A genetic basis for molecular asymmetry at vertebrate electrical synapses

  1. Adam C Miller  Is a corresponding author
  2. Alex C Whitebirch
  3. Arish N Shah
  4. Kurt C Marsden
  5. Michael Granato
  6. John O'Brien
  7. Cecilia B Moens
  1. Fred Hutchinson Cancer Research Center, United States
  2. University of Pennsylvania Perelman School of Medicine, United States
  3. University of Texas Health Sciences Center at Houston, United States

Abstract

Neural network function is based upon the patterns and types of connections made between neurons. Neuronal synapses are adhesions specialized for communication and they come in two types, chemical and electrical. Communication at chemical synapses occurs via neurotransmitter release whereas electrical synapses utilize gap junctions for direct ionic and metabolic coupling. Electrical synapses are often viewed as symmetrical structures, with the same components making both sides of the gap junction. By contrast, we show that a broad set of electrical synapses in zebrafish, Danio rerio, require two gap-junction-forming Connexins for formation and function. We find that one Connexin functions presynaptically while the other functions postsynaptically in forming the channels. We also show that these synapses are required for the speed and coordination of escape responses. Our data identify a genetic basis for molecular asymmetry at vertebrate electrical synapses and show they are required for appropriate behavioral performance.

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.25364.001

Introduction

Brain function is derived from the patterns of connectivity and the types of synapses made between neurons within a circuit. Synapses come in two main types, chemical and electrical, that together dynamically define neural circuit function across all life stages and animal phyla (Connors and Long, 2004; Meier and Dermietzel, 2006; Pereda, 2014). Chemical synapses are broadly used within the nervous system and are overtly asymmetric at the molecular and functional level, with communication between neurons achieved via presynaptic neurotransmitter release and postsynaptic neurotransmitter reception. Electrical synapses are best known for their roles in early nervous system development, however they are found throughout the brain from inception to adulthood and contribute to function from sensation to central processing to motor output (Hormuzdi et al., 2004). Electrical synapses are generally viewed as molecularly and functionally symmetric, allowing for very fast bidirectional ionic and metabolic neuronal communication (Connors and Long, 2004). This direct route of communication is achieved by neuronal gap junctions (GJs) that are formed from plaques of tens to thousands of channels between the neurons (Raviola and Gilula, 1973; Hormuzdi et al., 2004). While often viewed as simple channels, work in invertebrates has found that unique GJ-forming proteins can be contributed asymmetrically from each side of the synapse (Phelan et al., 2008; Starich et al., 2009). Moreover, the molecular asymmetry can create functional asymmetry (rectification) in ionic flow through the GJ (Phelan et al., 2008). In vertebrate nervous systems electron microscopy and electrophysiology suggests that electrical synapse structure and function can also be asymmetric (Brightman and Reese, 1969; Rash et al., 2013). However, the genetic basis for such molecular asymmetry at vertebrate electrical synapses has never been identified.

Neuronal GJs are intercellular channels that establish cytoplasmic continuity between two cells allowing ions and molecules less than ~1 kilodalton to traverse between cells. Each channel is composed of two hemi-channels, one contributed by each neuron, that interact via their extracellular domains to form the channel (Figure 1A) (Connors and Long, 2004). Vertebrate GJ-forming proteins are the Connexins (Cx) while invertebrates use an evolutionarily distinct but functionally equivalent set of Innexins (Phelan, 2005). In mammals there are 21 genes encoding Cx proteins of which five are known to be expressed in neurons and to form electrical synapses (Connors and Long, 2004; Söhl et al., 2005). A partial genome duplication that occurred in teleost fish has increased the number of Cx-encoding genes, and the ensuing evolutionary trajectories saw some genes being duplicated and others lost (Eastman et al., 2006; Cruciani and Mikalsen, 2007; Zoidl et al., 2008). This diversity of Cxs encoded in the genome results in cases where individual cells or apposed cells express multiple different GJ-forming proteins providing the opportunity for heteromeric hemichannels made from multiple Cxs or heterotypic channels made by pairing hemichannels between cells with different Cxs (Palacios-Prado et al., 2014a). In vertebrates, heterotypic GJs between cells in non-neuronal tissue are common (Goodenough and Paul, 2009), however it has been notoriously difficult to identify heterotypic Cx configurations at electrical synapses in neurons.

gjd1a/cx34.1 is required for electrical synapse formation.

(A) Schematic of an electrical synapse, a specialized neuronal gap junction. (B) Simplified schematic of the zebrafish Mauthner (M) circuit in dorsal view with anterior to the left. Neurons and synapses of the hindbrain and two, of 30, spinal segments are shown. In the hindbrain mixed electrical/chemical synapses are made between auditory (Aud) afferent neurons and the M cell lateral dendrite (Aud/M electrical synapses). In the spinal cord the M axon makes electrical synapses with commissural local (CoLo) interneurons found in each segment (M/CoLo electrical synapses). (C,D) Two representative dorsal views of spinal cord segments from M/CoLo:GFP larvae at 5 days post fertilization. Images are maximum intensity projections of ~10 uM with anterior to the left. Scale bar = 10 uM. Larvae are stained for anti-GFP (magenta), anti-human-Connexin36 (Cx36, yellow), and for neurofilaments (RMO44, blue). Individual GFP and Cx36 channels are shown in neighboring panels. Associated experimental statistics are found in the figure-related table. The Cx36 staining found at wildtype M/CoLo synapses (C), red circles) is lost in dis3 mutants (D), red circles). (E) Genome wide RNA-seq-based mapping data. The average frequency of mutant markers (black marks) is plotted against genomic position. A single region on chromosome 5 (chr5) emerges with an allele frequency near 1.0 indicating linkage to the dis3 mutation (red arrow). Each chromosome is separated by vertical lines and labeled at the bottom. (F) Mutant reads from the RNA-seq mapping data at two separate positions within the gjd1a/cx34.1 gene are shown aligned to the reference genome identifying two missense changes within dis3 mutant animals. Wildtype reference (ref) genome nucleotides and encoded amino acids (aa) are noted. Aligned mutant (MUT) reads are shown as grey boxes; colored letters highlights differences from reference. (G) Electrical synapses are lost in trans-heterozygous animals carrying a dis3 and an 8 bp frameshift allele (dis3/8 bp) in gjd1a/cx34.1. Images of the spinal cord as in (C,D). Associated quantitation of Cx36 at wildtype or mutant synapses can be found in source data for Figure 1.

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.25364.002
Figure 1—source data 1

gjd1a/cx34.1 is required for electrical synapse formation.

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.25364.003

Here we identify two Cxs used asymmetrically for the formation and function of electrical synapses in zebrafish. We focus on the Mauthner neural circuit, which mediates a fast escape behavior, since it has uniquely identifiable pre- and postsynaptic neurons making stereotyped electrical synapses that contribute to the escape response. We find that two Cx-encoding genes, gjd1a/cx34.1 and gjd2a/cx35.5, are required for synapse assembly and function. Both of these Cx proteins are localized to synapses throughout the Mauthner circuit, as well as other synapses within the hindbrain and spinal cord, and each requires the other for recruitment to the synapse. Using chimeric analysis we demonstrate a dramatic asymmetry at electrical synapses in the circuit: gjd1a/cx34.1 is necessary and sufficient postsynaptically while gjd2a/cx35.5 is necessary and sufficient presynaptically for synapse formation. Using high throughput behavioral analysis we find that the electrical synapses contribute to the speed and coordination of the Mauthner-induced escape response. Together our data support a model wherein molecularly asymmetric electrical synapses act in concert with chemical synapses to impart performance onto the neural network output.

Results

The disconnect3 mutation disrupts gjd1a/cx34.1 and electrical synapse formation

To investigate genes required for electrical synapse formation in vivo we used the Mauthner (M) neural circuit of larval zebrafish (Figure 1B). The Mauthner circuit coordinates an escape response to a variety of threatening stimuli and produces the fastest sensorimotor reflex performed by fish (~5 milliseconds (ms) to initiate, ~10 ms to complete) (Eaton et al., 1977; Fetcho, 1991; Burgess and Granato, 2007; Satou et al., 2009). The Mauthner circuit that produces the escape is a combination of electrical, excitatory chemical, and inhibitory chemical synapses contained within a relatively simple set of neurons that must be coordinated to ensure fast and robust escapes (Fetcho, 1991). The circuit is wired to ensure that it makes unidirectional turns away from the stimulus. To do so, vibrational stimuli are transmitted to the Mauthner neurons via the VIIIth cranial nerve auditory afferents, which make prominent and stereotyped mixed electrical and glutamatergic chemical synapses at so-called Club Ending synapses, hereafter called the Aud/M synapse (Pereda et al., 2004; Yao et al., 2014). Mauthner in turn sends a contralateral projection down the length of the spinal cord where it activates primary motorneurons (MNs) in each hemisegment and drives the stereotypical C-bend-shaped escape away from stimulus (Fetcho, 1991). The projecting Mauthner axon concurrently activates CoLo interneurons in each segment via excitatory electrical synapses; CoLo in turn re-crosses the spinal cord and inhibits MNs on the stimulus side, thereby ensuring coordinated turns away from the stimulus (Satou et al., 2009). Throughout the paper we use the terms pre- and postsynaptic for electrical synapses based on three main criteria: (1) information flow, as described above, from auditory afferents (pre) to Mauthner dendrite (post), then from Mauthner axon (pre) to CoLo (post) (Fetcho, 1991; Satou et al., 2009); (2) Mauthner itself is a bipolar neuron with unique axonal (pre) and dendritic (post) compartments (Kimmel et al., 1981); (3) both the Aud/M and M/CoLo electrical synapses have closely associated chemical synapse components with the neurotransmitter receptors localizing to postsynaptic compartments of the neurons (Mauthner dendrite in the hindbrain [Yao et al., 2014], CoLo proximal portion of the neurite on the ipsilateral side of the spinal cord [Miller et al., 2015]). The Mauthner and CoLo neurons can be visualized using the transgenic line zf206Et(Tol-056), hereafter called M/CoLo:GFP, which expresses GFP in both neuron types (Satou et al., 2009). The electrical synapses of the Mauthner circuit can be visualized by immunostaining using a polyclonal antibody against the human Cx36 protein (Figure 1C)(Miller et al., 2015).

