Dynamic modulation of activity in cerebellar nuclei neurons during pavlovian eyeblink conditioning in mice
Abstract
While research on the cerebellar cortex is crystallizing our understanding of its function in learning behavior, many questions surrounding its downstream targets remain. Here, we evaluate the dynamics of cerebellar interpositus nucleus (IpN) neurons over the course of Pavlovian eyeblink conditioning. A diverse range of learning-induced neuronal responses was observed, including increases and decreases in activity during the generation of conditioned blinks. Trial-by-trial correlational analysis and optogenetic manipulation demonstrate that facilitation in the IpN drives the eyelid movements. Adaptive facilitatory responses are often preceded by acquired transient inhibition of IpN activity that, based on latency and effect, appear to be driven by complex spikes in cerebellar cortical Purkinje cells. Likewise, during reflexive blinks to periocular stimulation, IpN cells show excitation-suppression patterns that suggest a contribution of climbing fibers and their collaterals. These findings highlight the integrative properties of subcortical neurons at the cerebellar output stage mediating conditioned behavior.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.28132.001Introduction
The cerebellar cortex, like the neocortex, is well suited for establishing new associations required during memory formation. It is becoming increasingly clear that, like cortical and subcortical structures (e.g. Constantinople and Bruno, 2013; Koralek et al., 2012; Igarashi et al., 2014; Douglas et al., 1995; Sherman and Guillery, 2004), the cerebellar cortex and nuclei exhibit complex interplay, for instance through reciprocal nucleo-cortical projections (Gao et al., 2016). Classical Pavlovian eyeblink conditioning has proven an ideal model for studying the neural mechanisms underlying associative learning, and offers a suitable paradigm to address the question of how the cerebellar nuclei integrate their cerebellar cortical and extra-cerebellar input.
As a simple and quintessential behavioral manifestation of learning and memory, eyeblink conditioning depends on the cerebellar cortex and nuclei (McCormick et al., 1982; McCormick and Thompson, 1984; Yeo et al., 1985a1985, 1985b), which are known to facilitate processes that require precise timing (Ivry and Keele, 1989; Breska and Ivry, 2016). In short, animals learn to respond to a neutral conditional stimulus (CS), such as a light, with a well-timed conditioned blink response (CR), when the CS is consistently paired at a fixed temporal interval with an unconditional blink-eliciting stimulus (US), such as a corneal air puff. Lobule HVI of the cerebellar cortex and its downstream target, the interposed nucleus (IpN), are essential for the manifestation of this conditioned eyelid behavior (McCormick et al., 1982; McCormick and Thompson, 1984; Yeo et al., 1985a1985, 1985b; Clark et al., 1992; Krupa and Thompson, 1997; Ohyama et al., 2006; Mostofi et al., 2010; Heiney et al., 2014b). Genetic and pharmacological manipulations of cerebellar cortical Purkinje cells indicate that the expression of CRs may require various cell physiological processes, including plasticity at the excitatory parallel fiber to Purkinje cell synapse (Ito and Kano, 1982; Koekkoek et al., 2003; Schonewille et al., 2010), inhibition at the molecular layer interneuron to Purkinje cell synapse (ten Brinke et al., 2015), as well as intrinsic mGluR7-mediated processes in Purkinje cells (Johansson et al., 2015). IpN neurons that can drive eyeblink behavior through the red nucleus and facial nucleus provide a feedback to the cerebellar cortex that amplifies the CR (Gao et al., 2016; Giovannucci et al., 2017). Moreover, IpN neurons receive excitatory inputs from mossy fiber and climbing fiber collaterals, which need to be integrated with the inhibitory inputs from the Purkinje cells and local interneurons and possibly even from recurrent collaterals of the nucleo-olivary neurons (Chan-Palay, 1973, Chan-Palay, 1977; de Zeeuw et al., 1988, 1997; Van der Want et al., 1989; Uusisaari et al., 2007; Uusisaari and Knöpfel, 2008, 2011, 2012; Bagnall et al., 2009; Kodama et al., 2012; Witter et al., 2013; Boele et al., 2013; Najac and Raman, 2015; Canto et al., 2016).
IpN neurons and their inputs are endowed with ample forms of plasticity, all of which may in principle be implicated in eyeblink conditioning (Ohyama et al., 2006; Foscarin et al., 2011; Zheng and Raman, 2010). These include for example short-term and long-term potentiation (LTP) at the mossy fiber to IpN neuron synapse (Person and Raman, 2010), both of which may facilitate the induction of intrinsic plasticity (Aizenman and Linden, 1999 and Aizenman and Linden, 2000; Zheng and Raman, 2010), and learning-dependent structural outgrowth of mossy fiber collaterals in the cerebellar nuclei, which may be directly correlated to the rate and amplitude of CRs (Boele et al., 2013). So far, extracellular recordings of neurons in the cerebellar nuclei have revealed CR-related increases in activity that lead the eyeblink movement (anterior IpN; McCormick and Thompson, 1984; Berthier and Moore, 1990; Gould and Steinmetz, 1996; Choi and Moore, 2003; Halverson et al., 2010; Heiney et al., 2014b), and CR-related increases and decreases in activity that lag the eyeblink movement (posterior IpN; Gruart et al., 2000; Delgado-García and Gruart, 2005; Sánchez-Campusano et al., 2007). However, comprehensive trial-by-trial analysis of different components of IpN modulation in relation to conditioned behavior, as well as evaluation against conditioning-related modulation of activity in the cerebellar cortex and other non-cortical inputs, is lacking.
Here, we recorded conditioning-related activity in an identified blink area of the anterior interpositus nucleus of awake behaving mice, and used in-depth trial-by-trial correlational analysis, optogenetic manipulation and computationally modeled IpN output based on previously reported eyelid-related Purkinje cell modulation (ten Brinke et al., 2015) to detail and cement the causal role of IpN activity in conditioned behavior. Our results reveal an intricate dynamic modulation of cerebellar nuclei activity during the generation of conditioned movements. We discuss how these findings shed light on a number of electrophysiological properties of the olivocerebellar network as integrated at the IpN in the normal functional context of eyeblink conditioning in awake, behaving mice. Together, our results highlight how neurons in the IpN integrate cortical and non-cortical input to establish a circuitry optimally designed to generate well-timed conditioned motor responses.
Results
Characterization of cerebellar IpN neurons
To explore the characteristics of IpN spike modulation and its relation to conditioned eyelid behavior, we made extracellular recordings across 19 mice that were trained or in training. During recordings, the mice were head-fixed on top of a cylindrical treadmill, fully awake and able to show normal eyelid behavior in response to the experimental stimuli, which included a 260 ms green LED light (CS) that co-terminated with a 10 ms corneal air puff (US), yielding a 250 ms CS-US interval (Figure 1A).

Interpositus nucleus electrophysiology overview.
(A) CS and US signals are transmitted through mossy fibers and climbing fibers, respectively, to Purkinje cells in the simplex lobule (HVI) of the cerebellar cortex, and to the IpN, to which cortical Purkinje cells send their inhibitory projection. Combined disinhibition and excitation then lead the IpN to drive CRs. Paired trials consisted of a 260 ms LED light CS, co-terminating with a 10 ms corneal air puff. (B) Coronal cerebellar section showing a typical recording site in the IpN. C-E Example eyelid traces for trials where the recorded cell showed spike facilitation (C), suppression (D), or no modulation (E) in the CS-US interval. (F-H) Electrophysiological trace showing IpN activity corresponding to the example eyelid traces in (C-E). (I) Spike trace averages for 60 cells from the first dataset showing at least 5 Hz facilitation in the CS-US interval; grand average is shown with SEM. (J) Same as in I, but for 23 suppressive cells. (K) Same as in I, but for 187 non-modulating cells. (L) Average traces of eyelid behavior for all 270 cells in the first dataset, color coded for average CR amplitude at US onset. (M) Daily CS-US interval averages of facilitatory cells (top traces, orange), suppressive cells (bottom traces, blue), and eyelid behavior for all cells showing modulation (middle traces, teal). Black scale bar denotes 25 Hz/% eyelid closure. (N, O) Proportion of cells showing facilitation, suppression, or no modulation, across cells with <20% CRs (n = 167), and cells with >20% CRs (n = 103). IpN: interpositus nucleus; IO: inferior olive; MN: motor nucleus; PC: Purkinje cell; PN: pontine nuclei.
Initially, we collected 270 recordings of IpN cells that were located below Purkinje cell layers at depths between 1500–3000 μm (Figure 1B), that were recorded during at least 10 valid CS-US trials, ánd that showed modulation in their firing rate in response to the CS and/or US. Firing frequency was similar along the depth of the IpN (r = 0.085, p=0.16, n = 270, Pearson), averaging 67 ± 27 Hz. Within the entire dataset, 60 cells showed at least 5 Hz facilitation in the CS-US interval (22.2%, Figure 1C,F,I), and 23 cells showed at least 5 Hz suppression (8.5%, Figure 1D,G,J), leaving 187 cells that did not show any clear spike rate modulation within the CS-US interval (69.3%, Figure 1E,H,K; see Materials and methods).
In terms of eyelid behavior across the dataset (Figure 1L), CRs were present in at least 20% of trials in 103 cells (38%); this is the criterion used throughout the paper when referring to recordings with behavior. The magnitude of spike facilitation showed a steady significant increase over the course of conditioning (r = 0.413, p=0.001, n = 60; Spearman); spike suppression was similarly inclined (r = 0.354, p=0.0972, n = 23; Spearman; Figure 1M). Although both types of spike rate modulation manifested before conditioned behavior did (Figure 1M), they were more prevalent in recordings with conditioned behavior (Figure 1O) than in those without (Figure 1N; facilitation: 29.1% (30/103) vs. 18% (30/167), p=0.0463; suppression: 15.5% (16/103) vs. 4.2% (7/167), p=0.0025, Chi-square test).
To corroborate the main findings of the first dataset, an independently obtained, second dataset of 102 IpN recordings was analyzed in the same way. The collection of this dataset was optimized in that: (i) rather than over the course of conditioning, these recordings were obtained after thorough training, and only from mice that showed at least 80% CRs; (ii) the recordings were all located no more than ~300 μm away from IpN sites confirmed to be eyelid-related through micro-stimulation (see Materials and methods, and Heiney et al., 2014b); and (iii) the CS was 220 ms, the US was 20 ms, resulting in a CS-US interval of 200 ms. Thus, rather than focusing on the course of training and across-trial variability, the recordings of the second dataset served to capture cerebellar nuclear dynamics in an optimally and fully conditioned system (for an overview of the two different datasets, see Figure 1—figure supplement 1).
In subsequent analyses, we focused on three relevant time intervals during stimulus presentation: the last 200 ms of the CS-US interval, during which broad Purkinje cell simple spike modulation takes place; 50–130 ms after the CS, during which CS-related climbing fiber activity emerges (Ohmae and Medina, 2015; ten Brinke et al., 2015); and the first 60 ms after the US, during which robust climbing fiber signals take place (the data structure in Source data 1 contains these variables for both datasets).
IpN facilitation but not suppression can drive conditioned eyelid behavior
We first sought to characterize whether facilitation and suppression observed in IpN neurons during the last 200 ms of the CS-US interval correlates with the amplitude of eyelid closure (Figure 2). Across the first dataset, there were 60 cells showing CS-US facilitation, with durations of 104 ± 41 ms, and magnitudes averaging 22.5 ± 20.2 Hz and correlating to both percentage CRs (r = 0.534, p<0.0001, n = 60, Spearman) and CR amplitude (r = 0.52, p<0.0001, Spearman). Out of 30 facilitation cells recorded in the presence of CRs (Figure 2A), 17 showed significant positive trial-by-trial correlations between firing rate and CR amplitude (57%; p=0.0001, bootstrap with 500 repetitions; Figure 2B), whereas none showed negative correlations. A linear mixed model with random intercepts and slopes for these significant cells predicts 0.49% more eyelid closure at US onset per unit increase of spike facilitation in Hz (Figure 2B, black dotted line; p<0.0001, Supplementary file 1). Exploration of trial-by-trial spike-eyelid correlations across a correlation matrix reveals their temporal distribution, as explained previously (ten Brinke et al., 2015). Here, correlations within 20 ms windows, taken at 10 ms steps across the trial timespan, show at which spike times and which eyelid times the strongest spike-eyelid correlations exist, both when taking eyelid position (Figure 2C) and eyelid velocity (Figure 2—figure supplement 1A) as the lead parameter. A cluster of markedly positive correlations can be seen in the upper triangle of the matrix in the second half of the CS-US interval, running parallel to the diagonal. This means the strongest across-trial correlations occurred between spiking and subsequent behavior. Indeed, temporal cross-correlations between average spike and eyelid traces confirmed that spike facilitation fits best with subsequent eyelid behavior, averaging 25 ± 25.6 ms (p=0.0001, Wilcoxon signed rank test; Figure 2D).

CS-US interval IpN modulation relates to eyelid behavior.
