Prediction error and repetition suppression have distinct effects on neural representations of visual information

  1. Matthew F Tang  Is a corresponding author
  2. Cooper A Smout
  3. Ehsan Arabzadeh
  4. Jason B Mattingley
  1. The University of Queensland, Australia
  2. The Australian National University, Australia

Abstract

Predictive coding theories argue that recent experience establishes expectations in the brain that generate prediction errors when violated. Prediction errors provide a possible explanation for repetition suppression, where evoked neural activity is attenuated across repeated presentations of the same stimulus. The predictive coding account argues repetition suppression arises because repeated stimuli are expected, whereas non-repeated stimuli are unexpected and thus elicit larger neural responses. Here we employed electroencephalography in humans to test the predictive coding account of repetition suppression by presenting sequences of visual gratings with orientations that were expected either to repeat or change in separate blocks of trials. We applied multivariate forward modelling to determine how orientation selectivity was affected by repetition and prediction. Unexpected stimuli were associated with significantly enhanced orientation selectivity, whereas selectivity was unaffected for repeated stimuli. Our results suggest that repetition suppression and expectation have separable effects on neural representations of visual feature information.

Data availability

The EEG data have been deposited on Dryad 10.5061/dryad.3d7kq

The following data sets were generated

Article and author information

Author details

  1. Matthew F Tang

    Queensland Brain Institute, The University of Queensland, St Lucia, Australia
    For correspondence
    m.tang1@uq.edu.au
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0001-5858-5126
  2. Cooper A Smout

    Queensland Brain Institute, The University of Queensland, St Lucia, Australia
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  3. Ehsan Arabzadeh

    Eccles Institute of Neuroscience, The Australian National University, Canberra, Australia
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  4. Jason B Mattingley

    Queensland Brain Institute, The University of Queensland, St Lucia, Australia
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.

Funding

Australian Research Council (CE140100007)

  • Ehsan Arabzadeh
  • Jason B Mattingley

Australian Research Council (DP170100908)

  • Ehsan Arabzadeh

Australian Research Council (FL110100103)

  • Jason B Mattingley

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Ethics

Human subjects: Each participant provided written informed consent prior to participation. The study was approved by The University of Queensland Human Research Ethics Committee (approval number 2012000392) and was in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki

Copyright

© 2018, Tang et al.

This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License permitting unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.

Metrics

  • 3,869
    views
  • 516
    downloads
  • 51
    citations

Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.

Download links

A two-part list of links to download the article, or parts of the article, in various formats.

Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)

Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)

Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)

  1. Matthew F Tang
  2. Cooper A Smout
  3. Ehsan Arabzadeh
  4. Jason B Mattingley
(2018)
Prediction error and repetition suppression have distinct effects on neural representations of visual information
eLife 7:e33123.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.33123

Share this article

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.33123

Further reading

    1. Computational and Systems Biology
    2. Neuroscience
    Anna Cattani, Don B Arnold ... Nancy Kopell
    Research Article

    The basolateral amygdala (BLA) is a key site where fear learning takes place through synaptic plasticity. Rodent research shows prominent low theta (~3–6 Hz), high theta (~6–12 Hz), and gamma (>30 Hz) rhythms in the BLA local field potential recordings. However, it is not understood what role these rhythms play in supporting the plasticity. Here, we create a biophysically detailed model of the BLA circuit to show that several classes of interneurons (PV, SOM, and VIP) in the BLA can be critically involved in producing the rhythms; these rhythms promote the formation of a dedicated fear circuit shaped through spike-timing-dependent plasticity. Each class of interneurons is necessary for the plasticity. We find that the low theta rhythm is a biomarker of successful fear conditioning. The model makes use of interneurons commonly found in the cortex and, hence, may apply to a wide variety of associative learning situations.

    1. Neuroscience
    Juan Carlos Boffi, Brice Bathellier ... Robert Prevedel
    Research Article

    Sound location coding has been extensively studied at the central nucleus of the mammalian inferior colliculus (CNIC), supporting a population code. However, this population code has not been extensively characterized on the single-trial level with simultaneous recordings or at other anatomical regions like the dorsal cortex of inferior colliculus (DCIC), which is relevant for learning-induced experience dependent plasticity. To address these knowledge gaps, here we made in two complementary ways large-scale recordings of DCIC populations from awake mice in response to sounds delivered from 13 different frontal horizontal locations (azimuths): volumetric two-photon calcium imaging with ~700 cells simultaneously recorded at a relatively low temporal resolution, and high-density single-unit extracellular recordings with ~20 cells simultaneously recorded at a high temporal resolution. Independent of the method, the recorded DCIC population responses revealed substantial trial-to-trial variation (neuronal noise) which was significantly correlated across pairs of neurons (noise correlations) in the passively listening condition. Nevertheless, decoding analysis supported that these noisy response patterns encode sound location on the single-trial basis, reaching errors that match the discrimination ability of mice. The detected noise correlations contributed to minimize the error of the DCIC population code of sound azimuth. Altogether these findings point out that DCIC can encode sound location in a similar format to what has been proposed for CNIC, opening exciting questions about how noise correlations could shape this code in the context of cortico-collicular input and experience-dependent plasticity.