To identify genes required for electrical synapse formation we mutagenized animals with N-ethyl-N-nitrosourea (ENU) to generate random genomic mutations and created gynogenetic-diploid mutant animals (Walker et al., 2009) to screen for disruptions to the Cx36 staining found at M/CoLo synapses at 3 days post fertilization (dpf)(Miller et al., 2015). We identified a mutation we called disconnect3 (dis3) that caused a complete loss of Cx36 staining at the M/CoLo synapses (Figure 1D). Cx36 staining was disrupted at Mauthner electrical synapses and other electrical synapses in dis3 mutants across all timepoints examined (2 to 14 dpf). The stereotyped number of Mauthner and CoLo neurons was not disrupted in dis3 mutants: Mauthner –wildtype, heterozygous, and mutant animals had 2 ± 0 Mauthners / animal; CoLo – wildtype, heterozygous, and mutant avg. 1.975 ± 0.06, 2 ± 0.06, and 2 ± 0 CoLos/8 segments / animal, respectively (segments 5–12 were counted, mean ± standard deviation, n = 5, 11, 5 for wildtype, heterozygous, and mutant, respectively). Moreover, in dis3 mutants the M and CoLo neurites contact one another in the spinal cord similar to wildtype (Figure 1C,D). These results suggest that the gene disrupted by the dis3 mutation may have a specific role in electrical synapse formation.

To identify the causative gene we used an RNA sequencing (RNA-seq)-based approach that identifies shared regions of genomic homozygosity in a pool of mutant animals (Miller et al., 2013). We separately pooled 108 mutant (-/-) and 108 wildtype (+/+ and +/-) siblings and extracted and sequenced mRNA (Illumina Hi-Seq) from each pool. The sequences were aligned to the genome and single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) were identified in the wildtype pool to serve as mapping markers. The SNP frequency was assessed in the mutant pool, identifying an ~1.8 megabase (Mb) region of homozygosity on chromosome 5 in dis3 mutants (Figure 1E). Within this region we used the RNA-seq data to look for potentially deleterious mutations and found that there were two missense changes to a Cx-encoding gene (Figure 1F). Cx proteins are labeled for their molecular weight, while the genes encoding the proteins are named for one of five classes in which they reside (Söhl and Willecke, 2004). The missense changes identified in dis3 mutants disrupts the gene gap junction delta 1a (gjd1a) encoding a Cx with a predicated molecular weight of 34.1 kiloDaltons (Cx34.1). This gene is highly related to the mammalian gjd2 gene encoding Cx36 (see below). For clarity throughout, when referring to zebrafish Cxs we will use both the official gene name as well as its molecular weight designation, in this case gjd1a/cx34.1. Within the ~1.8 Mb region of mapping there were an additional 10 missense mutations in other genes and one gene with significantly reduced expression compared to wildtype. To determine if the mutations in gjd1a/cx34.1 were causative for the phenotype we generated an eight base pair (bp) frame-shifting mutation at nucleotide 4 of exon 1 (gjd1afh436) using transcription activator-like-effector nucleases (TALENs) (Sanjana et al., 2012; Shah et al., 2015). We found that larvae homozygous for the frameshift mutation (gjd1afh436/fh436) phenocopy the loss of Cx36-staining at M/CoLo synapses observed in the dis3 mutants; moreover, gjd1afh436 failed to complement the dis3 mutation (Figure 1G). We renamed the dis3 mutation gjd1afh360 and conclude that gjd1a/cx34.1 is required for M/CoLo electrical synapse formation.

gjd1 (Cx34) and gjd2 (Cx35/Cx36) are part of a family of highly conserved genes/proteins

In mammals there are five unique Connexins that form electrical synapses, with the gjd2/cx36 gene being the most studied due to its widespread use in the nervous system (Connors and Long, 2004; Söhl et al., 2005; Pereda, 2014). Previous work in zebrafish and goldfish has implicated two Cx36-related proteins, Cx34.7 and Cx35.1, as being present at electrical synapses (Pereda et al., 2003; O'Brien et al., 2004; Carlisle and Ribera, 2014; Rash et al., 2013). We wondered how gjd1a/cx34.1 related to these and other Connexins and so used its sequence to search the zebrafish genome for similar genes. This identified three highly-related loci, two of these being the previously identified gjd1b/cx34.7 and gjd2b/cx35.1, while the third is predicted to encode a Cx with a molecular weight of 35.5 kD, gjd2a/cx35.5 (Figure 2). Within this group of four Cx-encoding genes, all of the predicted proteins share greater than 85% amino acid identity to each other and to mammalian Cx36 proteins (Figure 2A). The main variation in protein sequence arises in the intracellular loop, with some modest variation in the C-terminal tail (Figure 2A,B). We examined the evolutionary relationship between these proteins and found that they represent two distinct Cx sub-families (Figure 2C). In basal vertebrates (lamprey) we found only one gene within the family, this gene was subsequently duplicated creating two sub-families – the Cx35/36 proteins encoded by gjd2 genes and the Cx34 proteins encoded by genes we have named gjd1. Genes within both of these families are found in lineages up to and including birds, but in mammals the gjd1 family has apparently been lost. In bony fishes, including zebrafish, these gene families underwent another duplication event (Postlethwait et al., 2004), resulting in the four extant genes (Figure 2C). Previous work has found that the gjd1b/cx34.7 and gjd2b/cx35.1 genes are expressed in the nervous system of zebrafish, analogous to gjd2 (Cx36) expression in mammals (Li et al., 2009; Carlisle and Ribera, 2014). We examined published RNA-seq and EST datasets (Ensembl: WTSI stranded RNA-Seq, WTSI, KIT, Yale [Yates et al., 2016] and the UCSC Genome Browser [Kent et al., 2002]) and found that all four of the gjd1 and gjd2 genes in zebrafish are enriched for nervous system expression while being reduced or absent from other tissues. Together it appears that all of the gjd1/2 genes have the potential to play broad roles in neural circuit formation and function.

The zebrafish genome contains four Connexin-encoding genes related to mammalian gjd2/cx36.

(A) Amino acid alignment of four zebrafish Cx36-like proteins with human Cx36. Colored lines indicate approximate location of the domains shown in the schematic in B. (B) Schematic of Cx36-like proteins. Blue: extracellular (EC) loops; grey cylinders: transmembrane (TM) domains; black: intracellular N- and C-terminal tails; magenta: intracellular (IL) loop. Red stars indicate the positions of mutations in ENU-induced (fh329, fh360) and engineered (fh435, fh436, fh437, fh454) mutations in the four zebrafish genes encoding Cx36-like proteins: gjd1a/cx34.1, 1b/cx34.7, 2a/cx35.5, and 2b/cx35.1. Details of mutations can be found in the figure-related table. (C) Phylogeny of vertebrate Cx36 proteins. gjd2a and 2b are duplicates of the single mammalian gene gjd2, which encodes Cx36. Gjd1a and 1b are duplicates of a gene that was lost in the tetrapod lineage. Lamprey = Petromyzon marinus, duck = Anas platyrhynchos, lizard = Anolis carolinensis, toad = Xenopus tropicalis, mouse = Mus musculus, rat = Rattus norvegicus, human = Homo sapiens, zfish = Danio rerio. Sequence information of mutants can be found in source data for Figure 2.

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.25364.004
Figure 2—source data 1

The zebrafish genome encodes four Connexin-encoding genes related to mammalian gjd2/cx36.