(A) Average spike traces of 30 facilitatory neurons in the first dataset recorded with conditioned behavior. Black trace denotes average. Time is shown relative to CS onset. (B) Trial-by-trial spike-eyelid correlation lines for the cells in A, with plain red lines showing significant correlations, light red lines showing the non-significant ones, and the pie chart showing their proportionality. (C) Average correlation matrix showing the average temporal distribution of the spike-eyelid position correlations of the cells in A. R-values counter to the correlational direction of interest were nullified before averaging. (D) Temporal cross-correlations for the cells in A (light red) and their mean (plain red). Box plot shows the time of maximum correlation for all cells. (E-H) Same as in (A-D), here for 70 facilitation cells with conditioned behavior from the independent second dataset. (I-L) Same as in (A-D), here for 16 suppression cells recorded with conditioned behavior from the first dataset. (M-P) Same as in (I-L), here for 16 suppression cells recorded with conditioned behavior from the second dataset.
The character and predictive power of CS-US facilitation in the IpN was corroborated by the second dataset (Figure 2E–H), with 70 cells showing facilitation (59.2 ± 38 Hz) in the presence of conditioned behavior (Figure 2E), and 49 of these cells showing significant positive correlations (70%). A linear mixed model with random intercepts and slopes predicted 0.64% more eyelid closure at US onset per 1 Hz increase in facilitation (p<0.0001, Supplementary file 1). The correlation matrices relating spike activity to eyelid position (Figure 2G) and velocity (Figure 2—figure supplement 1B) showed hotspots of strong correlation hovering above the diagonal in the CS-US interval, with temporal cross-correlations again confirming the best fit between spike facilitation and eyelid behavior to be at significantly positive offsets (33.4 ± 19.3 ms, p<0.0001, Wilcoxon signed rank test; Figure 2H).
In the first dataset, CS-US suppression was less prevalent than facilitation. Within the 23 cells (8.5%) showing an average drop of at least 5 Hz in the CS-US interval (11.2 ± 6 Hz, duration: 70 ± 44 ms), the magnitude of suppression correlated to both CR percentage (r = 0.526, p=0.01, n = 23, Spearman) and CR amplitude at US onset (r = 0.443, p=0.0356, n = 23, Spearman). Additionally, cells with stronger suppression showed higher spike irregularity (i.e., higher CVs: r = 0.587, p=0.0038, Spearman). Compared to facilitation, suppression had lower amplitudes (baseline z-score: 5.3 ± 1.5 vs 7.6 ± 4.7, p=0.0007, Mann-Whitney U test, MWU) and lower durations (53 ± 38.4 vs 95.5 ± 36.2 ms, p=0.0012, MWU), but similar onset (132 ± 21.2 vs 136.5 ± 19.9 ms, p=0.6, MWU) and modulation peak times (176 ± 37.2 vs 166 ± 34.2 ms, p=0.6, MWU). Relative spike suppression was further correlated to eyelid closure at US-onset relative to baseline for all recordings of suppressive cells with conditioned behavior (n = 16, Figure 2I). Of these cells, three showed significant negative trial-by-trial correlations between firing rate and CR amplitude (19%, p<0.0001, bootstrap with 500 repetitions; Figure 2J), whereas none showed positive correlations. A linear mixed model with random intercepts and slopes for the three significant cells estimated 0.27% more eyelid closure at US onset per unitary increase of suppression in Hz (p=0.0272, Supplementary file 1). The correlation matrix showed no particular concentration of the spike-eyelid correlations of suppressive cells with conditioned behavior within the CS-US interval (position: Figure 2K; velocity: Figure 2—figure supplement 1C), and temporal cross-correlation showed a best fit between average spike and eyelid traces at very small temporal offsets that were on average close to zero (6.4 ± 23 ms, p=0.2336, Wilcoxon signed rank test; Figure 2L).
In the second dataset, 16 cells showed CS-US spike suppression (12.1 ± 5 Hz) in the presence of conditioned behavior (Figure 2M), with 8 cells showing a significant negative relationship between relative firing rate and CR amplitude at US onset (p<0.0001, bootstrap with 500 repetitions; Figure 2N) and a linear mixed model estimating 1.01% more eyelid closure per 1 Hz deeper suppression (p<0.0001, Supplementary file 1). Although the second dataset showed stronger facilitation than the first dataset (59.2 ± 38.1 vs 30.1 ± 25.3 Hz, p=0.0009, MWU), arguably due to superior CR performance (mean percentage CRs: 86.2 ± 17.3%; mean CR amplitude: 43.5 ± 17% eyelid closure), the level of suppression was not different between the two datasets (11.9 ± 5 Hz vs 12.9 ± 6.4 Hz, p=0.864, MWU). Moreover, the correlation matrices for eyelid position (Figure 2O) and velocity (Figure 2—figure supplement 1D) echo-ed the finding that the time difference between suppressive modulation and eyelid movement was not significantly different from zero on average (6.9 ± 25 ms, p=0.2439, Wilcoxon signed rank test; Figure 2P).
Together, these results, found across two independent datasets, reveal a large portion of IpN cells showing spike facilitation during the CS-US interval that predicts subsequent conditioned eyelid behavior. In contrast, there is a smaller number of suppressive neurons that are unlikely to drive eyelid movements, because their activity is less correlated with CR amplitude and often lags the eyelid movement (or leads it by an insufficient amount).
Optogenetic inhibition of IpN eliminates CRs
If CS-US facilitation in the IpN indeed controls eyelid closure, one would assume that inhibiting IpN activity during the CS-US interval is sufficient to abolish CRs. Pharmacological interventions suggest this is indeed so, with muscimol and/or lidocaine injections in the anterior IpN eliminating CRs completely (rabbit: Bracha et al., 1994; Ohyama et al., 2006; rat: Freeman et al., 2005; mouse: Heiney et al., 2014b). To further establish the causal role of IpN facilitation in the expression of specifically conditioned eyelid behavior, we employed optogenetic inhibition of the IpN in a set of three fully trained mice. Optic fibers were implanted near the IpN of L7cre-Ai27 mice, in which ChR2 was expressed in all Purkinje cells (see Materials and methods). During optogenetic excitation of Purkinje cell axon terminals, IpN neurons suppress their firing activity (Witter et al., 2013; Canto et al., 2016); Figure 3A). If excitatory rather than suppressive modulation in the IpN plays a causal role in driving the CRs, CRs should be selectively eliminated by optogenetic stimulation. Indeed, the conditioned behavior acquired by all three mice was readily disabled in virtually all trials randomly chosen to include optogenetic stimulation (all p<0.0001, MWU; Figure 3B,C). This result expands on the observation that IpN facilitation can elicit eyeblinks (Hesslow, 1994b; Heiney et al., 2014b), by showing that it is decidedly necessary for the expression of conditioned eyelid responses.

Optogenetic prevention of IpN facilitation eliminates CRs.
(A) In L7cre-Ai27 mice, Purkinje cells in the simplex lobule that express ChR2 exert a powerful inhibitory influence on the IpN upon optogenetic stimulation (see Witter et al., 2013; Canto et al., 2016). (B) Average eyelid traces for trials with (dark blue) or without (green) optogenetic stimulation throughout the CS-US interval, for three mice. C Eyelid closure at US onset for the data shown in B, separated by mouse.
CS-related transient spike pauses and subsequent rapid excitation in IpN neurons
Next, we examined whether and how previously reported CS-related complex spikes driven by climbing fibers in Purkinje cells (Ohmae and Medina, 2015; ten Brinke et al., 2015) may be reflected in IpN activity. Although IpN cells do not show an identifiably distinct spike type upon climbing fiber activation, previous work characterized a cerebellar nuclear equivalent to synchronous complex spike activity as a transient suppression in spike rate (e.g. Hoebeek et al., 2010; Bengtsson et al., 2011; Witter et al., 2013; Bengtsson and Jörntell, 2014; Tang et al., 2016). Based on the latency of CS-related complex spikes in eyelid-related Purkinje cells (Figure 4A,D), we looked at rapid spike deviations exceeding 4.5 baseline SDs between 50–125 ms post-CS in IpN neurons. Seven cells showed a striking transient suppression at 88 ± 12 ms (Figure 4B,E), which we refer to as a CS pause, with five of them showing a subsequent facilitation in the CS-US interval (71.4%, vs 55/263 (21%), p=0.0006, Fisher's test).

CS pause response reflecting CS-related Purkinje cell complex spike.
(A) Combined complex spike raster plot for 9 Purkinje cells ordered by the latency of their clear CS-related complex spike response in addition to the US-related complex spike (data from ten Brinke et al., 2015). (B) Combined raster plot for 7 IpN neurons, ordered by the latency of their CS pause in spike activity. (C) Same as in B, but for 12 of the 41 IpN neurons in the second dataset that showed a CS pause. (D-F) Average spike traces corresponding to the cells shown in (A-C), with (D) showing the probability of a complex spike instead of relative spike rate. (G) CS pause latency plotted against the latency at which the CR passes 5% eyelid closure, for all 45 cells showing both properties across the original and the second dataset. (H) Probability across days of finding a transient spike response at CS-complex spike latency, inhibitory (this figure) or excitatory (Figure 4—figure supplement 1), in the first dataset.
The presence and character of CS pauses as observed in the first dataset were strongly reflected in the second dataset, which was obtained from fully trained, high-performing mice. Out of 102 cells, 41 showed CS pauses (Figure 4C,F), and here too cells with a CS pause were more likely to show CS-US facilitation than those without (39/41 (95%) vs 31/61 (51%) cells, p<0.0001, Fisher’s test). Importantly, there was a clear across-cell correlation between average CS pause latency and CR latency, whether looking at both datasets combined (n = 45, r = 0.582, p<0.0001, Pearson; Figure 4G), or just the second dataset in isolation (n = 41, r = 0.403, p=0.0089, Pearson).
Interestingly, there were four cells in the first dataset that showed a transient increase, rather than a decrease, in firing at the same latency as the CS pause (Figure 4—figure supplement 1A,B). Rather than showing subsequent facilitation, two of these cells showed CS-US suppression (50%, vs 21/266 (7.9%) in the other cells, p=0.0021, Fisher’s test). The suppression did not seem more enhanced in these cells compared to other suppression cells (all p>0.25; Figure 4—figure supplement 1C,D). Moreover, this transient increase in firing was not observed in any of the cells in the second dataset, which was specifically focused on eyelid-related IpN areas as identified by microstimulation-driven blinks (see Materials and methods).
Across the first dataset, which was obtained over the course of conditioning, none of the 11 cells showing a transient spike response (CS pause or a transient increase in spikes) at the CS-related complex spike latency were recorded before day 4 of training (day 1–3, 0/99 cells; day 4+, 11/171, p=0.0058, Fisher's test; Figure 4H), which is consistent with the notion that CS-related climbing fiber response were acquired during learning (Ohmae and Medina, 2015; ten Brinke et al., 2015).
To further study the potential impacts of the climbing fiber inputs on CN modulation during CS and US, we compared the activity patterns of neurons with CS pauses to those without (Figure 5A,B,F,G). We found that cells with CS pauses not only have larger facilitation amplitudes compared to those without, but also showed a markedly more rapid excitatory spike profile right after the CS pause latency. In the first dataset, they showed significantly higher CR-related increases in firing rate during the CS-US interval (41.5 ± 33.1 vs 15.2 ± 8.9 Hz, p=0.0013, MWU; Figure 5C), and they also showed distinctly higher maximum spike rate velocities (4.9 ± 1.6 vs 1.4 ± 0.5 Hz/ms, p=0.0002, MWU; Figure 5D). While in the second dataset maximum facilitation was not significantly higher in facilitation cells with a CS pause (65.1 ± 39.5 Hz) compared to those without (51.7 ± 35.4 Hz, p=0.1594, MWU; Figure 5H), the former group did show significantly higher maximum spike rate velocities (5.6 ± 2.5 vs 3.6 ± 1.5 Hz/ms, p=0.0004, MWU; Figure 5I). Note that CS pause cells tended to be recorded less deeply in the nuclei than non-CS pause cells, in both the first (1840 ± 320 vs 2130 ± 350 μm, p=0.0298, MWU; Figure 5E) and second dataset (2350 ± 140 vs 2450 ± 211 μm, p=0.0372, MWU; Figure 5J).

CS-US facilitation in IpN cells with and without CS pause.
(A) Average spike traces for 5 IpN cells showing CS-US facilitation and a CS pause. Grand average shown in red, with SEM. (B) Same as in A, here showing 55 IpN cells with CS-US facilitation but without a CS pause. (C) Maximum spike rate in the CS-US interval for facilitation cells with CS pause (P) and without CS pause (-), for the data in A and B. (D) Same as C, here showing maximum spike rate velocity. (E) Same as in C, here showing recording depth. (F-J) Same as in (A-E), here showing data from the second dataset; 39 IpN cells showed CS-US facilitation and a CS pause, and 31 cells showed CS-US facilitation but not a CS pause.
