4% paraformaldehyde. We reconstructed the recording sites using a combination of high-resolution magnetic resonance imaging (Lubenov and Siapas, 2009) and histology (brains were cryoprotected, embedded in gelatin, frozen, and sectioned at 20 μm; Figure 1—figure supplement 1).
All animal procedures were in accordance with the National Institutes of Health (NIH) guidelines, and with the approval of the Caltech Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee.
Training environment
Request a detailed protocolAll experiments were performed in an acoustically and electromagnetically shielded room containing a sleepbox and a 180 cm long, 7.5 cm wide linear track. Each end of the track terminates in a 22 × 16 cm endbox that contains a water port for reward delivery. An overhead video system tracks LED markers on the tetrode headstage to provide a measurement of animal position.
Animals were housed in the sleepbox within the recording environment. An automated direct-current lighting system provided a 12-hr light cycle with light onset at 9am.
After recovering from surgery, animals were trained to traverse the linear track for water reward. Animals were given at least 12 days to habituate to the environment and the linear track task before starting eyeblink conditioning.
Conditioning procedure
Request a detailed protocolWe used a 250 ms, 5 kHz tone as the CS, a 250 ms stimulus-free trace interval, and 10 ms bipolar stimulation of the eyelid muscle as the US. Lesion studies in rats have shown that this trace interval requires the hippocampus for successful acquisition (Weiss et al., 1999). The CS was delivered from a speaker located above the center of the track. The loudness of the tones used during training was determined during a tone-only habituation session by gradually increasing the loudness until startle responses (0–80 ms after CS onset) are detected in the eyelid EMG, then lowering the loudness until these responses disappear. We did not measure the CS loudness along the track, but afterwards verified that conditioned responses could be observed at all locations.
For all animals, we delivered eyeblink conditioning trials at random locations while they were running on the track. Animal C also received trials at random time intervals. Animals received between 38 and 186 eyeblink trials per training session (an average of 109), depending on how much they ran on the track. All animals received two training sessions per day, at the beginning and end of the 12-hr light cycle.
Behavioral analysis
Request a detailed protocolAfter filtering the differential EMG data with a 120–960 Hz passband, we computed its root-mean-square (RMS) value in three 100 ms windows: 'baseline' from –100 to 0 ms relative to CS onset, 'CR' from 380 to 480 ms (US onset occurs at 500 ms), and 'control' from –580 to –480 ms. Trials in which [CR − baseline] exceeded the 95th percentile of that animal’s [control − baseline] were considered blinks.
Spike detection and sorting
Request a detailed protocolAfter filtering the tetrode data with a 600–6000 Hz passband, we detected and aligned spikes on peaks in the nonlinear energy operator (Mukhopadhyay and Ray, 1998). Spikes were clustered by fitting a mixture model in a 12-dimensional feature space (three principal components per tetrode channel) (Calabrese and Paninski, 2011; Ecker et al., 2014). A cluster was considered a single unit if it was well-isolated (sum of false positive and negative rates < 10%), had a refractory period > 1.5 ms, had a spike amplitude > 120 μV, and its spike waveforms remained stable throughout the course of the recording.
These criteria identified 1585 single units, of which 1264 we deemed to be putative pyramidal cells based on firing rate (<4 Hz), spike width (>0.25 ms), and bursting (>5% of inter-spike intervals are <10 ms). Each of these 1264 units is treated independently in our analysis, even though several are likely to be repeated observations of the same neuron. We estimate that we recorded only 400–500 unique pyramidal cells.
Place field analysis
Request a detailed protocolWe constructed place field maps by dividing the number of spikes in each location by the occupancy of that location (Rich et al., 2014). Track rate maps are one-dimensional, computed for each direction independently, and smoothed with a σ = 3 cm Gaussian kernel. Endbox rate maps are two-dimensional, non-directional, and smoothed with σ = 1 cm.