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.25364.005

gjd1a/cx34.1 and gjd2a/cx35.5 are required for electrical synapse formation and function

While we identified gjd1a/cx34.1 as being required for M/CoLo synapse formation (Figure 1) we wondered if any of the other closely related genes were required for Mauthner circuit synaptogenesis. To examine these genes we generated mutations in each of them (Figure 2B) and analyzed the effect of their loss on electrical synapse formation (Shah et al., 2015). We examined the electrical synapses made onto the M cell body and dendrite (Figure 3A) and particularly focused on the stereotyped Aud/M synapses (Figure 3A arrows, Figure 3B) and the M/CoLo electrical synapses of the spinal cord (Figure 3C). The anti-human-Cx36 antibody used for staining recognizes all four of the proteins generated by the zebrafish gjd1/2 genes when each is expressed in HeLa cells (see Materials and methods). We found that gjd1a/cx34.1 mutants affected not only the M/CoLo synapses in the spinal cord, but also the Aud/M synapses, as well as other electrical synapses made onto the Mauthner cell body (Figure 3D–F). In analyzing mutants for the other three genes, we found that gjd2a/cx35.5 mutants also lost Mauthner electrical synapses in the hindbrain and spinal cord (Figure 3G–I) while gjd1b/cx34.7 and gjd2b/cx35.1 had no effect on Cx36 staining (Figure 3J,K). In quantifying the amount of Cx36-staining at Mauthner synapses we found that in gjd2a/cx35.5 mutants there was a complete absence of detectable Cx protein at synapses throughout the Mauthner circuit. In gjd1a/cx34.1 mutants the Cx36 staining at M/CoLo synapses was lost, but we note that ~ 30% of staining remained (an ~4 fold reduction) at the Aud/M synapses (Figure 3E,J,K). This residual staining was not eliminated in double or triple mutants between gjd1a/cx34.1 and gjd1b/cx34.7 or gjd2b/cx35.1 (Figure 3L). On the whole we conclude that the gap junctions that form the Mauthner circuit electrical synapses require both gjd1a/cx34.1 and gjd2a/cx35.5 for their formation.

Electrical synapses of the Mauthner circuit require both gjd1a/cx34.1 and gjd2a/cx35.5.

(A–I). In this and all subsequent figures, unless otherwise specified, images are dorsal views of hindbrain, Mauthner lateral dendrite, and two spinal cord segments from M/CoLo:GFP larvae at 5 days post fertilization. Hindbrain, lateral dendrite, and spinal cord images are maximum intensity projections of ~30,~5, and ~10 uM, respectively. Anterior is to the left. Scale bar = 10 uM. Larvae are stained for anti-GFP (magenta), anti-human-Connexin36 (Cx36, yellow), and for neurofilaments (RMO44, blue). Individual GFP and Cx36 channels are shown in neighboring panels. Electrical synapses are found on the Mauthner cell body (A) with prominent and stereotyped Aud/M synapses found on Mauthner’s lateral dendrite (arrows in A), (B) and at M/CoLo synapses in the spinal cord (C), red circles). (D–I) In gjd1a/cx34.1 and gjd2a/cx35.5 mutants Mauthner electrical synapses are lost or, in the specific case of Aud/M synapses in gjd1a/cx34.1 mutants, diminished. (J–L) Bar graphs represent the mean of the indicated value quantified at synapses with each circle representing the average of 12–16 M/CoLo or 8–12 Aud/M synapses within an animal. (J,K) M/CoLo electrical synapses are absent in gjd1a/cx34.1 and gjd2a/cx35.5 mutants, while Aud/M club endings are strongly reduced in gjd1a mutants and absent in gjd2a mutants. gjd1b and gjd2b have no effect on Mauthner electrical synapses. N = L. The remaining Cx36 staining observed at Aud/M synapses in gjd1a mutants is not due to gjd1b or gjd2b. For reference, the first bar is a duplication of gjd1a mutant data from K. In gjd1a;gjd2a double mutants the remaining Cx36 staining is lost as expected given that gjd2a is required for Aud/M synapses (K). In double and triple mutants combinations between gjd1a, gjd1b, and gjd2b there is no effect on the remaining Cx36 staining at Aud/M synapses. Associated quantitation of Cx36 at wildtype or mutant synapses can be found in Figure 3—source data 1 for Figure 3 and Figure 3—source data 2 for Figure 3.

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.25364.006
Figure 3—source data 1

Electrical synapses of the Mauthner circuit require both gjd1a/cx34.1 and gjd2a/cx35.5.

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.25364.007
Figure 3—source data 2

Electrical synapses of the Mauthner circuit require both gjd1a/cx34.1 and gjd2a/cx35.5.

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.25364.008

To investigate whether Cx34.1 and Cx35.5 were required for electrical synapse function we examined whether the passage of the gap junction permeable dye neurobiotin (Nb) was impaired in mutants. We retrogradely labeled Mauthner axons with Nb from caudal spinal cord transections (Figure 4A) and then detected Nb within the CoLo cell bodies in the M/CoLo:GFP line (Figure 4B) (Miller et al., 2015). In both gjd1a/cx34.1 and gjd2a/cx35.5 mutants we found that no Nb was transferred from Mauthner to CoLo (Figure 4C–G). By contrast, we found that in gjd1b/cx34.7 and gjd1b/cx35.1 mutants Nb transfers from Mauthner to CoLo similar to wildtype (Figure 4G). That Nb passage is completely blocked in both gjd1a/cx34.1 and gjd2a/cx35.5 mutants supports the idea that Mauthner circuit electrical synapses are dependent on both Cx34.1 and Cx35.5 proteins for their function. Taken together these data suggest that there is an intimate interaction between these two Cxs required for the establishment of M electrical synapses.

Electrical synapses are functionally defective in gjd1a/cx34.1 and gjd2a/cx35.5 mutants.

Retrograde labeling of Mauthner axons with the gap junction permeable dye Neurobiotin (Nb) from a caudal transection. Hindbrain and spinal cord images are maximum intensity projections of ~30 and~10 uM, respectively. Anterior is to the left. Scale bar = 10 uM. Spinal cord images are at the level of the CoLo cell bodies (circles), which is dorsal to the synapses. (A–F) Larvae are stained for anti-GFP (magenta), biotin (Nb, cyan), and anti-human-Connexin36 (Cx36, yellow). Nb labels the Mauthner cell bodies and other caudally projecting neurons in the hindbrain (A) and passes from the Mauthner axon through the electrical synapses to fill the CoLo cell bodies (B), circles). Other neurons are also labeled due to projections caudally into the lesion site (A, non-Mauthner neurons, B, non-circled cell bodies). (C–F) In gjd1a/cx34.1 and gjd2a/cx35.5 mutants Nb labels M normally (C,E) however none passes into CoLos (D,F). (G) Quantitation of the ratio of Nb in CoLo to M cell bodies in wildtype and mutants. Each circle represents the average Nb fluorescence within 8–12 CoLo cell bodies compared to the 2 Mauthner cell bodies in an animal. Associated experimental statistics are found in the figure-related table. Associated quantitation of Nb transfer in wildtype or mutant can be found in source data for Figure 4.

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.25364.009
Figure 4—source data 1

Electrical synapses are functionally defective in gjd1a/cx34.1 and gjd2a/cx35.5 mutants.

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.25364.010

gjd1a (Cx34.1) and gjd2a (Cx35.5) are localized at and required for the majority of electrical synapses in the hindbrain and spinal cord

To examine whether Cx34.1 and Cx35.5 are both present at electrical synapses we generated antibodies that specifically recognized either protein (see Materials and methods for details). Both antibodies generated staining patterns highlighting the same structures in the hindbrain and spinal cord as staining with the antibody against human Cx36 (Figure 5A–C). Cx34.1 and Cx35.5 staining overlapped nearly completely throughout the hindbrain and spinal cord, but there were rare examples where staining was apparent for only one of the two Cx proteins (Figure 5A). At Mauthner circuit electrical synapses Cx34.1 and Cx35.5 staining was completely overlapping at the individually identifiable Aud/M and M/CoLo synapses in the hindbrain and the spinal cord (Figure 5A–C). In gjd1a/cx34.1 mutants we found that Cx34.1 staining was lost while in the gjd2a/cx35.5 mutants we found that Cx35.5 staining was lost supporting both the specificity of the antibodies as well as the deleterious nature of the mutations (Figure 5D–I). In gjd2a/cx35.5 mutants we found that Cx34.1 staining was lost from Aud/M and M/CoLo synapses suggesting Cx34.1 requires Cx35.5 for its localization at Mauthner synapses (Figure 5G–I). In gjd1a/cx34.1 mutants we found that Cx35.5 was completely lost from M/CoLo synapses (Figure 5F) and residual Cx35.5 staining was found at Aud/M synapses similar to what was observed with the human Cx36 antibody (Figure 5D,E; Figure 3D,E,J,K). Additionally, in both gjd2a/cx35.5 and gjd1a/cx34.1 mutants we detected dim Cx34.1 and Cx35.5-positive puncta, respectively, that were not associated with the Mauthner circuit (Figure 5D–I). Such staining may represent synapses at which Cx34.1 and Cx35.5 do not require one another for their localization. Altogether our results suggest that both Cx34.1 and Cx35.5 are broadly expressed and used throughout the hindbrain and spinal cord of zebrafish. Moreover, while each Cx can apparently be used independently, where Cx34.1 and Cx35.5 colocalize their mutual presence supports the robust localization of the other at electrical synapses.

