A genetic basis for molecular asymmetry at vertebrate electrical synapses

  1. Adam C Miller  Is a corresponding author
  2. Alex C Whitebirch
  3. Arish N Shah
  4. Kurt C Marsden
  5. Michael Granato
  6. John O'Brien
  7. Cecilia B Moens
  1. Univeristy of Oregon, United States
  2. Fred Hutchinson Cancer Research Center, United States
  3. University of Pennsylvania Perelman School of Medicine, United States
  4. McGovern Medical School, University of Texas Health Sciences Center at Houston, United States

Abstract

Neural network function is based upon the patterns and types of connections made between neurons. Neuronal synapses are adhesions specialized for communication and they come in two types, chemical and electrical. Communication at chemical synapses occurs via neurotransmitter release whereas electrical synapses utilize gap junctions for direct ionic and metabolic coupling. Electrical synapses are often viewed as symmetrical structures, with the same components making both sides of the gap junction. By contrast, we show that a broad set of electrical synapses in zebrafish, Danio rerio, require two gap-junction-forming Connexins for formation and function. We find that one Connexin functions presynaptically while the other functions postsynaptically in forming the channels. We also show that these synapses are required for the speed and coordination of escape responses. Our data identify a genetic basis for molecular asymmetry at vertebrate electrical synapses and show they are required for appropriate behavioral performance.

Data availability

The following data sets were generated
    1. Miller
    (2013) Dis2 RNA-seq wildtype and mutant
    Publicly available at the NCBI Sequence Read Archive (accession no: PRJNA172016).

Article and author information

Author details

  1. Adam C Miller

    Institute of Neuroscience, Univeristy of Oregon, Eugene, United States
    For correspondence
    acmiller@uoregon.edu
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0001-7519-3677
  2. Alex C Whitebirch

    Division of Basic Sciences, Fred Hutchinson Cancer Research Center, Seattle, United States
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  3. Arish N Shah

    Division of Basic Sciences, Fred Hutchinson Cancer Research Center, Seattle, United States
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  4. Kurt C Marsden

    Department of Cell and Developmental Biology, University of Pennsylvania Perelman School of Medicine, Philadelphia, United States
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  5. Michael Granato

    Department of Cell and Developmental Biology, University of Pennsylvania Perelman School of Medicine, Philadelphia, United States
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  6. John O'Brien

    Department of Ophthalmology and Visual Science, McGovern Medical School, University of Texas Health Sciences Center at Houston, Houston, United States
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0002-0270-3442
  7. Cecilia B Moens

    Division of Basic Sciences, Fred Hutchinson Cancer Research Center, Seattle, United States
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.

Funding

National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (F32NS074839)

  • Adam C Miller

National Institute of Mental Health (R01MH109498)

  • Michael Granato

National Eye Institute (R01EY012857)

  • John O'Brien

Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (R01HD076585)

  • Cecilia B Moens

National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (R21NS076950)

  • Cecilia B Moens

National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (K99/R00NS085035)

  • Adam C Miller

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Ethics

Animal experimentation: All animals were raised in an Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (IACUC)-approvedfacility at the Fred Hutchinson Cancer Research Center (Study ID 50552, Submittal ID 7237, IRO #1392).

Copyright

© 2017, Miller et al.

This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License permitting unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.

Metrics

  • 2,149
    views
  • 422
    downloads
  • 44
    citations

Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.

Download links

A two-part list of links to download the article, or parts of the article, in various formats.

Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)

Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)

Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)

  1. Adam C Miller
  2. Alex C Whitebirch
  3. Arish N Shah
  4. Kurt C Marsden
  5. Michael Granato
  6. John O'Brien
  7. Cecilia B Moens
(2017)
A genetic basis for molecular asymmetry at vertebrate electrical synapses
eLife 6:e25364.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.25364

Share this article

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.25364

Further reading

    1. Neuroscience
    2. Stem Cells and Regenerative Medicine
    Desiree Böck, Maria Wilhelm ... Gerald Schwank
    Research Article

    Parkinson’s disease (PD) is a multifactorial disease caused by irreversible progressive loss of dopaminergic neurons (DANs). Recent studies have reported the successful conversion of astrocytes into DANs by repressing polypyrimidine tract binding protein 1 (PTBP1), which led to the rescue of motor symptoms in a chemically-induced mouse model of PD. However, follow-up studies have questioned the validity of this astrocyte-to-DAN conversion model. Here, we devised an adenine base editing strategy to downregulate PTBP1 in astrocytes and neurons in a chemically-induced PD mouse model. While PTBP1 downregulation in astrocytes had no effect, PTBP1 downregulation in neurons of the striatum resulted in the expression of the DAN marker tyrosine hydroxylase (TH) in non-dividing neurons, which was associated with an increase in striatal dopamine concentrations and a rescue of forelimb akinesia and spontaneous rotations. Phenotypic analysis using multiplexed iterative immunofluorescence imaging further revealed that most of these TH-positive cells co-expressed the dopaminergic marker DAT and the pan-neuronal marker NEUN, with the majority of these triple-positive cells being classified as mature GABAergic neurons. Additional research is needed to fully elucidate the molecular mechanisms underlying the expression of the observed markers and understand how the formation of these cells contributes to the rescue of spontaneous motor behaviors. Nevertheless, our findings support a model where downregulation of neuronal, but not astrocytic, PTBP1 can mitigate symptoms in PD mice.

    1. Neuroscience
    Xiaoqian Yan, Sarah Shi Tung ... Kalanit Grill-Spector
    Research Article

    Organizing the continuous stream of visual input into categories like places or faces is important for everyday function and social interactions. However, it is unknown when neural representations of these and other visual categories emerge. Here, we used steady-state evoked potential electroencephalography to measure cortical responses in infants at 3–4 months, 4–6 months, 6–8 months, and 12–15 months, when they viewed controlled, gray-level images of faces, limbs, corridors, characters, and cars. We found that distinct responses to these categories emerge at different ages. Reliable brain responses to faces emerge first, at 4–6 months, followed by limbs and places around 6–8 months. Between 6 and 15 months response patterns become more distinct, such that a classifier can decode what an infant is looking at from their brain responses. These findings have important implications for assessing typical and atypical cortical development as they not only suggest that category representations are learned, but also that representations of categories that may have innate substrates emerge at different times during infancy.