Only periods where the animal was moving (velocity > 2 cm/s) were included in the construction of the firing rate maps. Furthermore, the 2-s window following the CS onset was excluded so that eyeblink responses would not influence the construction of the place field maps. Areas with an occupancy < 50 ms/cm2 were considered to have an indeterminate spatial firing rate, and any eyeblink trials that occurred in these locations were excluded from analysis.
We found that 38% of cells were not active in this environment (i.e. did not have a place field with peak > 2 Hz), but this underestimates the actual fraction of silent cells, as we are unable to reliably cluster single units with an overall firing rate < 0.01 Hz.
The place field intensity on a given trial is the value of the place field map at the location in space where the trial was delivered. Since the animal may be moving, this is taken as the average over the animal’s trajectory:
where is the place field map for this cell and are the animal’s trajectory over time. and define the time window for averaging. When sorting rasters (Figures 2B–E, 3A–B, and 4), this corresponds to the window visible in the figure; for analysis of CS response (Figures 2F and 3C), this corresponds to the 500 ms post-CS analysis window.
Statistical analysis of CS response
Request a detailed protocolAll analysis of CS response is based on 500 ms windows immediately prior to ('pre-CS') and immediately following ('post-CS') the CS onset.
To characterize a cell’s CS response, we used the two-sided Wilcoxon signed-rank test without correction for multiple comparisons. We found 123 cells (10% of all pyramidal cells) that were significant at a p<0.01 level. Of these, 87 increased their firing rate and 36 decreased their firing rate. However, this statistical test should be interpreted as a heuristic measure only, as the influences of spatial tuning and brain state violate the test’s assumption that samples are independent and identically distributed.
To quantify the reliability of a cell’s CS response (Figure 1F), we looked for differences in pre- vs. post-CS firing on a trial-by-trial basis. For a rate-increasing cell like the one shown in Figure 1E, the observed response reliability is the fraction of trials in which it fired more spikes post-CS than pre-CS. The expected response reliability is the probability of this occurring given the cell’s average pre- and post-CS firing rates, assuming that spike counts are Poisson distributed. Specifically,
where is the Poisson probability mass function and are this cell’s average number of pre- and post-CS spikes, respectively.
To quantify the spatial tuning of the CS-evoked firing (Figure 2F), we computed Spearman’s rank correlation coefficient between the CS-evoked firing and the place field intensity at the trial delivery locations. Cells that never fired any spikes following the CS were assigned a correlation of zero.
Ripple analysis
Request a detailed protocolWe detected ripples by looking for high-frequency oscillations in the local field potential (LFP). First, we filtered the tetrode data with an 80–250 Hz passband and looked for peaks in the RMS power. For each detected peak, we determined its frequency using a short-time Fourier transform (STFT) centered around the event and considered it a putative ripple if its frequency was between 120 and 250 Hz and it was detected on at least 3 tetrodes.
Theta analysis
Request a detailed protocolWe analyzed the LFP to identify the effects of the CS/US on the hippocampal theta rhythm. For each animal, we used a single tetrode for all theta analysis. We chose tetrodes superficial to the cell layer (as determined by sharp wave polarity) so that small changes in the tetrode depth would have a minimal effect on the recorded theta phase (Lubenov and Siapas, 2009).
For each trial, we estimate the instantaneous theta frequency and phase using a short-time Fourier transform (STFT) with a 400 ms Kaiser window (α = 1). At each time point, we determine the instantaneous theta frequency by looking for a peak in the spectral power density in the 4–12 Hz range. We then obtain the instantaneous theta amplitude and phase from the modulus and argument of the STFT at that frequency (Figure 5—figure supplement 3B). The mean resultant length (Figure 5D) is given by
where is the theta phase for trial at time . This measure of circular concentration ranges between 0 (phases are symmetrically distributed, e.g. uniform) and 1 (phases are all concentrated at a single value).