Recruitment of Gjd1a/Cx34.1 and Gjd2a/Cx35.5 to electrical synapses is co-dependent.

Larvae are stained for anti-GFP (magenta), anti-Connexin34.1 (Cx34.1, yellow), and for anti-Connexin35.5 (Cx35.5, cyan). Individual Cx34.1 and Cx35.5 channels are shown in neighboring panels. Hindbrain, lateral dendrite, and spinal cord images are maximum intensity projections of ~30,~5, and ~10 uM, respectively. Anterior is to the left. Scale bar = 10 uM. (A–C) Cx34.1 and Cx35.5 are found colocalized at electrical synapses throughout the hindbrain (A) including at Aud/M (arrows in A, (B), and M/CoLo synapses (C). In gjd1a/cx34.1 mutants the localization of Cx35.5 to electrical synapses is lost (D–F) or diminished at Aud/M synapses (E); in gjd2a/cx35.5 mutants the localization of Cx34.1 to electrical synapses is lost (G–I). Associated antibody information can be found in source data for Figure 5.

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.25364.011
Figure 5—source data 1

Recruitment of Gjd1a/Cx34.1 and Gjd2a/Cx35.5 to electrical synapses is co-dependent.

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.25364.012

Mauthner circuit electrical synapses require gjd1a/cx34.1 in postsynaptic neurons and gjd2a/cx35.5 in presynaptic neurons

Since we found that (gjd1a) Cx34.1 and (gjd2a) Cx35.5 are colocalized and support one another’s localization at Mauthner synapses we wondered where the two Cxs were required for electrical synapse assembly. Given that electrical synapses are composed from Cx hemichannels contributed by apposing neurons (Figure 1A), both Cx34.1 and Cx35.5 could be found on both sides of the synapse (heteromeric hemichannels), or instead each could be found exclusively on one side or the other (heterotypic channels). To test where each gene was required for electrical synapse formation we performed cell transplants at the blastula stage (Kemp et al., 2009) to create chimaeric animals containing wildtype and mutant cells and examined in which neuron of the circuit (auditory, Mauthner, or CoLo) the genes were required. We focused on the Mauthner circuit because we are able to associate the stereotyped synapses of the circuit with the uniquely identifiable pre- and postsynaptic neurons (see Figure 1B and associated discussion). To create chimaeric animals we transplanted cells at the blastula stage from donor embryos marked with either the M/CoLo:GFP transgene or Biotin Dextran (BD) into host embryos lacking the same marker and examined the resulting embryos at 5 dpf (Figure 6). To mark the presynaptic auditory afferent neurons and examine the hindbrain Aud/M synapses we transplanted cells from non-transgenic, BD-injected embryos into an M/CoLo:GFP host. To mark the postsynaptic Mauthner neurons in experiments examining the Aud/M synapse, and in all experiments in the spinal cord examining M/CoLo synapses, we transplanted cells from M/CoLo:GFP transgenic donors into a non-transgenic host. Such chimaeric animals allowed us to unambiguously identify from which embryo the neurons of the Mauthner circuit were derived based on the presence or absence of the markers used in each experiment.

gjd1a/cx34.1 and gjd2a/cx35.5 are required asymmetrically at Mauthner electrical synapses.

Dorsal views of chimeric larvae containing Biotin-Dextran- (BD) or GFP-marked cells transplanted from a donor embryo of noted genotype into a wildtype (wt) host; throughout the figure the neurons derived from the donor embryo are displayed in magenta, while those from the host are in blue. Synapses (stained with anti-human-Cx36, yellow) associated with a transplanted neuron (cyan circles and boxes) can be directly compared to wildtype host synapses (red circles and boxes). Hindbrain, lateral dendrite, and spinal cord images are maximum intensity projections of ~30,~5, and ~10 uM, respectively. Anterior is to the left. Scale bar = 10 uM. (A–C) At the Aud/M synapses presynaptic auditory afferent neurons (in A, stained with BD, magenta) synapse onto the postsynaptic Mauthner lateral dendrite (stained with anti-GFP, blue in A, magenta in B,C). (D,E) At the M/CoLo synapses the presynaptic Mauthner axon (stained with anti-GFP, magenta in D) synapses with the postsynaptic CoLo (stained with anti-GFP, magenta in E). (F–J) Presynaptic removal of gjd1a/cx34.1 function has no effect on Aud/M and M/CoLo synapses (cyan circles in F,I). By contrast, removing gjd1a/cx34.1 postsynaptically causes a loss of electrical synapse staining (cyan boxes and circles in G,H,J; note residual Cx36 staining at Aud/M synapses when gjd1a/cx34.1 is removed from only the postsynaptic neuron, (H). (K–O) Conversely, gjd2a/cx35.5 function is required exclusively presynaptically (K,N) and is dispensable postsynaptically (L,M,O). (P,Q) Quantitation of the ratio of Cx36 at donor-associated synapses to wildtype host synapses in chimaeric embryos. Each circle represents the average ratio of 1–8 donor-associated to 8–12 host-associated synapses within an animal, varying depending on the synapse and chimaera. Associated quantitation of Cx36 at chimeric synapses can be found in Figure 6—source data 1 for Figure 6 and Figure 6—source data 2 for Figure 6.

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.25364.013
Figure 6—source data 1

gjd1a/cx34.1 and gjd2a/cx35.5 are required asymmetrically at Mauthner electrical synapses.

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.25364.014
Figure 6—source data 2

gjd1a/cx34.1 and gjd2a/cx35.5 are required asymmetrically at Mauthner electrical synapses.

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.25364.015

In control chimeras with donor-derived presynaptic or postsynaptic neurons we saw no effect on Cx36 staining at Aud/M or M/CoLo synapses (Figure 6A–E). Similarly, when presynaptic neurons (auditory afferent neurons in the hindbrain [Figure 6F] or Mauthner in the spinal cord [Figure 6I]) lacked gjd1a/cx34.1 the electrical synapses were normal. However, when the postsynaptic neuron (Mauthner in the hindbrain [Figure 6G,H] or CoLo in the spinal cord [Figure 6J]) lacked gjd1a/cx34.1 the electrical synapses were affected. Conversely, we found exactly the opposite requirement for gjd2a/cx35.5: removing its function presynaptically, but not postsynaptically, resulted in the loss of associated electrical synapses (Figure 6K–O). Moreover, presynaptic removal of gjd2a/cx35.5 resulted in a complete loss of Cx36 staining at any associated synapse, while postsynaptic loss of gjd1a/cx34.1 resulted in no staining at M/CoLo synapses and an ~4 fold reduction of Cx36 staining at Aud/M synapses (Figure 6P,Q), consistent with the decrease observed in whole mutant animals (Figure 3J,K). This suggests that at M/CoLo synapses Cx35.5 and Cx34.1 are the exclusive pre- and postsynaptic Cxs, respectively, while at the Aud/M synapses Cx35.5 is the sole presynaptic Cx while Cx34.1 is required postsynaptically for the majority of GJ channels (Figure 6P,Q). We note that even within the single Mauthner neuron both Cx-encoding genes are required for electrical synapse formation, but their functions are spatially restricted to the dendrite (gjd1a/cx34.1, Figure 6G,H) versus the axon (gjd2a/cx35.5, Figure 6N). We conclude that gjd2a/cx35.5 and gjd1a/cx34.1 are required pre- and postsynaptically, respectively, at Mauthner circuit electrical synapses.

The above transplant experiments suggest that some GJ channels at Mauthner electrical synapses are heterotypic, with unique Cxs used on each side of the synapse. Moreover, our results suggest that a trans-synaptic interaction between the Cxs is required for stabilization, and thereby detection via antibody staining, at the Mauthner synapses. This would help explain why when we remove either gjd2a/cx35.5 or gjd1a/cx34.1 we observe reduced staining of the other Cx at the synapse. This model predicts that in a gjd2a/Cx35.5 mutant the postsynaptic Cx34.1 should still be available to make electrical synapses if provided with an appropriate presynaptic Cx (and vice versa). To test this notion and ask if Cx35.5 or Cx34.1 are sufficient pre- and postsynaptically, respectively, we created chimeric animals as above except we transplanted from wildtype M/CoLo:GFP embryos into a non-transgenic mutant host (Figure 7). We found that when the postsynaptic neuron was wildtype in a gjd1a/cx34.1 mutant host the electrical synapses were rescued (Figure 7A,B,D); however, when the presynaptic neuron was wildtype there was no rescue (Figure 7C). Conversely, a wildtype presynaptic neuron was sufficient to rescue the synapse in a gjd2a/cx35.5 mutant (Figure 7G), but had no effect when the wildtype neuron was postsynaptic (Figure 7E,F,H). In other words, Cx35.5 is sufficient to rescue the synapse if it is present in the presynaptic neurons while Cx34.1 is sufficient to rescue the synapse if it is present in the postsynaptic neuron. We note that the fold-rescue of Cx36 staining by a wildtype postsynaptic neuron was greater at M/CoLo as compared to Aud/M synapse (Figure 7I,J); this ~4 fold rescue is consistent with the fold reduction seen in gjd1a mutants and postsynaptic loss of gjd1a at these same synapses (see Figure 3J,K and Figure 6P,Q). Altogether we conclude that electrical synapse formation in the Mauthner circuit requires presynaptic Cx35.5 and postsynaptic Cx34.1 and that robust Cx localization at the synapse requires a trans-synaptic interaction with an appropriate Cx in an adjacent cell.