-
Figure 5—source data 1
DCN model properties.
- https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.28132.011
To find out to what extent the CS-US facilitation in the IpN could result from merely Purkinje cell simple spike suppression (without taking the complex spike responses or collateral input to IpN neurons into account), we used simple spike profiles of 26 Purkinje cells reported previously (ten Brinke et al., 2015), to model spike modulation for as many IpN neurons (Figure 5—figure supplement 1A,B), using model parameters based on Yamazaki and Tanaka, 2007 and Person and Raman (2011); Figure 5—source data 1). The modeled CS-US facilitation resulting from averaging the simple spike activity of all Purkinje cells together aligned better with the recorded CS-US facilitation cells without a CS pause than those with CS pause, in the first dataset (Figure 5—figure supplement 1C–H). The model could also reproduce the much stronger and rapid excitatory response in the IpN neurons with a CS pause, but only if these IpN neurons were assumed to receive input from the Purkinje cells whose activity was suppressed the most (Figure 5—figure supplement 1A,B,D, blue traces)
Together, these findings establish a clear reflection in the IpN of the CS-related climbing fiber response as a transient spike suppression and support its selective and acquired nature. Moreover, the link between CS pauses and the subsequent high and/or fast facilitation rates in IpN neurons across the datasets is suggestive of a potential functional role of CS-related climbing fiber activity in triggering strong excitation in the IpN, a possibility that is further explored below.
IpN responses to the US
Central to the eyeblink conditioning paradigm is the notion that the US activates climbing fibers and evokes complex spikes in eyelid-related Purkinje cells, in particular if the mouse fails to make a conditioned response (Ohmae and Medina, 2015; ten Brinke et al., 2015). We therefore analyzed the spike modulation during the US epoch and searched for the modulation pattern indicative for an imprint of the US evoked complex spike activation. Interestingly, 10.6 ± 4.7 ms after US onset we found a marked increase in IpN spikes (99 ± 59 Hz), which exceeded five baseline SDs in 211 cells of the first dataset (78.2%, Figure 6A,B, see Materials and methods). This IpN response, here referred to as a US peak, had two important features: First, US peak amplitude, measured from baseline, diminished over the course of training, as apparent from a comparison of recordings made in the first four training days and those made subsequently (88 ± 48 vs. 29 ± 44 Hz, p<0.0001, MWU; Figure 6C). Second, US peak amplitude correlated negatively with conditioned behavior, in terms of both CR percentage (r = −0.426, p<0.0001, n = 270, Spearman) and CR amplitude at US onset (r = −0.333, p<0.0001, n = 270, Spearman), confirming its reduction as conditioned behavior is acquired. Furthermore, among 62 recordings with a US peak and CR behavior, trial-by-trial correlations revealed 13 cells in which US peak amplitude correlated negatively to CR amplitude (p<0.0001, bootstrap with 500 repetitions; Figure 6D). These significant recordings were twice as likely as the other 49 cells to also show significant positive correlations between CR amplitude and CS-US facilitation (69.2% (9/13) versus 36.7% (18/49), p=0.0077, Fisher test), which itself was three times as likely to occur in cells with US peak compared to those without (26.1% (55/211) vs. 5/59, 8.5% (5/59), p=0.007, Fisher test). Together, these results establish a substantial prevalence of US peak responses in the IpN, demonstrate their co-occurrence with conditioning-related facilitation and spike-eyelid correlations in the CS-US interval, and show cell- and trial-wide conditioning-related dynamics that are similar to those of US-related climbing fiber responses.

US peak and pause responses.
(A) Average spike traces for cells showing a US peak but not a US pause response (n = 134); left panel shows activity in the CS-US interval aligned to baseline, right panel shows post-US activity aligned to the last 50 ms of the CS-US interval. (B) Same as in A, but for cells showing both a US peak and a US pause (n = 77). The pie chart shows the proportion of cells with a US peak (yellow), a US pause (blue), both (green), or neither (gray). (C) Left panel: average traces with SEM of spike activity relative to baseline for all cells recorded without conditioned behavior on days 1–4 (black), and all cells recorded after day four and with conditioned behavior (red). Middle panel: US peak amplitude relative to baseline was significantly higher in early recordings without conditioned behavior (pre-) than in later recordings with behavior (post+; p<0.0001). Right panel: same as the middle panel, here showing minimum firing rate in the US pause window relative to the last 50 ms in the CS-US interval; early recordings without CR behavior show lower values than later recordings with behavior (p=0.0312). (D) Significant (plain orange) and non-significant (light orange) trial-by-trial correlation lines for all cells showing conditioned behavior and a US peak (n = 62). Black dotted line shows a fit from a linear mixed model incorporating the significant cells. The pie chart inset shows the proportion of cells that were significant. (E) Same as in A, but for cells without a clear post-US response (n = 31). (F) Same as in A, but for cells with only a US pause response (n = 28). (G) Recording depth was different between cells separated on US responses, getting progressively less deep from cells with only a US peak (yellow, 2182 ± 373 μm), through cells showing both US peak and pause (green, 2075 ± 322 μm), to cells showing only a US pause (blue, 1939 ± 299 μm; p=0.0025). (H) Similar to D, but for all cells showing conditioned behavior and a US pause (n = 37). Note that the y-axis shows the firing rate during the US pause period, with higher values implying less pronounced pauses.
Interestingly, another relationship between the US peak and US-related climbing fiber responses was observed in a subgroup of neurons. Occasionally, mice tended to open their eye relative to baseline in response to the CS. Among 149 of the recordings with a US peak but without CR behavior, 17 showed positive across-trial correlations between eyelid amplitude and the US peak response (11.4%, p<0.0001, bootstrap with 500 repetitions; Figure 6—figure supplement 1A), i.e. more eyelid opening related to weaker US peaks. We analyzed the Purkinje cell data from ten Brinke et al. (2015) to see if eyelid opening responses were related to US-complex spikes. Indeed, across trials where mice opened their eye further in the CS-US interval, those without a US-related complex spike showed slightly more eyelid opening (−2.65 ± 1.85%) than those with a US-complex spike (−0.84 ± 1.86%, p=0.0254, MWU; Figure 6—figure supplement 1B,C). Thus, this additional property seems to link the US peak to US-related climbing fiber activity.
In addition to US peak responses, transient suppressive responses were also observed at 28.2 ± 4.9 ms post-US in 105 IpN cells (39%), with mean magnitudes of −50 ± 24 Hz (Figure 6F). The latency of this response, referred to as the US pause, fits with the idea of climbing fiber input inhibiting the cerebellar nuclei indirectly via Purkinje cell complex spikes (e.g. Hoebeek et al., 2010; Lu et al., 2016). Consistent with this notion, the majority of cells that showed a CS pause also showed a US pause (86%, vs. 105/263 (40%), p=0.0012, Fisher’s exact test). We found considerable overlap in the prevalence of US peaks and pauses: a third of the cells showing the one response also showed the other (77 out of 239, 32.2%; Figure 6B). Additionally, cells with only a US pause tended to be more dorsally located (1939 ± 299 μm) than cells with only a US peak (2182 ± 373 μm), and cells that showed both responses averaged an intermediate depth (2075 ± 322 μm; p=0.0025, Kruskal-Wallis; Figure 6G). In terms of conditioning-related dynamics, the US pause showed characteristics that were similar to those of US peaks. Across the first four training days, 47.1% of cells (66/140) showed a US pause, compared to 30% (39/130) on subsequent training days in the first dataset (p=0.0057, Chi-square test). Moreover, further into training (day 5+), the US pause showed lower magnitudes than those observed on earlier days (−13 ± 30.3 vs −28.8 ± 30.4 Hz, resp., p=0.0018, MWU). Furthermore, on a trial-by-trial basis, 4 out of 41 recordings with a US pause and at least 20% CRs showed deeper pauses in trials with worse behavior (p=0.0017, bootstrap with 500 repetitions; Figure 6H). Note that, as with the US peak, US pauses were observed less frequently in the second dataset (23.5%). These findings establish a transient post-US suppressive response that is observed most reliably in conditions associated with climbing fiber-driven complex spikes in Purkinje cells.
US pauses are followed by fast excitatory responses in IpN neurons after conditioning
In addition to the US peak and pause responses, 83 IpN cells in the first dataset showed a rapid excitatory response that peaked at 46 ± 7 ms and showed magnitudes of 61.4 ± 28.3 Hz (39.4%; Figure 7A, arrow), which we refer to as the second US peak. While the amplitudes of the first and second US peaks were correlated trial-by-trial in IpN neurons (Figure 7D), their relation to the behavioral paradigm was different. Like the first US peak, the second peak was more prevalent in recordings with poor conditioned behavior (26 ± 32 Hz, vs 13 ± 26 Hz, p=0.025, MWU; Figure 7E), but it did not show a steady decline over the course of training days (r = 0.102, p=0.13, n = 224, Spearman). To the contrary, across recordings in which CR performance was poor, the second US peak actually grew stronger over the course of conditioning days (r = 0.287, p=0.0007, n = 138, Spearman), with maximum peak amplitudes 30–60 ms post-US of 16 ± 28.3 Hz on training days 1–4 (n = 121), and 46.5 ± 33 Hz on later training days (n = 46, p=0.0001, MWU; Figure 7B). Similarly, spike rate velocity was lower during the second US peak on earlier training days compared to later ones (0.55 ± 0.32 vs 0.77 ± 0.45 Hz/ms, p=0.0072, MWU; Figure 7C).

Second US peak response akin to rapid post-CS pause excitation.
(A) Raster plot (upper panel) and average spike trace (lower panel) of an example IpN cell showing a second US peak (arrow). (B) Significant (plain orange) and non-significant (light orange) trial-by-trial correlation lines for cells showing both a first and a second US peak (n = 83). Pie chart shows the proportion of significant cells, black dotted line shows fit from a linear mixed model, integrating significant cells. (C) IpNs with a second US peak (Pk2) show lower average CR amplitudes at US onset than IpNs without (-), in both the first dataset (left boxplot pair; p<0.0001), as well as in the second dataset (right boxplot pair; p=0.021). (D) Average traces with SEM of spike activity relative to baseline for IpNs recorded without conditioned behavior on days 1–4 (black), and those recorded without conditioned behavior after day 4 (yellow). (E) Maximum spike rate velocity of the second US peak was significantly higher in recordings without CR behavior on day five and over (post-) compared to earlier recordings without behavior (pre-; p=0.0072). (F) Average spike traces of paired trials, aligned by CS pause minima (brown), and of US-only trials from the corresponding IpN cell, aligned by US pause (yellow) minima, for 12 recordings from the second dataset. The traces were standardized by the activity −150 to −50 ms relative to the pause minima. (G) Averages of the CS pause (brown) and US pause (yellow) traces in F, with SEM. For reference, US pause-aligned traces from paired trials of IpNs from the first dataset, with US pauses, without CR behavior, and from day 1–4, are shown in black. (H) The difference between US-responses in US-only trials after training (yellow) and US-responses in paired trials early in training before CR behavior is acquired (gray) highlight the absence of the first US peak and the substantial presence of a second peak after the US pause, in well-trained animals. Additionally, the difference between the CS pause-aligned traces from paired trials (brown) and the US-pause aligned traces from US-only trials of the same IpN set (yellow) suggests the two profiles only start to diverge substantially approx. 50 ms after the pause response.
Consistent with the observation that US-related responses in IpN neurons tended to occur during recordings in which CR performance was poor (Figure 6; Figure 7E), we found that in the second dataset (in which CR performance was very high) only 9 cells (8.8%) showed a first US peak response, only 24 (23.5%) showed a US pause, and only 11 (10.8%) showed a second US peak (CR amplitude: 31.3 ± 15.9 vs 44.9 ± 16.6% eyelid closure, p=0.021, MWU; Figure 7E). To examine US responses of IpN neurons in the second dataset, we interspersed US-only trials (n = 13 ± 2) during recordings of 25 cells (24.5%), of which 13 showed CS-US facilitation (Figure 7—figure supplement 1A,B). Indeed, both the first US peak (10/25 cells, 40%) and US pause (10/25, 40%) showed a higher prevalence in US-only trials than in paired trials, but the second US peak was the most substantial, occurring in 18 cells (72%), and outranking the first US peak in terms of amplitude (94.6 ± 82.9 vs 27.8 ± 48.7 Hz, p=0.0012, MWU). Interestingly, it was especially pronounced in US-only trials in the 12 recordings that showed clear CS pauses and CS-US facilitation in paired trials (Figure 7—figure supplement 1A–C). In fact, there was a remarkable similarity between the pause and subsequent rapid excitation observed after the US in US-only trials, and the pause and subsequent rapid excitation observed after the CS in paired trials (Figure 7F,G), hinting at the possibility of a similar underlying mechanism.