This STFT-based approach guarantees that our theta estimates cannot be influenced by any features in the data more than ± 200 ms away. For example, we often observed a high-amplitude transient following the US delivery. This clearly skews the estimated theta phase in the vicinity of the US, but our analysis approach ensures that phase estimates 200 ms before or after the transient are unaffected. In contrast, techniques that involve the Hilbert transform or an analysis of IIR-filtered signals cannot guarantee that that the influence of such transients remain localized to a fixed time interval.
These guarantees ensure that our phase continuity analysis (Figure 5E) does not introduce spurious autocorrelations. The pre-CS window ends at the CS onset, and the post-CS window starts 100 ms after that (Figure 5—figure supplement 3C), therefore the pre- and post-CS phase estimates are derived from independent segments of data. To assist in comparison, we use the corresponding frequency estimates to extrapolate both phase estimates to the same point in time (the midpoint of this 100 ms gap, i.e. 50 ms after the CS onset).
References
-
Sound-induced modulation of hippocampal θ oscillationsNeuroReport 25:1368–1374.https://doi.org/10.1097/WNR.0000000000000274
-
Single-unit analysis of different hippocampal cell types during classical conditioning of rabbit nictitating membrane responseJournal of Neurophysiology 50:1197–1219.
-
Kalman filter mixture model for spike sorting of non-stationary dataJournal of Neuroscience Methods 196:159–169.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneumeth.2010.12.002
-
Eyeblink conditioning contingent on hippocampal theta enhances hippocampal and medial prefrontal responsesJournal of Neurophysiology 105:2213–2224.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00801.2010
-
Forward and reverse hippocampal place-cell sequences during ripplesNature Neuroscience 10:1241–1242.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1961
-
Robust conjunctive item-place coding by hippocampal neurons parallels learning what happens whereJournal of Neuroscience 29:9918–9929.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1378-09.2009
-
Sequence of single neuron changes in CA1 hippocampus of rabbits during acquisition of trace eyeblink conditioned responsesJournal of Neurophysiology 78:1030–1044.
-
A new interpretation of nonlinear energy operator and its efficacy in spike detectionIEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering 45:180–187.https://doi.org/10.1109/10.661266
-
Context and conditioning: A place for spacePhysiological Psychology 8:218–228.https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03332853
-
Hippocampal theta-band activity and trace eyeblink conditioning in rabbitsBehavioral Neuroscience 123:631–640.https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015334
-
BookHippocampal Neurophysiology in the Behaving AnimalIn: Anderson P, Morris R, Amaral D, Bliss T, O’Keefe J, editors. The Hippocampus Book. Oxford: Oxford University Press. pp. 475–548.https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195100273.003.0011
-
Combinatorial amygdalar inputs to hippocampal domains and hypothalamic behavior systemsBrain Research Reviews 38:247–289.https://doi.org/10.1016/S0165-0173(01)00080-7
-
Loss of recent memory after bilateral hippocampal438 lesionsJournal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry 20:11–21.
-
Memory and the hippocampus: a synthesis from findings with rats, monkeys, and humansPsychological Review 99:195–231.https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.99.2.195
-
Time-dependent reorganization of the brain components underlying memory retention in trace eyeblink conditioningJournal of Neuroscience 23:9897–9905.
-
Hippocampal lesions prevent trace eyeblink conditioning in the freely moving ratBehavioural Brain Research 99:123–132.https://doi.org/10.1016/S0166-4328(98)00096-5
Article and author information
Author details
Funding
Larry L. Hillblom Foundation
- Maria Papadopoulou
National Science Foundation (IOS-1146871)
- Athanassios G Siapas
National Institutes of Health (1DP1OD008255)
- Athanassios G Siapas
G Harold and Leila Y. Mathers Foundation
- Athanassios G Siapas
Gordon and Betty Moore Foundation
- Athanassios G Siapas
National Institutes of Health (5DP1MH099907)
- Athanassios G Siapas
The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.
