A theory of working memory without consciousness or sustained activity

  1. Darinka Trübutschek  Is a corresponding author
  2. Sébastien Marti
  3. Andrés Ojeda
  4. Jean-Rémi King
  5. Yuanyuan Mi
  6. Misha Tsodyks
  7. Stanislas Dehaene
  1. Ecole des Neurosciences de Paris Ile-de-France, France
  2. Université Paris-Sud, France
  3. University of Oxford, United Kingdom
  4. New York University, United States
  5. Institute of Basic Medical Sciences, China
  6. Weizmann Institute of Science, Israel
  7. Institut national de la santé et de la recherche médicale, France

Abstract

Working memory and conscious perception are thought to share similar brain mechanisms, yet recent reports of non-conscious working memory challenge this view. Combining visual masking with magnetoencephalography, we investigate the reality of non-conscious working memory and dissect its neural mechanisms. In a spatial delayed-response task, participants reported the location of a subjectively unseen target above chance-level after several seconds. Conscious perception and conscious working memory were characterized by similar signatures: a sustained desynchronization in the alpha/beta band over frontal cortex, and a decodable representation of target location in posterior sensors. During non-conscious working memory, such activity vanished. Our findings contradict models that identify working memory with sustained neural firing, but are compatible with recent proposals of ‘activity-silent’ working memory. We present a theoretical framework and simulations showing how slowly decaying synaptic changes allow cell assemblies to go dormant during the delay, yet be retrieved above chance-level after several seconds.

Article and author information

Author details

  1. Darinka Trübutschek

    Ecole des Neurosciences de Paris Ile-de-France, Paris, France
    For correspondence
    darinkat87@gmail.com
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0001-7977-1366
  2. Sébastien Marti

    Cognitive Neuroimaging Unit, CEA DSV/I2BM, INSERM, Université Paris-Sud, Gif-sur-Yvette, France
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  3. Andrés Ojeda

    Department of Zoology, University of Oxford, Oxford, United Kingdom
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  4. Jean-Rémi King

    Department of Psychology, New York University, New York, United States
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  5. Yuanyuan Mi

    Brain Science Center, Institute of Basic Medical Sciences, Beijing, China
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  6. Misha Tsodyks

    Department of Neurobiology, Weizmann Institute of Science, Rehovot, Israel
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  7. Stanislas Dehaene

    Cognitive Neuroimaging Unit, Institut national de la santé et de la recherche médicale, Gif sur Yvette, France
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.

Funding

Ecole des Neurosciences de Paris (PhD Fellowship)

  • Darinka Trübutschek

Fondation Schneider Electric (PhD Fellowship)

  • Darinka Trübutschek

CEA

  • Stanislas Dehaene

INSERM

  • Stanislas Dehaene

Collège de France

  • Stanislas Dehaene

European Research Council (Senior Grant,NeuroConsc)

  • Stanislas Dehaene

Fondation Roger de Spoelberch

  • Stanislas Dehaene

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Reviewing Editor

  1. Tatiana Pasternak, University of Rochester, United States

Ethics

Human subjects: The study was approved by the by CPP IDF under the reference CPP 08 021. All subjects gave written informed consent and consent to publish before participating in the study.

Version history

  1. Received: December 2, 2016
  2. Accepted: July 13, 2017
  3. Accepted Manuscript published: July 18, 2017 (version 1)
  4. Version of Record published: September 7, 2017 (version 2)

Copyright

© 2017, Trübutschek et al.

This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License permitting unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.

Metrics

  • 10,322
    views
  • 1,462
    downloads
  • 90
    citations

Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.

Download links

A two-part list of links to download the article, or parts of the article, in various formats.

Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)

Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)

Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)

  1. Darinka Trübutschek
  2. Sébastien Marti
  3. Andrés Ojeda
  4. Jean-Rémi King
  5. Yuanyuan Mi
  6. Misha Tsodyks
  7. Stanislas Dehaene
(2017)
A theory of working memory without consciousness or sustained activity
eLife 6:e23871.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.23871

Share this article

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.23871

Further reading

    1. Neuroscience
    Alexandra L Jellinger, Rebecca L Suthard ... Steve Ramirez
    Research Article

    Negative memories engage a brain and body-wide stress response in humans that can alter cognition and behavior. Prolonged stress responses induce maladaptive cellular, circuit, and systems-level changes that can lead to pathological brain states and corresponding disorders in which mood and memory are affected. However, it is unclear if repeated activation of cells processing negative memories induces similar phenotypes in mice. In this study, we used an activity-dependent tagging method to access neuronal ensembles and assess their molecular characteristics. Sequencing memory engrams in mice revealed that positive (male-to-female exposure) and negative (foot shock) cells upregulated genes linked to anti- and pro-inflammatory responses, respectively. To investigate the impact of persistent activation of negative engrams, we chemogenetically activated them in the ventral hippocampus over 3 months and conducted anxiety and memory-related tests. Negative engram activation increased anxiety behaviors in both 6- and 14-month-old mice, reduced spatial working memory in older mice, impaired fear extinction in younger mice, and heightened fear generalization in both age groups. Immunohistochemistry revealed changes in microglial and astrocytic structure and number in the hippocampus. In summary, repeated activation of negative memories induces lasting cellular and behavioral abnormalities in mice, offering insights into the negative effects of chronic negative thinking-like behaviors on human health.

    1. Neuroscience
    Alexandra H Leighton, Juliette E Cheyne, Christian Lohmann
    Research Article

    Synaptic inputs to cortical neurons are highly structured in adult sensory systems, such that neighboring synapses along dendrites are activated by similar stimuli. This organization of synaptic inputs, called synaptic clustering, is required for high-fidelity signal processing, and clustered synapses can already be observed before eye opening. However, how clustered inputs emerge during development is unknown. Here, we employed concurrent in vivo whole-cell patch-clamp and dendritic calcium imaging to map spontaneous synaptic inputs to dendrites of layer 2/3 neurons in the mouse primary visual cortex during the second postnatal week until eye opening. We found that the number of functional synapses and the frequency of transmission events increase several fold during this developmental period. At the beginning of the second postnatal week, synapses assemble specifically in confined dendritic segments, whereas other segments are devoid of synapses. By the end of the second postnatal week, just before eye opening, dendrites are almost entirely covered by domains of co-active synapses. Finally, co-activity with their neighbor synapses correlates with synaptic stabilization and potentiation. Thus, clustered synapses form in distinct functional domains presumably to equip dendrites with computational modules for high-capacity sensory processing when the eyes open.