Discussion
By quantifying neuronal responses in the cerebellar interpositus nucleus (IpN) during and after Pavlovian eyeblink conditioning, the present study sheds light on the question of how cerebellar cortical and extra-cortical afferents are integrated at the level of the cerebellar nuclei. Trial-by-trial correlations and their temporal distribution across the trial timespan, as well as optogenetic abolition of CRs, evince and characterize the causal relation between IpN facilitation and conditioned eyelid-behavior. Our findings, consistent across two independent datasets, together characterize cerebellar cortico-nuclear integration as a multi-faceted process involving the impact of common afferents. The data suggest that the dynamic modulation of activity in the IpN during the generation of conditioned eyelid movements can be affected through a learned Purkinje cell simple spike suppression on the one hand, and through climbing fiber mediated Purkinje cell complex spike activity, possibly in tandem with activation of mossy fiber and climbing fiber collaterals, on the other (Figure 8).

Eyelid-related modulation in cerebellar cortex and nuclei.
(A) Mean spike traces for simple and complex spikes in Purkinje cells and IpN spike show how eyelid-related cerebellar cortical and nuclear modulation relate after training. On the one hand, simple spike suppression translates relatively straightforwardly to facilitation in the IpN. Stimulus-related complex spike input leaves a transient spike trough, with the well-timed CS-related complex spike input showing a tendency to be followed by strong rebound-like excitation. Extra-cortical afferents underlie the oft observed transient post-US excitatory response, and presumably interact with the other modulatory components. The timing component in the baseline represents the coefficient of variation (CV). (B) Cell-by-cell correlations between occurrence, amplitude, and timing of the different eyelid behavior and IpN spiking components. Black lines indicate negative correlations, white lines indicate positive ones. (C) Trial-by-trial correlations between the components also labeled in B, with lines indicating the presence of a significant number of cells showing significant correlations of the corresponding direction. For each parameter pair, recordings were included only if they were considered to show the associated phenomena. Dashed white and black lines indicate that both positive and negative correlations were significantly present among cells.
Facilitation drives the learned response
About one third of the US-responding cells recorded across the IpN over the course of conditioning showed a significant change in spiking activity within the CS-US interval, with a facilitation to suppression ratio of 2.6 (60:23). In the second dataset, recording from optimally conditioned mice in eyelid-controlling IpN regions, 85% showed CS-US spike modulation, with a facilitation to suppression ratio of 4.1 (70:17). Although facilitation has also been shown to predominate over suppression in previous work (McCormick et al., 1982; Berthier and Moore, 1990; Gould and Steinmetz, 1996; Freeman and Nicholson, 2000; Gruart et al., 2000; Choi and Moore, 2003; Green and Arenos, 2007; Halverson et al., 2010; Heiney et al., 2014b), its profile and relation to eyelid behavior has been not been consistently characterized as leading (for posterior IpN, see Gruart et al., 2000; Delgado-García and Gruart, 2005) and its role has been hypothesized to be only a facilitator, rather than a main driver, of CRs (Delgado-García and Gruart, 2005). In all, the comprehensive trial-by-trial correlational analysis reported here confirms the leading nature of facilitation in IpN cells, in terms of the temporal profiles of modulation as well as the temporal distribution of spike-eyelid correlations. Moreover, optogenetic stimulation of Purkinje cells, which was shown to suppress the activity of cerebellar nuclei neurons (Witter et al., 2013; Canto et al., 2016), effectively abolished CRs. One potential complication could have been the obstruction of CRs by the elicitation of twitches, but the inhibitory effect in the IpN that results from our protocol would only have been able to induce twitches at its offset (Witter et al., 2013), which fell outside the CS-US interval. The opposite potential caveat could be a sudden drop in muscle tone and a resulting immobility. However, this should have been reflected in reduced natural fluctuations of eyelid behavior compared to baseline, for which there was no indication in our data. Thus, while it was shown previously that activation of IpN via optogenetic suppression of Purkinje cells is sufficient to elicit eyelid behavior (Heiney et al., 2014a), we here expand on pharmacological evidence for the necessity of facilitation in the IpN for the expression of conditioned eyelid behavior (Freeman et al., 2005; Ohyama et al., 2006; Bracha et al., 1994; Heiney et al., 2014b), by inactivating IpN cells, only during the CS-US interval, through optogenetic stimulation of their inhibitory Purkinje cell input.
IpN neurons showing CR-related suppression of activity have previously been reported in Berthier and Moore (1990) and Gruart et al., 2000, and were hypothesized to facilitate conditioned behavior through inactivation of antagonistic muscles (Gruart et al., 2000). However, whereas the facilitation IpN cells that we recorded showed predictive trial-by-trial correlations to conditioned eyelid responses, to the modest extent that the suppressive IpN neurons showed correlations, they did only to simultaneous or past, but not future, behavior, across both datasets. While the overall minor presence of suppression in IpN neurons, and their modest, reflective correlations may be indicative of a minimal role in driving behavior, hinting perhaps at sensory feedback, the relaxation of antagonistic muscles is a secondary, passive means of facilitating CRs; its benefit would in part comprise a reduced energy cost to effect behavior, and may therefore escape proper detection in a correlational analysis focusing only on eyelid kinematics. Alternatively, as our electrodes might have preferentially targeted the large projection neurons, the suppression cells might also represent smaller inhibitory interneurons of the nuclei that in fact could serve to further shape the activity of the facilitation cells, smoothing the CR.
Stereotyped pause-excitation responses in the IpN
Many IpN neurons responded at the onset of the conditioned eyelid movement with a characteristic transient pause, which was followed by a period of increased activity. The transient IpN spike pauses observed across datasets strikingly mirror the CS- and US-related complex spike responses found in eyelid-related Purkinje cells (Ohmae and Medina, 2015; ten Brinke et al., 2015 and Figure 4). The translation from complex spike input to a pause response in the IpN is supported by the findings that climbing fiber activation can trigger a prominent inhibition in the cerebellar nuclei (Hoebeek et al., 2010; Bengtsson et al., 2011; Lu et al., 2016; Tang et al., 2016) and that the strength of this inhibition can be related to the synchrony of the complex spikes (De Zeeuw et al., 2011; Tang et al., 2016).
The excitatory response of IpN neurons is normally attributed to learning-related suppression of Purkinje cell activity, plasticity of the excitatory mossy fiber to IpN connection, or a combination of the two (Medina et al., 2000; Ohyama et al., 2006; Longley and Yeo, 2014; Freeman, 2015). To these potential mechanisms, our results add one more: In the first dataset, the fastest and strongest cases of excitatory modulation in the IpN neurons occurred right after the CS-related spike pause (Figure 4). Moreover, in the second dataset, a similar pattern of pause followed by strong excitation was observed in US-alone trials, which are known to trigger climbing fiber-driven complex spikes in Purkinje cells (Hesslow, 1994a; Mostofi et al., 2010). This CS-related spike pause is likely to have a direct impact on the subsequent spike modulation, as the modulation amplitudes and velocity were significantly higher in the cells with a CS-related spike pause (Figure 5). This characteristic profile fits well with the notion of rebound depolarization in the cerebellar nuclei (Hoebeek et al., 2010; Bengtsson et al., 2011). This phenomenon is well established in vitro (Jahnsen, 1986; Llinás and Mühlethaler, 1988; Aizenman and Linden, 1999; Tadayonnejad et al., 2009), and can be elicited through electrical (cerebellar nuclei and inferior olive, Hoebeek et al., 2010; inferior olive and skin, Bengtsson et al., 2011) and optogenetic (inferior olive, Lu et al., 2016; cerebellar nuclei, Witter et al., 2013) stimulation. In particular, rebound-driven behavioral responses can be elicited by synchronized nuclear inhibition lasting as little as 25 ms (Witter et al., 2013). Bengtsson et al., 2011 and Hoebeek et al. (2010) report that particularly synchronized complex spike input seems fit to induce substantial rebound excitation in the cerebellar nuclei. Nevertheless, there are also stimulation studies showing less convincing rebound (Chaumont et al., 2013), and its spontaneous occurrence in vitro or in vivo seems negligible (Alviña et al., 2008; Reato et al., 2016). Given the dependence of several models of cerebellar learning on the ability of the cerebellar nuclei to use rebound depolarization to trigger mechanisms of plasticity (e.g., Kistler et al., 1999; Steuber et al., 2007; Wetmore et al., 2008; De Zeeuw et al., 2011), the data here presented are particularly relevant in that they offer some evidence for an affirmative answer to the open question of whether rebound may occur during learning (Reato et al., 2016).
IpN neurons respond widely to unconditioned stimuli
We observed that many IpN neurons responded strongly to presentation of the US (Figure 6). The rapid post-US excitatory response had an average latency of 10.6 ms (25th-75th percentile: 7–12 ms), which allows for both mossy fiber and climbing fiber collateral activation as possible pathways for driving the response (Berthier and Moore, 1990; Cody and Richardson, 1979; Mostofi et al., 2010). Mostofi et al. (2010) point at latencies < 6 ms for mossy fiber activation as recorded in the cerebellar cortex in awake rabbits in response to peri-ocular electrical stimulation, and latencies > 9 ms for climbing fiber responses, which is in line with work in cats by Cody and Richardson (1979).
The fact that we used air puff instead of electrical stimulation, which introduces a delay, and that our subjects were mice, which may introduce a small reduction in transmission time, confound proper appraisal of the pathway underlying the US peak based on its latency. Nevertheless, several properties of the US peak provide some clues as to its origin. On the one hand, one could argue that the robust and ubiquitous manifestation of the US peak across the IpN suggests mossy fiber collaterals are a more likely source than climbing fiber collaterals. IpN cells that showed eyelid-related facilitation constitute a selective subgroup of cells in the IpN, and neurons showing US pauses were more commonly observed in less deeply located IpN neurons, which agrees with the notion that the anterior IpN is the main target region for eyelid-related Purkinje cell output (Yeo et al., 1985a1985; Steinmetz et al., 1992; Bracha et al., 1994; Krupa and Thompson, 1997; De Zeeuw and Yeo, 2005; Freeman et al., 2005; Heiney et al., 2014b). If climbing fiber collaterals would be distributed as widely throughout the IpN as the US peak was observed to be, this would almost certainly mean they are not constrained within eyelid-related olivocerebellar modules. Therefore, mossy fiber input, which is not assumed to adhere to such a modular organization and forms a more robust input to the cerebellar nuclei (e.g. Person and Raman, 2010), may more appropriately fit the observed manifestation of the US peak. Moreover, the reduction of US peak responses over the course of conditioning may well reflect the reduced impact of the air puff US due to protection of the eyelid, and a resultant reduced mossy fiber signal. On the other hand, there is research to suggest that climbing fiber collateral input may be more robust (Van der Want et al., 1989; Hesslow, 1994b; De Zeeuw and Yeo, 2005) than the notion of ‘sparse collaterals’ suggests (Chan-Palay, 1977). Moreover, stimulation of the inferior olive results in a consistent excitatory response in the cerebellar nuclei (Kitai et al., 1977; Hoebeek et al., 2010), and spontaneous complex spikes elicit a co-occurrence of excitatory and inhibitory responses in the nuclei 24% of the time (Blenkinsop and Lang, 2011), which is not very different from the percentage we observed (32%). The cell- and trial-wide correlations suggesting weaker US-peaks with better CRs may also be explained in terms of a changing level of nucleo-olivary inhibition (Medina et al., 2002), which would agree with a contribution of the climbing fiber collaterals (De Zeeuw et al., 1997). Moreover, the interesting occurrence of weaker US peaks in trials where mice opened their eyes further than baseline, agrees with Purkinje cell US-complex spike data from ten Brinke et al. (2015), underlining another similarity between the US peak and climbing fiber activity. Thus, while these arguments suggest it is likely that climbing fibers contribute to the US peak response, its broad prevalence across the IpN suggests mossy fibers are also involved.
The question remains as to why the US-related responses were substantially more widespread than the CS-related responses. Considering the strong convergence of Purkinje cells onto IpN neurons (approx. 40:1) (Person and Raman, 2011), only a relatively small portion of the IpN neurons might evoke functional behavioral responses. As the large pool of remaining IpN neurons may show US-responses that relay general sensory information of the face, including that of the areas surrounding the eyelid region (Koekkoek et al., 2002), or feed into cognitive processes (Wagner et al., 2017; Sokolov et al., 2017), there may also be a substantial amount of redundant, yet detectable, spike responses. This would be consistent with recent findings that dense population coding during eyeblink conditioning also occurs at the cerebellar input stage, i.e. in the granule cell layer (D'Angelo et al., 2009; Giovannucci et al., 2017), which may partly result from nucleocortical feedback originating in the IpN (Houck and Person, 2015; Gao et al., 2016).