Acknowledgements
We thank C Wierzynski for development of the experimental paradigm with tetrode recordings, A Hoenselaar for development of the data acquisition system and data processing toolchain, J Mok for assistance with histology, and S Cassenaer, B Hulse, and B Sauerbrei for valuable comments on the manuscript.
Ethics
Animal experimentation: This study was performed in strict accordance with the recommendations in the Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals of the National Institutes of Health. All of the animals were handled according to approved institutional animal care and use committee (IACUC) protocols (#1465) of the California Institute of Technology. All surgeries were performed under aseptic conditions and under isoflurane anesthesia.
Copyright
© 2016, Shan et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
-
- 2,299
- views
-
- 414
- downloads
-
- 20
- citations
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.
Download links
Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)
Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)
Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)
Further reading
-
- Cell Biology
- Neuroscience
Experience shapes the brain as neural circuits can be modified by neural stimulation or the lack of it. The molecular mechanisms underlying structural circuit plasticity and how plasticity modifies behaviour are poorly understood. Subjective experience requires dopamine, a neuromodulator that assigns a value to stimuli, and it also controls behaviour, including locomotion, learning, and memory. In Drosophila, Toll receptors are ideally placed to translate experience into structural brain change. Toll-6 is expressed in dopaminergic neurons (DANs), raising the intriguing possibility that Toll-6 could regulate structural plasticity in dopaminergic circuits. Drosophila neurotrophin-2 (DNT-2) is the ligand for Toll-6 and Kek-6, but whether it is required for circuit structural plasticity was unknown. Here, we show that DNT-2-expressing neurons connect with DANs, and they modulate each other. Loss of function for DNT-2 or its receptors Toll-6 and kinase-less Trk-like kek-6 caused DAN and synapse loss, impaired dendrite growth and connectivity, decreased synaptic sites, and caused locomotion deficits. In contrast, over-expressed DNT-2 increased DAN cell number, dendrite complexity, and promoted synaptogenesis. Neuronal activity modified DNT-2, increased synaptogenesis in DNT-2-positive neurons and DANs, and over-expression of DNT-2 did too. Altering the levels of DNT-2 or Toll-6 also modified dopamine-dependent behaviours, including locomotion and long-term memory. To conclude, a feedback loop involving dopamine and DNT-2 highlighted the circuits engaged, and DNT-2 with Toll-6 and Kek-6 induced structural plasticity in this circuit modifying brain function and behaviour.
-
- Neuroscience
Memory impairment in chronic pain patients is substantial and common, and few therapeutic strategies are available. Chronic pain-related memory impairment has susceptible and unsusceptible features. Therefore, exploring the underlying mechanisms of its vulnerability is essential for developing effective treatments. Here, combining two spatial memory tests (Y-maze test and Morris water maze), we segregated chronic pain mice into memory impairment-susceptible and -unsusceptible subpopulations in a chronic neuropathic pain model induced by chronic constrictive injury of the sciatic nerve. RNA-Seq analysis and gain/loss-of-function study revealed that S1P/S1PR1 signaling is a determinant for vulnerability to chronic pain-related memory impairment. Knockdown of the S1PR1 in the dentate gyrus (DG) promoted a susceptible phenotype and led to structural plasticity changes of reduced excitatory synapse formation and abnormal spine morphology as observed in susceptible mice, while overexpression of the S1PR1 and pharmacological administration of S1PR1 agonist in the DG promoted an unsusceptible phenotype and prevented the occurrence of memory impairment, and rescued the morphological abnormality. Finally, the Gene Ontology (GO) enrichment analysis and biochemical evidence indicated that downregulation of S1PR1 in susceptible mice may impair DG structural plasticity via interaction with actin cytoskeleton rearrangement-related signaling pathways including Itga2 and its downstream Rac1/Cdc42 signaling and Arp2/3 cascade. These results reveal a novel mechanism and provide a promising preventive and therapeutic molecular target for vulnerability to chronic pain-related memory impairment.