The exclusive asymmetric functions of gjd1a/cx34.1 and gjd2a/cx35.5 are sufficient for electrical synapse formation.

Dorsal views of chimeric larvae containing GFP-marked cells transplanted from a wildtype (wt) donor embryo into a mutant host of noted genotype; throughout the figure the neurons derived from the wt donor embryo are displayed in magenta, while those from the mutant host are in blue. Synapses (stained with anti-human-Cx36, yellow) associated with a transplanted neuron (cyan circles and boxes) can be directly compared to mutant host synapses (red circles and boxes). Hindbrain, lateral dendrite, and spinal cord images are maximum intensity projections of ~30, ~5, and ~10 uM, respectively. Anterior is to the left. Scale bar = 10 uM. (A–D) When the postsynaptic neuron of the Aud/M or M/CoLo synapse is wildtype in a gjd1a/cx34.1 mutant the electrical synapses are rescued (A,B,D). By contrast, when the presynaptic neuron is wildtype in a mutant there is no effect on Cx36 staining at the synapse (C). (E–H) Conversely, electrical synapses are rescued in gjd2a/cx35.5 mutants only when the presynaptic, and not the postsynaptic, neuron is wildtype. (I,J) Quantitation of the fold increase in the ratio of Cx36 at wt-donor-associated synapses to mutant host synapses in chimaeric embryos. Each circle represents the average ratio of 1–8 donor-associated to 8–16 host-associated synapses within an animal. Associated quantitation of Cx36 at chimeric synapses can be found in Figure 7—source data 1 for Figure 7 and Figure 7—source data 2 for Figure 7.

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.25364.016
Figure 7—source data 1

The exclusive asymmetric functions of gjd1a/cx34.1 and gjd2a/cx35.5 are sufficient for electrical synapse formation.

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.25364.017
Figure 7—source data 2

The exclusive asymmetric functions of gjd1a/cx34.1 and gjd2a/cx35.5 are sufficient for electrical synapse formation.

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.25364.018

Electrical synapses are required for speed and coordination of the Mauthner-induced escape response

To test how electrical synapses contributed to the escape response we examined the behavior of mutant and wildtype siblings using high-throughput behavioral analysis in free-swimming 6 dpf larvae (Wolman et al., 2015). The Mauthner circuit generates a fast escape response away from threatening stimuli using a combination of both electrical and chemical synapses (Fetcho, 1991); for an explanation of Mauthner circuit function see Figure 1B and associated discussion above. At 6 dpf there are approximately six auditory afferent neurons making mixed electrical/glutamatergic chemical synapses onto Mauthner (Aud/M); the electrical component of the postsynaptic response occurs 1–3 ms after afferent stimulation and happens just prior to that of its glutamatergic counterpart (Yao et al., 2014). By contrast, electrophysiology at the M/CoLo synapse suggests a single, excitatory electrical coupling allowing Mauthner to activate CoLo during an escape (Satou et al., 2009). Thus, electrical synapses are suggested to have unique effects on the initiation (Aud/M) and coordination (M/CoLo) of the Mauthner-induced escape response.

To test mutant behavior animals were placed into individual chambers in a multi-well testing stage illuminated by infrared light and were subjected to startling vibrational (acoustic) stimuli. Behavior was monitored with a high speed camera (1000 frames per second) to capture the kinematics, or body movements, of responses. Each animal and its behavioral response was analyzed using FLOTE software (Burgess and Granato, 2007), which automatically tracks movement parameters of the larvae. FLOTE can distinguish between Mauthner-induced short latency C-bend (SLC) escape responses and the longer latency escapes generated by related but distinct circuits (Burgess and Granato, 2008). We analyzed the kinematics of the 6 dpf larval escape response from crosses between heterozygous animals (incross of gjd1a/cx34.1+/− or gjd2a/cx35.5+/−). Behavioral testing and analysis was performed blind followed by genotyping of each larva and comparisons of responses between wildtype siblings and mutants. We found that mutants produced SLCs at similar frequencies compared to their wildtype siblings (Figure 8A, note that we found no difference between wildtype siblings from the gjd1a/cx34.1+/− or gjd2a/cx35.5+/− crosses and so have collapsed these two groups into a single wt data point in Figure 8; individual statistics can be found in the source data and lead to the same conclusions). However, we found that both gjd1a/cx34.1 and gjd2a/cx35.5 mutants initiated escape responses ~2 ms (40%) slower than their wildtype siblings (Figure 8B). In these ‘slow’ SLC responses the kinematics of the escape often had normal turn angles, maximum angular velocity, and other parameters known to be associated with Mauthner induced escapes, albeit happening later than in wildtype (Figure 8C–H). However, a subset of mutant responses (~20% in each genotype) occurred with an abnormally shallow maximum angle of the C-bend (Figure 8C, arrow) as well as abnormally slow maximum angular velocities (Figure 8D, arrow). These abnormal responses suggested a defect in performing the stereotyped C-bend shape of the escape response and so we reanalyzed the video data from these events and found that both gjd1a/cx34.1 and gjd2a/cx35.5 mutants produced abnormal postural responses to the auditory startle stimuli (Figure 8I–L). We found that as these animals initiated movements they displayed only slight bends to one side, creating ‘kinked’ or ‘S-shaped’ postures, and also shortening their body axis. These animals would stay in this state for 4–10 ms then produce a secondary movement in the opposite direction of the initial movement, followed most often by swimming behavior. These secondary movements were likely the normal counter bend and swimming motions that occur after a Mauthner induced escape (Burgess and Granato, 2008; Satou et al., 2009), but in these cases they occurred after a failed escape. From these data we conclude that electrical synapses contribute to the speed and coordination of the Mauthner induced escape response.

gjd1a/cx34.1 and gjd2a/cx35.5 mutants have delayed and abnormal escape responses.

M-induced escape responses (Short Latency C-bends, SLCs) to a startling vibrational (sound) stimuli executed by 6 day post fertilization larvae were analyzed by high-speed (1000 frame per second) videomicroscopy. Scale bar = 1 mM. (A) Frequency of elicited SLCs in wildtype (wt) and indicated mutants in 10 trials per animal. Note that the FLOTE analysis software removes some trials if they cannot be classified. Bar graphs represent data as mean ± SEM with each circle representing an individual animal’s average % of response. Mutant larvae execute escapes as frequently as WT (n = 52, 20, and 29 larvae for wt, gjd1a/cx34.1, and gjd2a/cx35.5, respectively; 1 way ANOVA not significant (ns), Dunn’s Multiple Comparison Test: wt to gjd1a/cx34.1 and wt to gjd2a/cx35.5 both ns). (B) Latency of elicited SLCs in all individual trials. Bar graphs represent data as mean ± SEM with each circle representing individual SLC latencies. Mutant larvae are significantly delayed in their latency to initiate an M-induced SLC (n = 359, 160, and 180 SLCs from 52, 20, and 29 larvae from wt, gjd1a/cx34.1, and gjd2a/cx35.5, respectively; 1 way ANOVA p<0.0001, Dunn’s Multiple Comparison Test: wt to gjd1a/cx34.1 and wt to gjd2a/cx35.5 both significant at p<0.001). (C,D). Kinematic analysis of the maximum SLC turn angle (C) and angular velocity (D) plotted as the average number of events within an indicated bin. Arrows indicate shallow angle and low velocity turns exhibited by mutants. (E–H) Time-lapse analysis of a normal (E,F) and delayed (G,H) M-induced escape response (SLCs). Individual snapshots taken at the indicated times (ms = milliseconds) are overlaid on an individual image (E,G). A line representing the midline body axis at each time was drawn by hand to indicate the movement (F,H). (I–L) A normal escape bend at its maximum angle (I) compared to abnormally shaped escape bends executed by gjd1a (J,K) or gjd2a (L) mutant larvae. Associated experimental statistics can be found in source data for Figure 8.

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.25364.019
Figure 8—source data 1

gjd1a/cx34.1 and gjd2a/cx35.5 mutants have delayed and abnormal escape responses.