Functional implications
In the present study, we have detailed the learning-related dynamic modulation of cerebellar IpN cells and evaluated it against the previously observed modulation of Purkinje cells of the cerebellar cortex by which they are inhibited during Pavlovian eyeblink conditioning. Through the use of detailed trial-by-trial correlations with conditioned eyelid behavior as well as computational modeling and optogenetic confirmation we established the necessity of facilitation of IpN cells for the production of conditioned responses. Moreover, we have uncovered a well-timed imprint of presumptive complex spike activity in the IpN and a subsequent strong excitation. Thus, possibly in interaction with direct input to the IpN, climbing fiber input to the cerebellar cortex could provide an additional mechanism through which spike modulation may be elicited in the IpN to fine-tune the timing of CRs (De Zeeuw and Ten Brinke, 2015). These results underline the relevance of olivary climbing fibers beyond their conventional role of providing teaching signals, highlighting a unique attribute of the olivocerebellar system compared to other networks, which generally segregate the signals that are used for inducing plasticity during learning from signals that are used for performance during memory recall.
Materials and methods
Surgery
Request a detailed protocolSubjects were 12–20 week-old wild-type C57Bl/6 mice (n = 25), housed individually with food and water ad libitum in a normal (n = 22, first dataset) or reversed (n = 6, second dataset) 12:12 light/dark cycle. For the optogenetics experiment, we used L7cre-Ai27 mice (n = 3) that express channelrhodopsin-2 in Purkinje cells. The experiments were approved by the institutional animal welfare committees (Erasmus MC, Rotterdam, The Netherlands and Baylor College of Medicine, Houston, USA). Mice were anesthetized with an isoflurane +oxygen mixture (5% for induction, 2% for maintenance) and body temperature was kept constant at 37°C. After fixation in a standard mouse stereotaxic alignment system (Stoelting Co., Wood Dale IL, USA), the scalp was opened to expose the skull. Membranous tissue was cleared, and the bone was surgically prepared with Optibond prime and adhesive (Kerr, Bioggio, Switzerland). A small brass pedestal was attached to the skull with Charisma (Heraeus Kulzer, Armonk NY, USA), using an xyz-manipulator, allowing for fixation to a head bar at right angles during training and electrophysiology. For the craniotomy performed after training, skin and muscle tissue was cleared from the left half of the occipital bone, where, after applying a local analgesic (bupivacainehydrochloride 2.5 mg ml-1), a roughly 1 mm wide craniotomy was performed, 1.5 mm from midline. A small rim of Charisma was made around the craniotomy and anti-inflammatory (Dexamethasone 4 mg ml-1) solution was applied inside, after which the chamber was closed with a very low viscosity silicone elastomer sealant (Kwik-cast, World Precision Instruments, Sarasota FL, USA).
Eyeblink conditioning
Request a detailed protocolTwo days after surgery, mice were head-fixed to a brass bar suspended over a cylindrical treadmill (Chettih et al., 2011) and placed in the electrophysiology set-up, contained in a light-isolated Faraday cage. A first habituation session consisted of 30–45 min during which no stimuli were presented. During a second and third habituation session on consecutive days 10 CS-only trials were presented to allow the mice to get used to the green LED light and to acquire a baseline measurement. Eight mice were subjected to a training paradigm for five days, before the electrophysiology phase, receiving 200 paired trials daily, with an inter-trial interval of 10 ± 2 s, amounting to approximately half an hour per session. In the remaining 11 mice, electrophysiology was performed from the start of training onwards, restricting the amount of paired trials to 250 per day to ensure comparability between mice across days.
The CS was a 260 ms green LED light, placed ~7 cm in front of the mouse. The US was a 10 ms corneal air-puff at 40 psi delivered through a 27.5-gauge needle tip positioned 5–10 mm from the left eye, co-terminating with the CS, which amounts to a CS-US interval of 250 ms. National Instruments NI-PXI (National Instruments, Austin TX, USA) processors and a low-noise Axon CNS Digidata 1440a acquisition system (AutoMate Scientific Inc., Berkely CA, USA) were used to trigger and keep track of stimuli whilst capturing data. Eyelid movements were recorded with a 250-fps camera (scA640-120gm, Basler, Ahrensburg, Germany). Eyeblink conditioning for the second dataset was performed as described in Heiney et al. (2014b).
For each recording, eyelid traces were normalized to the full blink range, which consisted of the minimal resting baseline value reflecting the open eye position as established visually during the experiment, and the mean of the UR peak values reflecting the closed eye position. The traces were smoothed using a 2nd degree Savitzky-Golay method with a span of 10 ms. An iterative Grubbs' outlier detection test (α = 0.05) on trial baseline standard deviations was used to remove trials that had an unstable baseline. Additionally, trials were removed if the eye was not at least halfway open. Next, CR amplitude was quantified as the maximum eyelid position within the CS-US interval relative to the trial baseline position, expressed as percentage of full blink range. Trials were considered to contain a CR when eyelid closure exceeded 5% from the baseline mean within the CS-US interval. CR onset was determined as the first time point of a continuous positive eyelid velocity leading to up to the fifth percentile of the amplitude from baseline to CR peak.
Electrophysiology
Request a detailed protocolNeurons for the first dataset were recorded with glass capillaries (Ø=2 mm, Harvard Apparatus, Holliston MA, USA) that were heated and pulled to obtain a 2–5 μm tip, and filled with a 2M NaCl solution. The electrode was stereotactically lowered into the IpN using an electrode holder that was positioned at a 40° angle in the caudal direction on the sagittal plane and controlled by a manipulator system (Luigs and Neumann SM7, Germany). The obtained electrical signal was pre-amplified with a computer-controlled microelectrode amplifier (Axon CNS, MultiClamp 700B, AutoMate Scientific, Inc. US) and digitized at 20 kHz using the Axon Digidata acquisition system. Neurons for the second dataset were recorded as described in Heiney et al., (2014b). The IpN contains a high density of neurons that are deeper than and well separated from cortical layers, and almost always show clear, single unit spike activity. Upon encountering cells with responses to the CS and/or US, and verifying a stable recording, animals were subjected to blocks of paired trials. The trial-by-trial spike-eyelid correlations in ten Brinke et al., (2015) showed significant r values ranging from 0.34 to 0.73, effect sizes that require 12–65 trials to be detected with the ideal statistical power of 80%. Since the same correlations for IpN neurons may be higher since their activity is located more closely to eyelid behavior, and since there was no previous information suggesting expected effect sizes for the other neuronal responses, we included all recordings with at least 10 trials. Neurons were recorded for up to 7 days, and 1% Cholera toxin B subunit (CTB, C9903, Sigma-Aldrich) was used to replace 2M saline in the electrode on the last day of electrophysiology. Eventually, 50 nL of CTB was injected after the last recoding. Electrophysiological recordings were band-pass filtered at 150–6000 Hz and spikes were thresholded in MATLAB (RRID:SCR_001622, The Mathworks, Natick MA, USA) using custom-written code and SpikeTrain (Neurasmus, Rotterdam, The Netherlands). Spike density functions (referred to as average spike traces) were computed for all trials by convolving binary 1 ms-bin vectors containing spike occurrences with a Gaussian kernel with a 5 ms width.
Microstimulation mapping of eyeblink-controlling regions in IpN
Request a detailed protocolFunctional mapping of the CN was performed using 80-μm-diameter platinum iridium monopolar electrodes (100 KΩ; Alpha Omega) as in Heiney et al., (2014b). Electrodes were positioned using stereotaxic coordinates relative to a mark on the cement. Mapping was mostly confined to the anterior interpositus (IpN), dorsolateral hump (DLH), and lateral nucleus (LN). Electrodes were advanced in steps of 100 μm, and currents in the range of 1–15 μA (200 ms pulse trains; 250 μs biphasic pulses; 500 Hz) were systematically tested to identify the threshold for evoking movement. The eyeblink-controlling region of IpN was defined as the location at which discrete and sustained eyelid closure could be evoked with low currents (<5 μA). This region was subsequently targeted for electrophysiological recordings.
Optogenetics
Request a detailed protocolFor optogenetic inhibition of the cerebellar nuclei, we used the L7cre-Ai27 mouse line, in which ChR2 is specifically expressed in Purkinje cells upon the expression of Pcp2-cre. An optic cannula fiber (⌀105 µm, 0.22NA, 2 mm, Thorlabs) was vertically implanted over the IpN (AP 2.5 mm, ML 2 mm) and attached to the skull. In addition, a brass pedestal was attached to the skull between bregma and lambda. Animals were allowed to recover from surgery for 2 weeks. After eyeblink conditioning, mice were head-fixed on a set up that is equipped with an optogenetic blue light source (wave length 470 nm, M470F3, Thorlabs) and a high-power light driver (M00283732, Thorlabs). In a 50-trial session, 10 trials were randomly presented together with a 5V optic stimulus that gave 5 ms pulses at 100 Hz for the duration of the CS (260 ms), thus terminating at CS and US offset.
Spike modulation in the last 200 ms of the CS-US interval
Request a detailed protocolSpike rate suppression and facilitation in the CS-US interval (Figure 2), relative to a 500 ms baseline period, were determined separately. We chose to determine average spike modulation of cells in terms of absolute instead of relative change from the average baseline firing frequency, as we wanted to not process the data further away from actual firing frequency than necessary. Moreover, absolute change was not different across firing frequencies in terms of facilitation (r = 0.025, p=0.68, n = 270, Spearman), and absolute suppression was only slightly deeper in faster firing cells (r = −0.131, p=0.0308, n = 270, Spearman), whereas relative mean modulation was more clearly linked to average firing frequency (facilitation: r = −0.378, p<0.0001; suppression: r = −0.211, p=0.0005; n = 270, Spearman). Thus, expressing spike modulation in absolute terms thus avoids the use of a modulation criterion that is implicitly looser for cells with lower firing frequencies. We considered cells to carry CS-US facilitation if they showed an average increase of at least 5 Hz above baseline mean in the last 200 ms of the CS-US interval, after subtracting the mean above-baseline activity within the baseline itself. Similarly, cells showing an activity of at least 5 Hz below baseline mean, correcting for below-baseline activity within the baseline itself, were considered suppressive. For both types of modulation, this threshold roughly corresponds to 3 SDs among the incidentally negatively modulating cells, i.e. cells where the baseline showed more facilitation/suppression than the CS-US interval. Duration of modulation was calculated as the sum of ms-bins exceeding 3 baseline SDs in the last 200 ms of the CS-US interval; spike firing needed to exceed this threshold for at least 10 ms during the CS-US interval.
CS-responses
Request a detailed protocolTo see how IpN neurons may reflect CS-related Purkinje cell complex spikes, rapid shifts in spike frequency were assessed between 50 and 125 ms post-CS in average spike traces that were standardized to baseline. A rapid drop exceeding 4.5 SDs was considered to be a CS pause if it was followed by a peak of at least 2 SDs. Similarly, an upstroke of 4.5 SDs was labeled a CS peak if it was followed by a drop of at least 2 SDs.
US-responses
Request a detailed protocolFirst, the presence of US peak and pause responses in individual cells were quantified on the basis of their average spike traces, standardized to the 500 ms baseline. The time windows used to quantify the first US peak and pause (Figure 6), and the second US peak (Figure 7) were 1–30 ms, 16–45 ms, and 31–60 ms post-US, respectively. A first US peak was identified if it exceeded either 5 or 2 baseline SDs from the average of the last 50 ms of the CS-US interval, with the latter case also requiring a 5 SD difference from baseline. This way, cells that showed high facilitation in the CS-US interval were not ignored simply because they did not spare enough room for the spike increase required to meet the first criterion. Pauses were recognized if they exceeded 5 baseline SDs below the final 50 ms of the CS-US interval, and were at least 2 SDs below baseline. Alternatively, they could be 2 SDs below the end of the CS-US interval and exceed 5 SDs from baseline. This means pauses that did not dip below baseline level were not recognized as such, as it is difficult to ascertain whether they are pauses, or just the space between two peaks. Finally, second peaks were considered significant if they were still at least 2 SDs above the last 50 ms of the CS-US interval as well as 5 SDs above baseline. For both the pause and the second peak, the subsequent turnaround had to regress at least 2 SDs back to baseline, so as to avoid the inclusion of blunt, unpeak-like movements in the average spike traces. For trial-by-trial analyses, absolute firing frequency was used, taking the maximum (or, for pauses, minimum) values within 30 ms time-windows centered at the average peak- or pause-time.
Simple spike-based model of IpN neurons
Request a detailed protocolUsing Python software (RRID:SCR_008394), A total of 26 IpN neurons were modeled (Figure 5) using the parameters shown in Figure 5—source data 1, with each modeled IpN neuron incorporating the simple spike modulation of a Purkinje cell from the dataset in ten Brinke et al. (2015). For each modeled trial of IpN modulation, 30 simple spike patterns were used as input to reflect the reported convergence rate of 20–50 Purkinje cells to 1 nuclear cell (Person and Raman, 2011). It is not known how similar or dissimilar simple spike modulation of Purkinje cells projecting to the same nuclear cell is. We here opted to take the same reference Purkinje cell for each of the thirty simple spike patterns used to model IpN activity. To avoid reusing trials, we expanded the Purkinje cell simple spike data. For each Purkinje cell, inter-spike interval distribution (ISI) characteristics (mean, SD, skewness, kurtosis) were determined across trials for each 1 ms bin in the trial timespan. By iteratively sampling a random ISI from the local ISI distribution, trials of simple spike activity are created that carry the same firing and modulation characteristics as the original Purkinje cell. The resulting set of 900 trials of simple spike activity was fed to the model, resulting in 30 trials of IpN spike activity. Spike density functions were subsequently computed for each IpN cell and parameters across the combined data were compared with actual IpN recordings.
