Lovastatin fails to improve motor performance and survival in methyl-CpG-binding protein2-null mice

Abstract

Previous studies provided evidence for the alteration of brain cholesterol homeostasis in 129.Mecp2-null mice, an experimental model of Rett syndrome. The efficacy of statins in improving motor symptoms and prolonging survival of mutant mice suggested a potential role of statins in the therapy of Rett syndrome. In the present study, we show that Mecp2 deletion had no effect on brain and serum cholesterol levels and lovastatin (1.5 mg/kg, twice weekly as in the previous study) had no effects on motor deficits and survival when Mecp2 deletion was expressed on a background strain (C57BL/6J; B6) differing from that used in the earlier study. These findings indicate that the effects of statins may be background specific and raise important issues to consider when contemplating clinical trials. The reduction of the brain cholesterol metabolite 24S-hydroxycholesterol found in B6.Mecp2-null mice suggests the occurrence of changes in brain cholesterol metabolism and the potential utility of using plasma levels of 24S-OHC as a biomarker of brain cholesterol homeostasis in RTT.

Article and author information

Author details

  1. Claudia Villani

    Laboratory of Neurochemistry and Behaviour, Department of Neuroscience, Istituto di Ricerche Farmacologiche Mario Negri, Milano, Italy
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0001-6334-9013
  2. Giuseppina Sacchetti

    Laboratory of Neurochemistry and Behaviour, Department of Neuroscience, Istituto di Ricerche Farmacologiche Mario Negri, Milano, Italy
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  3. Renzo Bagnati

    Analytical Instrumentation Unit, Department of Environmental Health Sciences, Istituto di Ricerche Farmacologiche Mario Negri, Milano, Italy
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  4. Alice Passoni

    Analytical Instrumentation Unit, Department of Environmental Health Sciences, Istituto di Ricerche Farmacologiche Mario Negri, Milano, Italy
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  5. Federica Fusco

    Genetics of Neurodegenerative Diseases Unit, Department of Neuroscience, Istituto di Ricerche Farmacologiche Mario Negri, Milano, Italy
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  6. Mirjana Carli

    Laboratory of Neurochemistry and Behaviour, Department of Neuroscience, Istituto di Ricerche Farmacologiche Mario Negri, Milano, Italy
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  7. Roberto William Invernizzi

    Laboratory of Neurochemistry and Behaviour, Department of Neuroscience, Istituto di Ricerche Farmacologiche Mario Negri, Milano, Italy
    For correspondence
    rinvernizzi@marionegri.it
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0002-6017-9781

Funding

Istituto di Ricerche Farmacologiche Mario Negri (Intramural funding)

  • Roberto William Invernizzi

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Ethics

Animal experimentation: The Istituto di Ricerche Farmacologiche "Mario Negri" adheres to the principle set out in the following law, regulations, and policies governing the care and use of laboratory animals: Italian Governing Law (D.lgs.26/2014; Authorisation n. 19/2008-A issued March 6, 2008 by Ministry of Health); Mario Negri Institutional Regulations and Policies providing internal authorization for persons conducting animal experiments (Quality Management System Certificate - UNI EN ISO 9001:2008 - Reg. N{degree sign} 6121); the NIH Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals (2011 edition) and EU directives and guidelines (EEC Council Directive 2010/63/UE). The statement of Compliance (Assurance) with the Public Health Service (PHS) Policy on Human Care and Use of Laboratory Animals has been recently reviewed (9/9/2014) and will expire on September 30, 2019 (Animal Welfare Assurance #A5023-01). The protocol was approved by the Italian Ministry of Health (Permit Number 946/2015-PR).

Copyright

© 2016, Villani et al.

This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License permitting unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.

Metrics

  • 1,089
    views
  • 188
    downloads
  • 15
    citations

Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.

Download links

A two-part list of links to download the article, or parts of the article, in various formats.

Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)

Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)

Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)

  1. Claudia Villani
  2. Giuseppina Sacchetti
  3. Renzo Bagnati
  4. Alice Passoni
  5. Federica Fusco
  6. Mirjana Carli
  7. Roberto William Invernizzi
(2016)
Lovastatin fails to improve motor performance and survival in methyl-CpG-binding protein2-null mice
eLife 5:e22409.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.22409

Share this article

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.22409

Further reading

    1. Neuroscience
    Xinlin Hou, Peng Zhang ... Dandan Zhang
    Research Article

    Emotional responsiveness in neonates, particularly their ability to discern vocal emotions, plays an evolutionarily adaptive role in human communication and adaptive behaviors. The developmental trajectory of emotional sensitivity in neonates is crucial for understanding the foundations of early social-emotional functioning. However, the precise onset of this sensitivity and its relationship with gestational age (GA) remain subjects of investigation. In a study involving 120 healthy neonates categorized into six groups based on their GA (ranging from 35 and 40 weeks), we explored their emotional responses to vocal stimuli. These stimuli encompassed disyllables with happy and neutral prosodies, alongside acoustically matched nonvocal control sounds. The assessments occurred during natural sleep states using the odd-ball paradigm and event-related potentials. The results reveal a distinct developmental change at 37 weeks GA, marking the point at which neonates exhibit heightened perceptual acuity for emotional vocal expressions. This newfound ability is substantiated by the presence of the mismatch response, akin to an initial form of adult mismatch negativity, elicited in response to positive emotional vocal prosody. Notably, this perceptual shift’s specificity becomes evident when no such discrimination is observed in acoustically matched control sounds. Neonates born before 37 weeks GA do not display this level of discrimination ability. This developmental change has important implications for our understanding of early social-emotional development, highlighting the role of gestational age in shaping early perceptual abilities. Moreover, while these findings introduce the potential for a valuable screening tool for conditions like autism, characterized by atypical social-emotional functions, it is important to note that the current data are not yet robust enough to fully support this application. This study makes a substantial contribution to the broader field of developmental neuroscience and holds promise for future research on early intervention in neurodevelopmental disorders.