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.25364.020

Discussion

Our genetic analyses in the zebrafish Mauthner circuit support a model where these vertebrate electrical synapses contain molecularly asymmetric gap junctions with unique pre- and postsynaptic Cxs (Figure 9A). One intriguing possibility is that differences in the pre- and postsynaptic Cxs would bias the passage of ionic flow through the GJ creating a rectified electrical connection (Palacios-Prado et al., 2014a). Indeed such is the case for two Innexins, Shaking-B(Neural +16) and Shaking-B(lethal), that in Drosophila form a molecularly asymmetric synapse; when these hemi-channels are paired in apposing Xenopus oocytes they allow for biased current flow (Phelan et al., 2008). In adult goldfish freeze-fracture immunolabeling and electron microscopy (EM) suggest that the Aud/M (club ending) synapses of Mauthner are molecularly asymmetric (see below for discussion of Cx usage) and electrophysiology found that ionic flow was preferentially directed from the post- to presynaptic terminal; the authors suggested that this functional asymmetry leads to cooperativity amongst the multiple converging auditory afferent axons thereby promoting the Mauthner escape response (Rash et al., 2013). Whether the configuration we find here of presynaptic Cx35.5 paired with postsynaptic Cx34.1 is sufficient to produce biased current flow across the channel is unclear; however Cx asymmetry may not act alone in biasing synaptic function. Cx function can be modified by differences in the intracellular milieu such as Mg++ concentration (Palacios-Prado et al., 2014b) and differential phosphorylation (Pereda, 2014) or perhaps different pre- and postsynaptic protein interactions may bias function. The molecular asymmetry of Cxs we identify suggests that GJ-associated proteins may also be asymmetrically distributed. Intriguingly, we previously found that the autism associated gene neurobeachin, which encodes a scaffolding protein thought to act within the trans-Golgi network, was required postsynaptically for electrical synaptogenesis (Miller et al., 2015). Future work is sure to extend our knowledge of molecular and functional asymmetry at electrical synapses where they are likely to be important for the development, function, and plasticity of neural circuits.

Schematics of the asymmetric functions of Cx proteins at zebrafish electrical synapses.

(A) Model of an electrical synapse with an exclusive pre- and postsynaptic Cx making up the gap junction plaque. We provide genetic evidence of this asymmetry at Mauthner synapses but it is likely broadly used in the nervous system. Such asymmetry is likely to drive functional asymmetry at these synapses (see Discussion). (B) Model of asymmetric electrical synapses found in concert with chemical synapses of the Mauthner circuit. The electrical synapses contribute speed and coordination to circuit function. The speed is likely imparted via the Aud/M synapses in the hindbrain while the coordination is likely via the M/CoLo synapses in the spinal cord (see Discussion).

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.25364.021

In our experiments we found that gjd1a/cx34.1 and gjd2a/cx35.5 are essential for the electrical synapses of the Mauthner circuit as well as many other synapses in zebrafish larvae. Previous freeze fracture immuno-EM work in goldfish has instead suggested that Cx35.1 and Cx34.7 may create the Aud/M synapses, yet we find that the genes encoding these two Cxs are not required in zebrafish (Figures 3 and 5). What accounts for this discrepancy? First, it is likely that the goldfish and zebrafish Aud/M synapses are constructed with the same Cxs, but this need not be the case and could account for differences. Moreover, the goldfish synapses were examined at adulthood while our work was in larval stages; it is conceivable that these electrical synapses could change their composition during development and maturation. Second, as others have shown (Satou et al., 2009; Yao et al., 2014), we found that antibodies generated against Cx34.7 and Cx35.1 and used in the goldfish studies (Rash et al., 2013) label zebrafish Aud/M and M/CoLo electrical synapses at larval stages from 3 to 7 dpf (see Materials and methods for antibody details). This, when put in the context of our genetic evidence for the requirement for gjd1a/cx34.1 postsynaptically and gjd2a/cx35.5 presynaptically, suggests that these previously used antibodies cross react with Cx34.1 and Cx35.5. Given the amino acid similarity of the two gjd1 proteins and the two gjd2 proteins (Figure 2), the most parsimonious scenario would be that the Cx34.7 antibody cross reacts with Cx34.1, while the Cx35.1 antibody cross reacts with Cx35.5. If correct, this would make for a consistent picture between the genetic evidence we present here and the goldfish immuno-EM data (Rash et al., 2013). Together, these data strongly support a model wherein the Aud/M synapses contain molecularly asymmetric gap junction channels. We also find that while Cx35.5 appears to be the sole presynaptic Cx at Aud/M synapses (Figure 6K), Cx34.1 may not act alone on the postsynaptic side of the synapse (Figure 6H). The nature of the ‘unidentified’ postsynaptic Cx is currently unknown but there are two main possibilities: (1) gjd2a/Cx35.5 may be used postsynaptically at these synapses or (2) an unidentified Cx is used postsynaptically to stabilize the observed Cx35.5 presynaptically. Whether the residual staining we observe in gjd1a/cx34.1 mutants at Aud/M gap junctions represents further Cx complexity (i.e. multiple postsynaptic Cxs) or instead is the result of compensatory changes only found in the mutant will require future experiments to resolve. Our genetic and chimaeric evidence also support Cx molecular asymmetry at M/CoLo synapses in the spinal cord (Figures 6 and 7). This configuration is likely common in zebrafish given the broad colocalization of Cx34.1 and Cx35.5 and their frequent co-requirement for localization (Figure 5). Molecular asymmetries at electrical synapses are not confined to fish, but are found broadly throughout the nervous systems of C. elegans (Starich et al., 2009), medicinal leech (Kandarian et al., 2012), D. melanogaster (Curtin et al., 1999; Phelan et al., 2008), crab (Shruti et al., 2014), and there have been suggestions, though no definitive proof, that such may occur in mammals as well (Cha et al., 2012; Haas et al., 2011; Zolnik and Connors, 2016). If such does occur in mammalian systems it would have to rely on a different set of Cxs than in zebrafish as mammals appear to have lost the gjd1/cx34 family of genes, leaving only the single, related gjd2/cx36 gene (Figure 2). However, there are four other Cxs known to make electrical synapses in mammals (Connors and Long, 2004; Söhl et al., 2005; Pereda, 2014), and the extent of their use and the configurations of GJs they create is poorly understood. We expect that molecularly asymmetric electrical synapses are likely to be a part of all nervous systems given that they underlie important functional effects on the properties of neural circuit computation (Phelan et al., 2008; Rash et al., 2013).

How do the electrical synapses contribute to neural circuit computation and behavioral output? We found that the cx mutants are slower to respond to vibrational stimuli, but can still produce many fast escape responses consistent with Mauthner-induced activation (Figure 8). We prefer the model that the reduction in response speed is due to the loss of the electrical component of the Aud/M synapses (Figure 9B) given that these synapses are required for Mauthner activation and sufficient to create the fast escape response (O'Malley et al., 1996; Tabor et al., 2014). This idea is supported by electrophysiological recordings of Aud/M synapses in wildtype larval zebrafish at stages similar to those we analyzed where the electrical response occurs ~2 milliseconds faster than that of the chemical synaptic response (Yao et al., 2014). It is important to note that we cannot rule out the possibility that electrical synapses upstream of Mauthner are required for the observed defects, or that the loss of electrical synapses affects chemical synapse function within the Mauthner circuit. We also observed that a subset of escape responses in cx mutants display coordination defects. This is likely due to the loss of the M/CoLo synapses in the spinal cord as similar behavioral defects have been observed when CoLos are laser ablated or when strychnine is used to block inhibitory synapses (Satou et al., 2009; Marsden and Granato, 2015). These uncoordinated responses likely only occur in a subset of escapes given that the CoLos are productively engaged in the behavioral output only in cases where both Mauthner neurons fire in response to the stimulus (Satou et al., 2009). In wildtype when both Mauthners fire turning is unidirectional; this is achieved by the first-activated Mauthner’s action potential exciting contralateral CoLos, which then recross the spinal cord where they inhibit the motor neurons before the ‘second-activated’ Mauthner’s action potential arrives – this ensures a unidirectional turn (see Figure 9B for circuit diagram)(Fetcho, 1991; Satou et al., 2009). When calcium activity of Mauthner is monitored in experimental paradigms similar to those we used it was found that both Mauthner neurons were activated at rates in line with the behavioral defects we observe (Satou et al., 2009; Marsden and Granato, 2015). Thus it is likely that the coordination defects we observe in mutants are due to the loss of the M/CoLo synapses. Our results on Mauthner-induced behavior with and without neuronal gap junctions reveal a critical interplay between electrical and chemical synapses in creating the performance needed for the escape response. While we have focused mainly on the Mauthner circuit here, given the breadth of Cx34.1/Cx35.5-based electrical synapses observed, we expect to identify broad contributions of the electrical synapses to many behaviors in zebrafish. Indeed, loss of gjd2/cx36 in mice results in defects in an ever increasing list of neurological functions including vision (Güldenagel et al., 2001; Cowan et al., 2016), smell (Christie et al., 2005), thalamic function (Zolnik and Connors, 2016), estrous cyclicity (Campbell et al., 2011), short-term spatial memory (Allen et al., 2011), motor performance (Frisch et al., 2005), as well as others. Future work in zebrafish and mouse will illuminate how electrical and chemical synapses collaborate in these complex, vertebrate nervous systems to produce appropriate behavior.