Statistics
Significance tests were performed using MATLAB. Unless otherwise specified, mean and standard deviation were used to report central tendency and measure of deviation. Correlations between two continuous variables were made using Pearson's r if there were at least 30 data points and data was normally distributed for both variables, after a Grubbs' test for outliers (α = 0.05); otherwise, Spearman's rho was used. Group differences were assessed with Mann-Whitney U tests. For combined analysis of multiple cells, mixed-effects linear regression was performed, with random intercepts and slopes for cells included based on likelihood ratio tests. Visual inspection of residuals safeguarded adequate normality and homoscedasticity, and helped identify extreme outliers (never more than four). Bootstrap tests were used to determine the probability for a certain number of cells to be significant across a cell population, by assessing significance across 500 random datasets with similar amounts of cells and similar amounts of trials. Correlation matrices were used to explore the temporal distribution of the correlations between spike modulation and behavior (Figure 2, Figure 2—figure supplement 1). The procedure is identical to the one explained in ten Brinke et al., (2015). In short, the trial timespan is divided up in 20 ms bins. Across trials, the average eyelid value within each bin is correlated to the average spike rate value within each bin, resulting in a 100 × 100 matrix of r-values, given the 2 s trial window. Matrices for multiple cells were subsequently compiled by taking the average r-values across matrices, after nullifying the r-values of the sign opposite to the sign of interest. Note that this constitutes a methodological tool used to assess the temporal distribution of correlations, and not to quantify their significance.
Data availability
-
Eyelid behavior and spike activity of cerebellar interpositus nucleus neurons during eyeblink conditioning in awake behaving micePublicly available at the Collaborative Research in Computational Neuroscience (http://crcns.org/).
References
-
Questioning the role of rebound firing in the cerebellumNature Neuroscience 11:1256–1258.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.2195
-
Glycinergic projection neurons of the cerebellumJournal of Neuroscience 29:10104–10110.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2087-09.2009
-
Synaptic action of the olivocerebellar system on cerebellar nuclear spike activityJournal of Neuroscience 31:14708–14720.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3323-11.2011
-
Axonal sprouting and formation of terminals in the adult cerebellum during associative motor learningJournal of Neuroscience 33:17897–17907.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0511-13.2013
-
Taxonomies of Timing: Where Does the Cerebellum Fit In?Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 8:282–288.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2016.02.034
-
Cytology and organization in the nucleus lateralis of the cerebellum: the projections of neurons and their processes into afferent axon bundlesZeitschrift for Anatomie und Entwicklungsgeschichte 141:151–159.https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00519882
-
BookCerebellar Dentate Nucleus: Organization, Cytology and TransmittersBerlin: Springer-Verlag.https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-66498-4
-
Adaptive Timing of Motor Output in the Mouse: The Role of Movement Oscillations in Eyelid ConditioningFrontiers in Integrative Neuroscience 5:72.https://doi.org/10.3389/fnint.2011.00072
-
Cerebellar neuronal activity expresses the complex topography of conditioned eyeblink responsesBehavioral Neuroscience 117:1211–1219.https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7044.117.6.1211
-
Spatiotemporal firing patterns in the cerebellumNature Reviews Neuroscience 12:327–344.https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3011
-
Motor Learning and the CerebellumCold Spring Harbor Perspectives in Biology 7:a021683.https://doi.org/10.1101/cshperspect.a021683
-
Climbing fibre collaterals contact neurons in the cerebellar nuclei that provide a GABAergic feedback to the inferior oliveNeuroscience 80:981–986.
-
Time and tide in cerebellar memory formationCurrent Opinion in Neurobiology 15:667–674.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2005.10.008
-
Developmental changes in eye-blink conditioning and neuronal activity in the cerebellar interpositus nucleusJournal of Neuroscience 20:813–819.
-
Cerebellar learning mechanismsBrain Research 1621:260–269.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2014.09.062
-
Hippocampal and cerebellar single-unit activity during delay and trace eyeblink conditioning in the ratNeurobiology of Learning and Memory 87:269–284.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2006.08.014
-
Cerebellar posterior interpositus nucleus as an enhancer of classically conditioned eyelid responses in alert catsJournal of Neurophysiology 84:2680–2690.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.2000.84.5.2680
-
Precise control of movement kinematics by optogenetic inhibition of Purkinje cell activityJournal of Neuroscience 34:2321–2330.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4547-13.2014
-
Cerebellar-dependent expression of motor learning during eyeblink conditioning in head-fixed miceJournal of Neuroscience 34:14845–14853.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2820-14.2014
-
Cerebellar premotor output neurons collateralize to innervate the cerebellar cortexJournal of Comparative Neurology 523:2254–2271.https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.23787
-
Timing functions of the cerebellumJournal of Cognitive Neuroscience 1:136–152.https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.1989.1.2.136
-
Electrophysiological characteristics of neurones in the guinea-pig deep cerebellar nuclei in vitroThe Journal of Physiology 372:129–147.https://doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.1986.sp016001
-
Delayed reverberation through time windows as a key to cerebellar functionBiological Cybernetics 81:373–380.https://doi.org/10.1007/s004220050569
-
Distribution of neural plasticity in cerebellum-dependent motor learningProgress in Brain Research 210:79–101.https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-444-63356-9.00004-2
-
Neuronal responses of the rabbit cerebellum during acquisition and performance of a classically conditioned nictitating membrane-eyelid responseJournal of Neuroscience 4:2811–2822.
-
Mechanisms of cerebellar learning suggested by eyelid conditioningCurrent Opinion in Neurobiology 10:717–724.https://doi.org/10.1016/S0959-4388(00)00154-9
-
Electrophysiological localization of eyeblink-related microzones in rabbit cerebellar cortexJournal of Neuroscience 30:8920–8934.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6117-09.2010
-
Integration of Purkinje cell inhibition by cerebellar nucleo-olivary neuronsJournal of Neuroscience 35:544–549.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3583-14.2015
-
Climbing fibers encode a temporal-difference prediction error during cerebellar learning in miceNature Neuroscience 18:1798–1803.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4167
-
Learning-induced plasticity in deep cerebellar nucleusJournal of Neuroscience 26:12656–12663.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4023-06.2006
-
Deep cerebellar nuclei rebound firing in vivo: much ado about almost nothing?
-
Synaptic Organization of the Brain311–359, Thalamus, Synaptic Organization of the Brain, Oxford University Press.
-
The cerebellum: adaptive prediction for movement and cognitionTrends in Cognitive Sciences 21:313–332.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2017.02.005
-
Disruption of classical eyelid conditioning after cerebellar lesions: damage to a memory trace system or a simple performance deficit?Journal of Neuroscience 12:4403–4426.
-
The cerebellar interpositus nucleus and the dynamic control of learned motor responsesJournal of Neuroscience 27:6620–6632.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0488-07.2007
-
Lock-and-key mechanisms of cerebellar memory recall based on rebound currentsJournal of Neurophysiology 100:2328–2347.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00344.2007
-
A spiking network model for passage-of-time representation in the cerebellumEuropean Journal of Neuroscience 26:2279–2292.https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05837.x
-
Classical conditioning of the nictitating membrane response of the rabbit. II. Lesions of the cerebellar cortexExperimental Brain Research 60:99–113.https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00237023
-
Classical conditioning of the nictitating membrane response of the rabbit. I. Lesions of the cerebellar nucleiExperimental Brain Research 60:87–98.
Decision letter
-
Naoshige UchidaReviewing Editor; Harvard University, United States
In the interests of transparency, eLife includes the editorial decision letter and accompanying author responses. A lightly edited version of the letter sent to the authors after peer review is shown, indicating the most substantive concerns; minor comments are not usually included.
Thank you for submitting your article "Dynamics of Cerebellar Nuclei Neurons during Pavlovian Eyeblink Conditioning" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by three peer reviewers, and the evaluation has been overseen by Naoshige Uchida as the Reviewing Editor and Richard Ivry as the Senior Editor. The following individuals involved in review of your submission have agreed to reveal their identity: John Freeman (Reviewer #1); David Herzfeld (Reviewer #3).
The reviewers have discussed the reviews with one another and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this decision to help you prepare a revised submission.
Summary:
Eyeblink conditioning has been a great model for studying the neural mechanism underlying classical conditioning. Although the cerebellum has long been implicated in eyeblink conditioning, how neurons in in the deep cerebellar nuclei (DCN) and those in the cerebellar cortex regulate eyeblink conditioning is yet to be determined. The present study provides a comprehensive analysis of the firing of DNC neurons (in particular, those in the interposed nucleus [IpN]) in relation with learning and Purkinje cell (PC) activities. Most notably:
1) The authors demonstrate a tight relationship between PC complex spikes and firing pauses of DCN neuron. This work builds on the work of Eric Lang (presence of complex spikes in DCN activity) and Javier Medina (presence of complex spikes due to the conditioned stimulus).
2) The authors show a trial-by-trial correlation between the activity of "facilitatory" DCN cells and eyeblink behavior.
3) Optogenetic activation of PCs caused inhibition of DCN neurons and reduced the conditioned response (eyeblink), supporting the necessity of normal IpN activity for conditioned behavior (although the enthusiasm of this result varied among the reviewers).
The reviewers thought that this work provides an important set of results that advances our understanding of the contributions of different neuron types in the cerebellum in eyeblink conditioning. Nonetheless, the reviewers noted some issues with respect to their interpretation and presentation of the results. We therefore would like to see the authors' response to the following points:
Essential points:
1) Most of the analyses rely on discrete classifications of neurons based on their response types, and do not provide a comprehensive view on the diversity of responses in each population. Furthermore, the main results such as correlation between neural activities and behaviors were obtained using a relatively small fraction of neurons that exhibited particular responses. The reviewers are generally concerned on these points, and suggest that the authors provide additional analyses or discussions to remedy these issues, taking into account the diversity of observed response types in the data set. More detailed comments regarding this are as follows:
1a) The authors have recorded from a large set of diverse DCN neurons. They are generally subdivided by a particular response property and then analyzed within those subdivisions. This subdivision can be confusing or problematic, as the criteria often change from experiment to experiment. For example, in Figure 1, facilitation/ suppression cells are defined as having at least 5 Hz up/down modulation. First, it would be helpful if the authors show a histogram of the population, as it is not clear that these are distinct populations. Second, later in the manuscript (subsection “CS-related transient spike trough reflects acquired CS-related complex spike response”), the amount of facilitation is compared between this population and a separate population which was defined in a different way. Furthermore, in some cases, the numbers of neurons in each category is so small (e.g. Figure 4F) that it remains unclear how many different response types there are, and how much each type really contributes to the behavior.
1b) The proportion of neurons with learning-relevant activity is lower than found in many of the previous studies cited. Only 29% showed facilitation and 17 of those showed positive correlations with the CR. This raises a concern regarding whether or not the recordings were from neurons in an eyelid control area of the IpN. A related concern is that some of the previous studies examining IpN activity used electrical stimulation through the recording electrode to elicit eyelid movement to determine whether the neurons recorded from that electrode were in an eyelid control area of the IpN. There are problems associated with this method such as activating fibers of passage, but the authors should note in the paper that some of the IpN neurons could have been controlling CRs for other muscles that happen to be highly correlated with eyelid closure.
1c) Modeling needs to be performed carefully, taking into account the scarcity of some of the key response types. In other words, a major challenge for the interpretation of the current results is how to weight the contributions of neurons exhibiting particular response types as indicated in Figure 9. For example, the authors stress the importance of the neurons showing a pause in spiking early in the CS period, but there were only 7 of these neurons. It appears that the study yielded sufficient data to model the interactions described in Figure 9, taking into account all of the various contributors in the population, weighted by their prevalence, magnitude of neuronal modulation, and strength of correlation with CRs.
1d) In the Materials and methods section, the descriptions of selection criteria for e.g. suppression vs. troughs are confusing. This is partly because the authors have specific terminologies that are not always obvious (for instance, it was not clear that there a fundamental difference in the meaning of 'suppression' vs. 'trough'). The authors should reference the specific figures/ experiments to which they refer for each section. Please use consistent descriptive terminology indicating response profiles associate with CS/US/complex spikes.
2) The comparison of the IpN neurons with predicted responses based on models of summed PC inputs (Figure 5) is very interesting. However, the authors in general seem to discount the role of non-PC inputs to DCN, specifically mossy fibers. For instance, the larger facilitation in neurons that show a CS-trough is interpreted as being due to rebound potentiation. How can enhanced MF plasticity onto those neurons (resulting from stronger CS modulation) be ruled out?
3) It is unclear about the putative relationship between PC complex spikes and the peak activity shown in Figure 4C. From Blenkinsop and Lang (2011), it would seem that if there is facilitation in the DCN following a PC complex spike, it should be brief compared to the depression (representing short-latency climbing fiber excitatory collaterals rather than complex spike related inhibition from the PCs). However, the duration of the excitatory peak in Figure 4C, F seems comparable to those provided in the early part of the same figure. Are these really complex spike mediated responses? Consider that IpN cell 2 in Figure 4C seems to show suppression with a presumptive complex spike following the US (opposite of the bursting relationship after CS).
In essence, there is no data to distinguish clearly between complex spike vs. climbing fiber afferent regulating DCN responses. The reviewers were concerned that the impact statement (as well as parts of the Discussion) goes beyond the data. Please revise the impact statement and other related descriptions to reflect this concern.
4) The Materials and methods section for the optogenetic experiments is missing.
5) Simultaneous activity of presynaptic PCs likely inhibits IpN cells well below their basal firing rate. Depending on the duration of stimulation, this could affect the kinematics of the unconditioned response. Do the authors see any changes in the kinematics of the eye blink for stimulation versus pre/early-training in which there is no CR?
6) It is unclear whether the results of this study are applicable to cerebral cortical interactions with the thalamus as suggested in the first paragraph of the Introduction and last paragraph of the Discussion. The reviewers thought that the nature of connectivity is very different, with cerebellar cortical output being exclusively inhibitory, for example. The reviewers thought that the Introduction is oversold and largely irrelevant. The reviewers suggest removing the discussion on cortical-subcortical interactions.
7) The author's trial-by-trial correlation analysis could be strengthened. The authors calculated correlations between 20ms bins of spike responses and behavior across trials. This analysis inherently assumes that each 20ms bin is independent, which is not the case. It would be good to use the actual time-dependent spiking responses to predict the CR behavior in time. A potential analysis: average rate responses of a neuron across trials in which a CR occurred (e.g., day 7+). For the same trial period determine the learned CR as the average difference between the eye position trace after learning and the position traces before learning. Now do a correlational analysis between these two average timeseries. What is the optimal lag/lead across neurons? How well does the facilitation timeseries predict the learned CR response (in time)?
8) The emergence of facilitation and suppression shown in Figure 1M occurs much more rapidly than does the emergence of behavior. This seems to disagree with the correlation results and optogenetic stimulation in the rest of the paper. If IpN facilitation is both necessary and sufficient to drive eye-blink behavior why does the end of Day 5 (which has approximately the same facilitation as Day 7) have significantly less CRs? Perhaps this is due to averaging spike responses across the entire CS-US period in Figure 1. If the authors plotted the 20ms bin average at the ideal lag (80ms), would this look qualitatively different that the average responses plotted in Figure 1M (e.g., emerging slower)?
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.28132.027Author response
Essential points:
1) Most of the analyses rely on discrete classifications of neurons based on their response types, and do not provide a comprehensive view on the diversity of responses in each population. Furthermore, the main results such as correlation between neural activities and behaviors were obtained using a relatively small fraction of neurons that exhibited particular responses. The reviewers are generally concerned on these points, and suggest that the authors provide additional analyses or discussions to remedy these issues, taking into account the diversity of observed response types in the data set. More detailed comments regarding this are as follows:
We understand the general concern that some of the numbers of neurons showing particular responses were low. We have therefore added more cells and analyses in addition to expanding our discussion of the findings. These additions would not have been possible without substantial efforts from Dr. Shane Heiney and Dr. Javier Medina at Baylor College of Medicine in Houston, who have been added as co-authors. As a consequence, a more comprehensive evaluation is now provided and all issues relating to the interpretation of the diversity of conditioning-associated responses, particularly those with modest prevalence, are addressed.
1a) The authors have recorded from a large set of diverse DCN neurons. They are generally subdivided by a particular response property and then analyzed within those subdivisions. This subdivision can be confusing or problematic, as the criteria often change from experiment to experiment. For example, in Figure 1, facilitation/ suppression cells are defined as having at least 5 Hz up/down modulation. First, it would be helpful if the authors show a histogram of the population, as it is not clear that these are distinct populations. Second, later in the manuscript (subsection “CS-related transient spike trough reflects acquired CS-related complex spike response”), the amount of facilitation is compared between this population and a separate population which was defined in a different way. Furthermore, in some cases, the numbers of neurons in each category is so small (e.g. Figure 4F) that it remains unclear how many different response types there are, and how much each type really contributes to the behavior.
We appreciate the point that the neuronal subdivision was not always clear, and have now made an effort to streamline our explanations in both Results and Materials and methods. Our choice to use a 5 Hz-threshold is based on a number of considerations. First, our preference was to not process spike modulation data further away from actual firing frequency than necessary. Second, when comparing baseline firing frequency to relative CS-US modulation expressed as Hz, it is clear that this approach does not introduce a baseline frequency dependent selection bias. In contrast, when we express it as a percentage in relation to baseline frequency, it does induce a bias in that neurons with low baseline frequency are preferred. For selection bias, see Author response image 1. A 5 Hz-threshold identifies modulating cells evenly (red plots), whereas percentage threshold shows preference towards low frequency cells (orange plots, general population top correlation plots, subsets of facilitation cells see bottom plot). Moreover, when looking at the distribution of suppression and facilitation (blue and red histograms, resp.), the negative values indicate occasions when there was more modulation in the baseline than in the CS-US interval; the one-sided 3 SD thresholds for these incidental magnitudes (dotted blue and red lines) both are in close agreement with a 5 Hz threshold (dashed black lines). Thus, given its intuitive straightforwardness, the fact that it does not introduce bias across neurons showing different baseline firing frequencies, and its statistical appropriateness, we decided on using a 5 Hz threshold. This rationale is now explained in the Materials and methods section.

With regards to the comparison in the subsection “CS-related transient spike trough reflects acquired CS-related complex spike response”, facilitation is not computed any differently in the two groups that are compared. Rather, all facilitation cells were split into a group with cells that also showed a putatively CS-complex spike related spike trough, and one with cells that did not. We have now rephrased this part to be clearer. Moreover, with the additional data, we now provide a more comprehensive dataset, upping the numbers for the different categories of neuronal responses. Lastly, we now provide a visualized classification of these different groups in Figure 1—figure supplement 1.
1b) The proportion of neurons with learning-relevant activity is lower than found in many of the previous studies cited. Only 29% showed facilitation and 17 of those showed positive correlations with the CR. This raises a concern regarding whether or not the recordings were from neurons in an eyelid control area of the IpN. A related concern is that some of the previous studies examining IpN activity used electrical stimulation through the recording electrode to elicit eyelid movement to determine whether the neurons recorded from that electrode were in an eyelid control area of the IpN. There are problems associated with this method such as activating fibers of passage, but the authors should note in the paper that some of the IpN neurons could have been controlling CRs for other muscles that happen to be highly correlated with eyelid closure.
The proportion of neurons with learning-relevant activity is dependent on the selection criteria and paradigms. Berthier and Moore (1990) observed similar numbers of 50 out of 165 cells showing CR-related facilitation (30.3%) in trained animals. Moreover, when we use the same criteria as Halverson, Lee, and Freeman (2010), we would have 41% facilitation cells. For this study we decided to use the 5 Hz selection threshold for the reasons elaborated in the previous point.
Our original approach, recording throughout the IpN, is one reason why the strong prevalence of almost immediate excitation after the US implies the ubiquity of whichever collaterals are responsible for them (climbing fibers seem to play a substantial role, given our data and the compatibility between the bimodal latencies of US-complex spikes in Mostofi et al. (2010) and US-peak responses in Berthier and Moore, (1990)). Nevertheless, we concede the downside that some of our IpN neurons could have been controlling other highly correlated muscles than the eyelid. We therefore addressed this concern by incorporating a second dataset, which allowed us to corroborate our findings in recordings that were demonstrably eyelid-controlling, by virtue of micro-stimulation. As pointed out by the reviewer this approach, although not perfect either, does indeed provide a higher percentage of facilitation cells (see new Figure 1—figure supplement 1). We now explain this comparison and highlight this reasoning in the Discussion.
1c) Modeling needs to be performed carefully, taking into account the scarcity of some of the key response types. In other words, a major challenge for the interpretation of the current results is how to weight the contributions of neurons exhibiting particular response types as indicated in Figure 9. For example, the authors stress the importance of the neurons showing a pause in spiking early in the CS period, but there were only 7 of these neurons. It appears that the study yielded sufficient data to model the interactions described in Figure 9, taking into account all of the various contributors in the population, weighted by their prevalence, magnitude of neuronal modulation, and strength of correlation with CRs.
We agree that in the original submission, the scarcity of key response types made it difficult to appraise their relevance, and indeed the relevance of their interactions as described in Figure 9. In the current version of the manuscript, we have alleviated this concern through the addition of substantial data obtained in optimally conditioned mice. This has greatly sharpened the image in terms of the manifestation of neuronal responses, e.g. by drastically increasing the number of cells showing CS pause responses (Figure 4), and in contrast by leaving the CS peak response wholly uncorroborated (and thus relegated to Figure 4—figure supplement 1). The layered pie-charts in Figure 1—figure supplement 1 now provide an extensive overview of how the different response types co-occur, also distinguishing between cells that do or don’t show significant spike-eyelid correlations. Finally, we have now updated the modeled diagram in Figure 9 (now Figure 8) to include the second dataset, thereby greatly strengthening the visualized interactions.
1d) In the Materials and methods section, the descriptions of selection criteria for e.g. suppression vs. troughs are confusing. This is partly because the authors have specific terminologies that are not always obvious (for instance, it was not clear that there a fundamental difference in the meaning of 'suppression' vs. 'trough'). The authors should reference the specific figures/ experiments to which they refer for each section. Please use consistent descriptive terminology indicating response profiles associate with CS/US/complex spikes.
We have now included references to specific figures/experiments in the Materials and methods, and, avoiding the terms “trough” and “facilitatory”, now use the following terminology:
CS-US facilitation | IpN spike facilitation in the CS-US interval (Figure 2). |
CS-US suppression | IpN spike suppression in the CS-US interval (Figure 2). |
CS pause | Transient IpN spike pause reflecting the CS-related complex spike (Figure 4). |
US peak | IpN spike burst occurring 0-30 ms after US onset (Figure 6). |
US pause | Transient IpN spike pause reflecting the US-related complex spike at 15-30 ms after US onset (Figure 6). |
Second US peak | Rapid spike facilitation following complex spike-related US pause (Figure 7). |
2) The comparison of the IpN neurons with predicted responses based on models of summed PC inputs (Figure 5) is very interesting. However, the authors in general seem to discount the role of non-PC inputs to DCN, specifically mossy fibers. For instance, the larger facilitation in neurons that show a CS-trough is interpreted as being due to rebound potentiation. How can enhanced MF plasticity onto those neurons (resulting from stronger CS modulation) be ruled out?
With the additional data that is now included in the manuscript (particularly in Figure 8F-H), it has become apparent that, after training, a US-only trial can elicit a spike trough and subsequent rapid excitatory response with a profile that quite accurately fits that of the trough and rapid excitation observed after the CS. This suggests that a contribution of potentiated mossy fiber input is not necessary per se. Still, we do not rule out MF plasticity, and would in fact agree that it could play a role in the facilitation observed in IpN neurons showing the CS-related spike trough. For MF-DCN synapses to potentiate, their activation likely needs to be combined with a post-inhibitory rebound depolarization (Pugh and Raman, 2006, 2008), which we propose could be provided by the synchronized complex spike input likely underlying the CS pause. Still, if the larger facilitation we report is indeed partly due to potentiated mossy fiber collateral synapses, the CS signals conveyed through them from the pontine nuclei would have to have a curious temporal profile, with a maximum right after the CS-related spike trough. While this is a rather tough requirement for MF-DCN LTP to explain the larger IpN facilitation we report, the temporal profiles of pontine activity reflecting 300 ms tone CSs does offer some plausibility (Figure 3 in Clark, Gohl, and Lavond, 1997). The more straightforward version of rebound excitation, not dependent on LTP at MF-DCN synapses, also involves the confluence of excitation and inhibition, which leads to concomitant activation of GABAA and mGluRs that together drive subsequent rebound firing (Zheng and Raman, 2011) with a temporal profile that readily fits the temporal profile of the larger IpN facilitation we observed. While the inhibition may be provided by the possibly synchronized CS-related complex spikes, the requisite excitation could be provided by mossy and/or climbing fiber collaterals. We now make reference to the potential role of both mossy and climbing fiber collaterals in the Discussion.