Materials and methods

Fish, lines, and maintenance

Request a detailed protocol

All animals were raised in an Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (IACUC)-approved facility at the Fred Hutchinson Cancer Research Center (Study ID 50552, Submittal ID 7237, IRO # 1392). Zebrafish, Danio rerio, were bred and maintained as previously described (Kimmel et al., 1995). Rachel Garcia provided animal care and Dr. Rajesh K. Uthamanthil, DVM, provided veterinary care. gjd1afh360(dis3) was isolated from an early-pressure, gynogenetic diploid screen (Walker et al., 2009) using ENU as a mutagen and animals were outcrossed from an *AB background into a Tuebingen background for mapping. gjd1afh436, gjd1bfh435, gjd2afh437, and gjd2bfh454 were generated using TALENs (Sanjana et al., 2012) targeting the first exon. gjd2bfh329 was generated via TILLING (Kettleborough et al., 2011). See Figure 2 and source data for information on all alleles. Stable zebrafish lines carrying each mutation were Sanger sequenced to verify mutations. All were maintained in the M/CoLo:GFP (Et(Tol-056:GFP)) background (Satou et al., 2009), which is a *AB/Tu background.

RNA-seq-based mutant mapping

Request a detailed protocol

Larvae in the F3 generation were collected at three dpf from crosses of known gjd1afh360(dis3) heterozygous animals, were anesthetized with MESAB (Sigma, A5040), and the posterior portion was removed and fixed for phenotypic analysis via immunohistochemistry (see below) while the anterior portion was placed in Trizol (Life Technologies, 15596–026), homogenized, and frozen to preserve RNA for sequencing. After phenotypic identification mutant (-/-) and wildtype sibling (+/+ and +/-) RNA was pooled separately from 108 embryos each. From each pool total RNA was extracted and cDNA libraries were created using standard Illumina TruSeq protocols. Each library was individually barcoded allowing for identification after multiplexed sequencing on an Illumina HiSeq 2000 machine. In brief, mapping was performed by identifying high quality ‘mapping’ single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) in the wildtype pool and assessing these positions in the mutant pool for their frequency. The average allele frequency in mutants, using a sliding-window of 50-neighboring loci, was plotted across the genome and linkage was identified as the region of highest average frequency. Within the linked region candidate mutations causing nonsense or missense changes, or those affecting gene expression levels, were identified as previously described (Miller et al., 2013). Details can be found at www.RNAmapper.org.

Cloning and characterization of connexin genes

Request a detailed protocol

We used the gjd1a/cx34.1 sequences to search the Zv10 genome of zebrafish for other genes with similar sequence. From these searches we identified four loci, gjd1a/cx34.1 (chr5), gjd1b/cx34.7 (chr15), gjd2a/cx35.5 (chr17), and gjd2b/cx35.1 (chr20), as the only locations with significant homology. All four are two-exon genes, however the gene encoding Cx35.5 is annotated on the zebrafish Zv10 genome as having the first exon downstream and in the opposite orientation of the second exon. This arrangement is most likely due to an inappropriately assembled region of the genome for three reasons: (1) we cloned a full-length transcript from a cDNA library using primers to the 5’- and 3’-UTRs, (2) the gene results in a protein product, (3) the gene results in a protein detectable by an antibody made against it (see results for points 2 and 3).

Antibody generation and validation

Request a detailed protocol

Antibodies were generated at the Fred Hutchinson Antibody Technology Facility (https://sharedresources.fredhutch.org/core-facilities/antibody-technology). We used the variable regions of the intracellular loop from gjd1a/Cx34.1 and gjd2a/Cx35.5 (Figure 2A,B) to generate peptide antibodies to each protein and first tested the specificity of antibodies on each Cx protein individually expressed in HeLa cells. HeLa CCL-2 cells were obtained as an authenticated and mycoplasma free line from ATCC (ATCC Number: CCL-2) and the original cells and early passages of the cells were frozen back and maintained in liquid nitrogen. Cells were thawed and utilized under low passage conditions with all experiments done on lines split fewer than 30 times. HeLa cells were used as previous work established them as a standard method for Cx protein expression and antibody validation (Rash et al., 2013). We found several antibodies against each Cx that specifically recognized only the intended target Cx and not the others and screened each of these using whole-mount immunohistochemistry of 5 dpf zebrafish larvae. We found that these antibodies generated staining patterns highlighting the same structures in the midbrain, hindbrain, and spinal cord as staining with the antibody against human Cx36 used above (Figure 5 and associated source data). The human anti-Cx36 antibody was also found to recognize all four zebrafish Cx36-related proteins (Figure 5 and associated source data). We did not detect staining in areas anterior of the midbrain, but we did not perform the sectioning required to recognize electrical synapses in the retina and other anterior regions (Li et al., 2009). In gjd1a/cx34.1 mutants we found that staining for Cx34.1 protein was lost while in the gjd2a/cx35.5 mutants we found that Cx35.5 protein staining was lost supporting both the specificity of the antibodies as well as the deleterious nature of the mutations (Figure 5).

Immunohistochemistry

Request a detailed protocol

Anesthetized embryos from 2 to 14 dpf were fixed in either 2% trichloroacetic (TCA) acid for 3 hr or 4% paraformaldehyde (PFA) for 1 hr. Fixed tissue was then washed in PBS + 0.5% TritonX100, followed by standard blocking and antibody incubations. Tissue was cleared step-wise in a 25%, 50%, 75% glycerol series and was dissected and mounted for imaging. Primary antibodies used were: chicken anti-GFP (abcam, ab13970, 1:250), rabbit anti-human-Cx36 (Invitrogen, 36–4600, 1:200), rabbit anti-Cx34.7 against the intracellular loop peptide of the perch protein (JOB, 1:20)(O'Brien et al., 2004), mouse anti-Cx35.1 against recombinant perch protein (EMD/Millipore, MAB3045, 1:200)(O'Brien et al., 2004), rabbit anti-Cx34.1 and mouse anti-Cx35.5 (both generated in this study, 1:100), mouse anti-RMO44 (Life Technologies, 13–0500, 1:100). All secondary antibodies were raised in goat (Life Technologies, conjugated with Alexa-405,–488, −555,–594, or −633 fluorophores, 1:250). Neurobiotin and Biotin Dextran were detected using fluorescently-tagged streptavidin (Life Technologies, conjugated with Alexa-633 fluorophores, 1:500).

Neurobiotin retrograde labeling

Request a detailed protocol

Anesthetized 5 dpf embryos were mounted in 1% agar and a caudal transsection through the dorsal half of the embryo was made with an insect pin at somite 20–25. A second insect pin loaded with 5% neurobiotin (Nb) solution was quickly applied to the incision. Animals were unmounted from the agar and allowed to rest for 3 hr while anesthetized to allow neurobiotin to pass from Mauthner into the CoLos. Animals were then fixed in 4% PFA for 2 hr and processed for immunohistochemistry. CoLo axons project posteriorly for a maximum of two segments; therefore measurements of Nb in CoLo were analyzed at least three segments away from the lesion site.

Cell transplantation

Request a detailed protocol

Cell transplantation was done using standard techniques at the blastula stage (Kemp et al., 2009). For ‘mutant into wildtype’ experiments examining the spinal cord M/CoLo synapses and assessing the postsynaptic Mauthner of Aud/M synapses, animals heterozygous for a cx mutation and carrying the M/CoLo:GFP transgene were incrossed while hosts were non-transgenic AB; for ‘wildtype into mutant’, M/CoLo:GFP animals were incrossed while hosts were an incross of animals heterozygous for a cx mutation that were not transgenic. For assessing the presynaptic auditory afferent contribution to the Aud/M synapses embryos were injected with biotin-dextran at the 1 cell stage and later transplanted from appropriate genotypes. Approximately 20 cells were deposited 1–5 cell diameters away from the margin at the sphere/dome stage (4–4.5 hpf) with a single embryo donating to 3–5 hosts. Embryos were allowed to grow until 5 dpf at which point they were processed for immunostaining. Donor (mutant to wildtype) or host (wildtype to mutant) embryos were genotyped.

Confocal imaging and analysis

Request a detailed protocol

All images were collected on a Zeiss LSM 700 confocal microscope using 405, 488, 555, and 639 laser lines, with each line’s data being collected sequentially using standard pre-programmed filters for the appropriate Alexa dyes. All Z-stacks used 1 uM steps. Images were processed and analyzed using Fiji (Schindelin et al., 2012) software. Within each experiment all animals were stained together with the same antibody mix, processed at the same time, and all confocal settings (gain, offset, objective, zoom) were identical. For quantitating fluorescent intensity, synapses were defined as the region of contact between the neurons of interest (Aud/M or M/CoLo). A standard region of interest (ROI) surrounding each synapse was drawn and the mean fluorescent intensity was measured across at least 4 uMs in the Z-direction with the highest value being recorded. For neurobiotin backfills fluorescent intensity was measured using a standard ROI encompassing the entire Mauthner or CoLo cell body. Statistics were computed using Prism software (GraphPad). Figure images were created using Fiji, Photoshop (Adobe) and Illustrator (Adobe). Colors for all figures were modified using the Fiji plugin Figure 5D.