3) It is unclear about the putative relationship between PC complex spikes and the peak activity shown in Figure 4C. From Blenkinsop and Lang (2011), it would seem that if there is facilitation in the DCN following a PC complex spike, it should be brief compared to the depression (representing short-latency climbing fiber excitatory collaterals rather than complex spike related inhibition from the PCs). However, the duration of the excitatory peak in Figure 4C, F seems comparable to those provided in the early part of the same figure. Are these really complex spike mediated responses? Consider that IpN cell 2 in Figure 4C seems to show suppression with a presumptive complex spike following the US (opposite of the bursting relationship after CS).
In essence, there is no data to distinguish clearly between complex spike vs. climbing fiber afferent regulating DCN responses. The reviewers were concerned that the impact statement (as well as parts of the Discussion) goes beyond the data. Please revise the impact statement and other related descriptions to reflect this concern.
Indeed, we agree that our datasets did not differentiate between complex spike vs climbing fiber afferent induced facilitation. Therefore, we are now more cautious to draw any conclusion in the Discussion. Interestingly enough, CS related peak responses were rarely observed in the second dataset added to the study, suggesting it is a rare phenomenon within eyelid-controlling regions of the IpN. We therefore relegated this response to Figure 4—figure supplement 1, now only presenting it as a curiosity. Thus, on the broader point of overreach in the impact statement and parts of the Discussion regarding a potential contribution of climbing fiber collaterals, we agree that this assertion was not based on sufficiently robust data in our study and falls short of accurately conveying the actual significance of our findings. As a consequence, together with the remarks in point 6, we have now rephrased the impact statement to better reflect our findings, and toned down our extrapolations.
4) The Materials and methods section for the optogenetic experiments is missing.
We have now added the missing section.
5) Simultaneous activity of presynaptic PCs likely inhibits IpN cells well below their basal firing rate. Depending on the duration of stimulation, this could affect the kinematics of the unconditioned response. Do the authors see any changes in the kinematics of the eye blink for stimulation versus pre/early-training in which there is no CR?
To explore the possibility that optogenetic stimulation could affect the US responses, we compared the US responses with optogenetic inhibition with those in the trails with no CR. In essence, we looked at the difference in UR kinematics between trials with a CR and those without a CR, where the No-CR trials were due to optogenetic stimulation in one dataset, and due to a natural absence of behavior in the other dataset (IpN recordings). From the latter dataset, we took all recordings (n = 12, shown in panel A) with at least 30 trials, with conditioned behavior (>20%), but no more than 90% CRs, and with an average CR amplitude that is within the range of the average CR amplitudes in the three optogenetic experiments (shown in panel B). For each recording/session, we next extracted the difference between the averages of CR and No CR trials (panel C). Finally, for each recording/session, we determined the z-scores, by performing a bootstrap analysis of 500 repetitions (panel D). The bootstrap specifics (per recording/session): for each repetition, we randomly redistributed trials across the CR/No-CR groups, retaining the original Ns per group, and computed the difference as in C. The z-scores for the actual differences in C were then computed by subtracting for each data-point the corresponding mean of the bootstrapped dataset and subsequently dividing by the corresponding standard deviation of the bootstrapped dataset.

Whereas on average the results indicate no significant difference between CR and No-CR trials anywhere in the UR phase in the IpN dataset, we do find an effect in the optogenetics data. All three experiments are suggestive of a tendency of the mice to keep their eyelid more closed between 100-300 ms post-US (= post-stimulation offset), with session’s average passing the 2 SD line, and another even passing the 3 SD line. The time window within which the average z-score trace for the optogenetic data was above 2 SDs is indicated by the shaded pink area in panels B and D. The delay between stimulation offset and this time window of substantial difference is roughly similar to that reported in Witter et al. (2013; Figure 5F). Thus, the data here presented suggest a behavioral effect due to post-stimulation rebound that is consistent with previous work using a similar optogenetic methodology (Witter et al., 2013). Given the already substantial size of the manuscript and this observation’s relative distance from the central aims of this study, we would prefer not to include it in the paper, unless the reviewers/Editors disagree.
6) It is unclear whether the results of this study are applicable to cerebral cortical interactions with the thalamus as suggested in the first paragraph of the Introduction and last paragraph of the Discussion. The reviewers thought that the nature of connectivity is very different, with cerebellar cortical output being exclusively inhibitory, for example. The reviewers thought that the Introduction is oversold and largely irrelevant. The reviewers suggest removing the discussion on cortical-subcortical interactions.
We have now removed most of the cortical-subcortical angle, and rewrote the Introduction’s first and Discussion’s last paragraph to more tightly reflect the scope of our study.
7) The author's trial-by-trial correlation analysis could be strengthened. The authors calculated correlations between 20ms bins of spike responses and behavior across trials. This analysis inherently assumes that each 20ms bin is independent, which is not the case. It would be good to use the actual time-dependent spiking responses to predict the CR behavior in time. A potential analysis: average rate responses of a neuron across trials in which a CR occurred (e.g., day 7+). For the same trial period determine the learned CR as the average difference between the eye position trace after learning and the position traces before learning. Now do a correlational analysis between these two average timeseries. What is the optimal lag/lead across neurons? How well does the facilitation timeseries predict the learned CR response (in time)?
The relation between the amplitudes of spiking and eyelid response can be analyzed in two orthogonal ways: I) by correlating across time points within individual trials or the average across trials, as now implemented in Figure 2—figure supplement 1 as per the reviewers’ suggestion; or II) by correlating across trials at individual time points (Figure 2E, F) or the average across time points within the CS-US interval (Figure 2C, D), which is our original approach. Whereas the data in the former across-time points analysis concern a discrete sampling of a continuous process (one could interpolate between time data points), the data from the across-trial analysis actually concerns discrete, independent measurements (from different trials), and is therefore suitable for statistical inferences, as we draw from single analyses in Figure 2C, D.
The temporal dynamics of the relationship between spike and eyelid modulation can be explored in terms of two aspects that are also orthogonal: I) the degree of relatedness at different times in the trial timespan; and II) the degree of relatedness at different temporal offsets, i.e. the lead/lag relationship. The suggested temporal cross-correlation method explores the latter, but gives no information about which part or parts of the trial timespan carry the relatedness between spikes and eyelid. By contrast, the correlation matrix analysis, as shown in Figure 2E, F, readily allows for the integrated evaluation of both aspects: relatedness can be assessed at different times in the trial timespan (I) by moving along the diagonal parallel to the dashed white line, and at different lead/lag relationships (II) by moving along the perpendicular/orthogonal diagonal. Thus, different temporal components of the spike-eyelid relationship can be identified, at different times and with different lead/lag relationships, such as the two hotspots in the Purkinje cell correlation matrix in ten Brinke et al. (2015; Figure 2D). The early hotspot in that matrix, relating the short CS-complex spike response to the full CR, and the relatively broad hotspot at an adjacent time in Figure 6A are consistent with the notion of a CS-related complex spike input triggering rebound-excitation in the IpN. Thus, the correlation matrix approach allows for but does not assume independent components across time-bins, whereas the suggested temporal cross-correlation approach actually assumes dependence across time points. This can be problematic when using spike activity with multiple components that may or may not be interdependent (as we see in our dataset), because with across-timepoint correlations, these components are conflated.
Nevertheless, we agree that temporal cross-correlation could indeed strengthen our analysis, for two reasons. First, in the event there is little meaningful across-trial amplitude variation, it can still provide information about the lead/lag relationship between the conditioned spike and eyelid responses. Second, when applied to each trial individually instead of the average across trials, the resultant dataset can give a 95% confidence interval of the optimal lead/lag relationship, based on independent measurements. This analysis is now included in the manuscript (Figure 2—figure supplement 1), and shows lead/lag relationships that are consistent with our original analyses.
8) The emergence of facilitation and suppression shown in Figure 1M occurs much more rapidly than does the emergence of behavior. This seems to disagree with the correlation results and optogenetic stimulation in the rest of the paper. If IpN facilitation is both necessary and sufficient to drive eye-blink behavior why does the end of Day 5 (which has approximately the same facilitation as Day 7) have significantly less CRs? Perhaps this is due to averaging spike responses across the entire CS-US period in Figure 1. If the authors plotted the 20ms bin average at the ideal lag (80ms), would this look qualitatively different that the average responses plotted in Figure 1M (e.g., emerging slower)?
The most straightforward conceptualization for simultaneous emergence of spike modulation and behavior would assume a uniform one-on-one relation, meaning the magnitude of spike modulation is representative of its population and proportional to the corresponding eyelid behavior. Based on our data, we think there could well be a threshold that needs to be reached before spike modulation starts manifesting as CRs. This could be in terms of magnitude of modulation, and/or number of IpN cells being recruited.
The finding that spike modulation occurring ahead of eyelid behavior is supported by previous work, such as Freeman and Nicholson (2000), Halverson, Lee, and Freeman (2010), and McCormick and Thompson (1984).
As per the reviewers’ suggestion, we have plotted the facilitation data from Figure 1M in 20ms bins, in four different ways, shown in Author response image 3. Red and blue distinguish between relative firing rates expressed in Hz or in Z-scores (resp.), and plain and light color shades distinguish between the full CS-US interval and the CS-US interval up until 170ms (resp.). These two ranges assume no lag or an 80ms lag, respectively, which together with the eyelid velocity analysis, and the new temporal cross-correlational analyses, is the outer limit (the optimal lag likely lies between the two). Note that the recordings included here all met the facilitation criterion, like the corresponding data in Figure 1M. While among these different visualizations one may agree with CR development better than others, there does not seem to be a game-changing difference with regards to the matter of asynchronous development of spike and eyelid modulation.

Altogether, rather than suboptimal averaging of spike responses, we would suggest, as presented in the manuscript, that the development of the spike modulation ahead of the behavioral expression reflects the actual development of the conditioning process.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.28132.028Article and author information
Author details
Funding
National Institutes of Health (R01MH093727)
- Javier F Medina
National Institutes of Health (RF1MH114269)
- Javier F Medina
National Institutes of Health (R21AA025572)
- Javier F Medina
Nederlandse Organisatie voor Wetenschappelijk Onderzoek
- Zhenyu Gao
- Chris I De Zeeuw
European Research Council
- Chris I De Zeeuw
The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.
Acknowledgements
Authors are grateful to Erika Goedknegt and Elise Haasdijk for technical assistance. We thank the Dutch Organization for Medical Sciences (ZonMw; CIDZ), Life Sciences (ALW; ZG, CIDZ), the ERC-advanced, CEREBNET and C7 programs of the European Community (CIDZ), and NIH grants R01MH093727, RF1MH114269 and R21AA025572 (JFM) for their financial support.
Ethics
Animal experimentation: The experiments were approved by the institutional animal welfare committee (Erasmus MC, Rotterdam, The Netherlands). All surgery was performed under isoflurane anaesthesia, and every effort was made to minimize suffering.
Reviewing Editor
- Naoshige Uchida, Harvard University, United States
Version history
- Received: May 1, 2017
- Accepted: December 6, 2017
- Accepted Manuscript published: December 15, 2017 (version 1)
- Version of Record published: January 9, 2018 (version 2)
Copyright
© 2017, ten Brinke et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
-
- 3,491
- Page views
-
- 562
- Downloads
-
- 66
- Citations
Article citation count generated by polling the highest count across the following sources: Scopus, Crossref, PubMed Central.
Download links
Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)
Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)
Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)
Further reading
-
- Cell Biology
- Neuroscience
The amyloid beta (Aβ) plaques found in Alzheimer’s disease (AD) patients’ brains contain collagens and are embedded extracellularly. Several collagens have been proposed to influence Aβ aggregate formation, yet their role in clearance is unknown. To investigate the potential role of collagens in forming and clearance of extracellular aggregates in vivo, we created a transgenic Caenorhabditis elegans strain that expresses and secretes human Aβ1-42. This secreted Aβ forms aggregates in two distinct places within the extracellular matrix. In a screen for extracellular human Aβ aggregation regulators, we identified different collagens to ameliorate or potentiate Aβ aggregation. We show that a disintegrin and metalloprotease a disintegrin and metalloprotease 2 (ADM-2), an ortholog of ADAM9, reduces the load of extracellular Aβ aggregates. ADM-2 is required and sufficient to remove the extracellular Aβ aggregates. Thus, we provide in vivo evidence of collagens essential for aggregate formation and metalloprotease participating in extracellular Aβ aggregate removal.
-
- Computational and Systems Biology
- Neuroscience
The cerebellar granule cell layer has inspired numerous theoretical models of neural representations that support learned behaviors, beginning with the work of Marr and Albus. In these models, granule cells form a sparse, combinatorial encoding of diverse sensorimotor inputs. Such sparse representations are optimal for learning to discriminate random stimuli. However, recent observations of dense, low-dimensional activity across granule cells have called into question the role of sparse coding in these neurons. Here, we generalize theories of cerebellar learning to determine the optimal granule cell representation for tasks beyond random stimulus discrimination, including continuous input-output transformations as required for smooth motor control. We show that for such tasks, the optimal granule cell representation is substantially denser than predicted by classical theories. Our results provide a general theory of learning in cerebellum-like systems and suggest that optimal cerebellar representations are task-dependent.