Behavioral imaging and analysis

Request a detailed protocol

Behavioral experiments were performed on six dpf larvae as previously described (Cruciani and Mikalsen, 2007; Wolman et al., 2011, 2015). Briefly, larvae were placed in individual wells of a 16-well grid and exposed to 3 ms, 1000 Hz acoustico-vibrational stimuli delivered by an acoustic shaker at 26 dB. Stimulus intensities were calibrated with a PCB Piezotronics accelerometer (model #355B04). All animals received 10 stimuli separated by 20 s, which is sufficient to ensure that no habituation occurs over the course of the experiment (Wolman et al., 2011). Movements were captured using a high-speed camera (RedLake MotionPro) at 1000 frames/sec, and movies were analyzed with the FLOTE software package as previously described (Burgess and Granato, 2007). Short latency C-bend startle responses (SLCs) driven by Mauthner were measured and defined by the previously established kinematic parameters of initiation latency, turning angle, turn duration, and maximum angular velocity (Burgess and Granato, 2007). After testing, larvae were transferred to 96-well plates and processed for subsequent genotyping.

Data availability

The following data sets were generated
    1. Miller AC
    (2013) Dis2 RNA-seq wildtype and mutant
    Publicly available at the NCBI Sequence Read Archive (accession no: PRJNA172016).

References

    1. Goodenough DA
    2. Paul DL
    (2009) Gap junctions
    Cold Spring Harbor Perspectives in Biology 1:a002576.
    https://doi.org/10.1101/cshperspect.a002576
    1. Güldenagel M
    2. Ammermüller J
    3. Feigenspan A
    4. Teubner B
    5. Degen J
    6. Söhl G
    7. Willecke K
    8. Weiler R
    (2001)
    Visual transmission deficits in mice with targeted disruption of the gap junction gene connexin36
    Journal of Neuroscience 21:6036–6044.
    1. Pereda A
    2. O'Brien J
    3. Nagy JI
    4. Bukauskas F
    5. Davidson KG
    6. Kamasawa N
    7. Yasumura T
    8. Rash JE
    (2003)
    Connexin35 mediates electrical transmission at mixed synapses on mauthner cells
    Journal of Neuroscience 23:7489–7503.

Article and author information

Author details

  1. Adam C Miller

    Division of Basic Sciences, Fred Hutchinson Cancer Research Center, Seattle, United States
    Present address
    Institute of Neuroscience, University of Oregon, Eugene, United States
    Contribution
    ACM, Conceptualization, Resources, Data curation, Formal analysis, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Validation, Investigation, Visualization, Methodology, Writing—original draft, Project administration, Writing—review and editing
    Contributed equally with
    Alex C Whitebirch
    For correspondence
    acmiller@uoregon.edu
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0001-7519-3677
  2. Alex C Whitebirch

    Division of Basic Sciences, Fred Hutchinson Cancer Research Center, Seattle, United States
    Contribution
    ACW, Formal analysis, Validation, Investigation, Visualization, Methodology, Writing—original draft, Writing—review and editing
    Contributed equally with
    Adam C Miller
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0002-9178-1636
  3. Arish N Shah

    Division of Basic Sciences, Fred Hutchinson Cancer Research Center, Seattle, United States
    Contribution
    ANS, Investigation, Methodology
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  4. Kurt C Marsden

    Department of Cell and Developmental Biology, University of Pennsylvania Perelman School of Medicine, Philadelphia, United States
    Contribution
    KCM, Conceptualization, Resources, Investigation, Methodology, Writing—review and editing
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  5. Michael Granato

    Department of Cell and Developmental Biology, University of Pennsylvania Perelman School of Medicine, Philadelphia, United States
    Contribution
    MG, Funding acquisition, Writing—review and editing
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  6. John O'Brien

    Department of Ophthalmology and Visual Science, McGovern Medical School, University of Texas Health Sciences Center at Houston, Houston, United States
    Contribution
    JO, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Writing—review and editing
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0002-0270-3442
  7. Cecilia B Moens

    Division of Basic Sciences, Fred Hutchinson Cancer Research Center, Seattle, United States
    Contribution
    CBM, Funding acquisition, Project administration, Writing—review and editing
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.

Funding

National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (F32NS074839)

  • Adam C Miller

National Institute of Mental Health (R01MH109498)

  • Michael Granato

National Eye Institute (R01EY012857)

  • John O'Brien

Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (R01HD076585)

  • Cecilia B Moens

National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (R21NS076950)

  • Cecilia B Moens

National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (K99/R00NS085035)

  • Adam C Miller

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Acknowledgements

We thank Rachel Garcia for superb animal care, Kathryn Helde for help with the forward genetic screen, Lila Solnica-Krezel for ENU-mutagenized male zebrafish, Shin-Ichi Higashijima for the M/CoLo:GFP line, and the Fred Hutchinson Cancer Research Center’s Genomic Resource Center, particularly Jeff Delrow, Andy Marty, Alyssa Dawson, and Ryan Basom, for sequencing library preparation, sequencing, and help in data processing. Funding was provided by the National Institute of Health, F32NS074839 and K99/R00NS085035 to ACM, R01MH109498 to MG, R01EY012857 to JO, and R01HD076585 and R21NS076950 to CBM.

Ethics

Animal experimentation: All animals were raised in an Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (IACUC)-approved facility at the Fred Hutchinson Cancer Research Center (Study ID 50552, Submittal ID 7237, IRO # 1392).

Copyright

© 2017, Miller et al.

This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.

Metrics

  • 2,142
    views
  • 421
    downloads
  • 43
    citations

Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.

Download links

A two-part list of links to download the article, or parts of the article, in various formats.

Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)

Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)

Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)

  1. Adam C Miller
  2. Alex C Whitebirch
  3. Arish N Shah
  4. Kurt C Marsden
  5. Michael Granato
  6. John O'Brien
  7. Cecilia B Moens
(2017)
A genetic basis for molecular asymmetry at vertebrate electrical synapses
eLife 6:e25364.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.25364

Share this article

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.25364

Further reading

    1. Neuroscience
    John P Grogan, Matthias Raemaekers ... Sanjay G Manohar
    Research Article

    Motivation depends on dopamine, but might be modulated by acetylcholine which influences dopamine release in the striatum, and amplifies motivation in animal studies. A corresponding effect in humans would be important clinically, since anticholinergic drugs are frequently used in Parkinson’s disease, a condition that can also disrupt motivation. Reward and dopamine make us more ready to respond, as indexed by reaction times (RT), and move faster, sometimes termed vigour. These effects may be controlled by preparatory processes that can be tracked using electroencephalography (EEG). We measured vigour in a placebo-controlled, double-blinded study of trihexyphenidyl (THP), a muscarinic antagonist, with an incentivised eye movement task and EEG. Participants responded faster and with greater vigour when incentives were high, but THP blunted these motivational effects, suggesting that muscarinic receptors facilitate invigoration by reward. Preparatory EEG build-up (contingent negative variation [CNV]) was strengthened by high incentives and by muscarinic blockade, although THP reduced the incentive effect. The amplitude of preparatory activity predicted both vigour and RT, although over distinct scalp regions; frontal activity predicted vigour, whereas a larger, earlier, central component predicted RT. The incentivisation of RT was partly mediated by the CNV, though vigour was not. Moreover, the CNV mediated the drug’s effect on dampening incentives, suggesting that muscarinic receptors underlie the motivational influence on this preparatory activity. Taken together, these findings show that a muscarinic blocker impairs motivated action in healthy people, and that medial frontal preparatory neural activity mediates this for RT.

    1. Neuroscience
    Samyogita Hardikar, Bronte Mckeown ... Jonathan Smallwood
    Research Article

    Complex macro-scale patterns of brain activity that emerge during periods of wakeful rest provide insight into the organisation of neural function, how these differentiate individuals based on their traits, and the neural basis of different types of self-generated thoughts. Although brain activity during wakeful rest is valuable for understanding important features of human cognition, its unconstrained nature makes it difficult to disentangle neural features related to personality traits from those related to the thoughts occurring at rest. Our study builds on recent perspectives from work on ongoing conscious thought that highlight the interactions between three brain networks – ventral and dorsal attention networks, as well as the default mode network. We combined measures of personality with state-of-the-art indices of ongoing thoughts at rest and brain imaging analysis and explored whether this ‘tri-partite’ view can provide a framework within which to understand the contribution of states and traits to observed patterns of neural activity at rest. To capture macro-scale relationships between different brain systems, we calculated cortical gradients to describe brain organisation in a low-dimensional space. Our analysis established that for more introverted individuals, regions of the ventral attention network were functionally more aligned to regions of the somatomotor system and the default mode network. At the same time, a pattern of detailed self-generated thought was associated with a decoupling of regions of dorsal attention from regions in the default mode network. Our study, therefore, establishes that interactions between attention systems and the default mode network are important influences on ongoing thought at rest and highlights the value of integrating contemporary perspectives on conscious experience when understanding patterns of brain activity at rest.