Abstract

Cognitive and social capacities require postnatal experience, yet the pathways by which experience guides development are unknown. Here we show that the normal development of motor and nonmotor capacities requires cerebellar activity. Using chemogenetic perturbation of molecular layer interneurons to attenuate cerebellar output in mice, we found that activity of posterior regions in juvenile life modulates adult expression of eyeblink conditioning (paravermal lobule VI, crus I), reversal learning (lobule VI), persistive behavior and novelty-seeking (lobule VII), and social preference (crus I/II). Perturbation in adult life altered only a subset of phenotypes. Both adult and juvenile disruption left gait metrics largely unaffected. Contributions to phenotypes increased with the amount of lobule inactivated. Using an anterograde transsynaptic tracer, we found that posterior cerebellum made strong connections with prelimbic, orbitofrontal, and anterior cingulate cortex. These findings provide anatomical substrates for the clinical observation that cerebellar injury increases the risk of autism.

Data availability

Code and data for the main figures are available via the GitHub repository https://github.com/wanglabprinceton/behavioral-development.The complete raw data for this study are available from the corresponding author upon request (including behavioral videos and serial two-photon tomographic brain images of each mouse).

Article and author information

Author details

  1. Aleksandra Badura

    Princeton Neuroscience Institute, Princeton University, Princeton, United States
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  2. Jessica L Verpeut

    Princeton Neuroscience Institute, Princeton University, Princeton, United States
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  3. Julia W Metzger

    Princeton Neuroscience Institute, Princeton University, Princeton, United States
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  4. Talmo D Pereira

    Princeton Neuroscience Institute, Princeton University, Princeton, United States
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0001-9075-8365
  5. Thomas J Pisano

    Princeton Neuroscience Institute, Princeton University, Princeton, United States
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  6. Ben Deverett

    Princeton Neuroscience Institute, Princeton University, Princeton, United States
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0002-3119-7649
  7. Dariya E Bakshinskaya

    Princeton Neuroscience Institute, Princeton University, Princeton, United States
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  8. Samuel S-H Wang

    Princeton Neuroscience Institute, Princeton University, Princeton, United States
    For correspondence
    sswang@princeton.edu
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0002-0490-9786

Funding

Nederlandse Organisatie voor Wetenschappelijk Onderzoek (Innovational Research Incentives Scheme VENI (NWO ZonMw))

  • Aleksandra Badura

Nancy Lurie Marks Family Foundation

  • Samuel S-H Wang

National Institutes of Health (R01 NS045193)

  • Samuel S-H Wang

New Jersey Commission on Brain Injury Research (CBIR16FEL010)

  • Jessica L Verpeut

National Science Foundation (Graduate Research Fellowship DGE-1148900)

  • Talmo D Pereira

Rutgers Robert Wood Johnson Medical School-Princeton University M.D.-Ph.D. Program

  • Thomas J Pisano
  • Ben Deverett

National Institutes of Health (R01 MH115750)

  • Samuel S-H Wang

National Institutes of Health (F30 MH115577)

  • Ben Deverett

National Institutes of Health (F31 NS089303)

  • Thomas J Pisano

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Ethics

Animal experimentation: Experimental procedures were approved by the Princeton University Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (protocol number: 1943-16) and performed in accordance with the animal welfare guidelines of the National Institutes of Health. All mice were housed in Optimice cages (Animal Care Systems, Centennial, CO) containing blended bedding (The Andersons, Maumee, OH), paper nesting strips, and one heat-dried virgin pulp cardboard hut (Shepherd Specialty Papers, Milford, NJ). PicoLab Rodent Diet food pellets (LabDiet, St. Louis, MO) and drinking water (or CNO water in the developmental groups) were provided ad libitum. Mice were relocated to clean cages with new component materials every two weeks. All mice were group-housed in reverse light cycle to promote maximal performance during behavioral testing. All surgery was performed under isoflurane anesthesia (5% for induction, 1-2% in oxygen; 1 L/min) and daily monitoring was employed to minimize suffering.

Copyright

© 2018, Badura et al.

This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License permitting unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.

Metrics

  • 8,277
    views
  • 1,086
    downloads
  • 148
    citations

Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.

Download links

A two-part list of links to download the article, or parts of the article, in various formats.

Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)

Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)

Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)

  1. Aleksandra Badura
  2. Jessica L Verpeut
  3. Julia W Metzger
  4. Talmo D Pereira
  5. Thomas J Pisano
  6. Ben Deverett
  7. Dariya E Bakshinskaya
  8. Samuel S-H Wang
(2018)
Normal cognitive and social development require posterior cerebellar activity
eLife 7:e36401.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.36401

Share this article

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.36401

Further reading

    1. Neuroscience
    Rongxin Fang, Aaron Halpern ... Xiaowei Zhuang
    Tools and Resources

    Multiplexed error-robust fluorescence in situ hybridization (MERFISH) allows genome-scale imaging of RNAs in individual cells in intact tissues. To date, MERFISH has been applied to image thin-tissue samples of ~10 µm thickness. Here, we present a thick-tissue three-dimensional (3D) MERFISH imaging method, which uses confocal microscopy for optical sectioning, deep learning for increasing imaging speed and quality, as well as sample preparation and imaging protocol optimized for thick samples. We demonstrated 3D MERFISH on mouse brain tissue sections of up to 200 µm thickness with high detection efficiency and accuracy. We anticipate that 3D thick-tissue MERFISH imaging will broaden the scope of questions that can be addressed by spatial genomics.

    1. Neuroscience
    Sam E Benezra, Kripa B Patel ... Randy M Bruno
    Research Article

    Learning alters cortical representations and improves perception. Apical tuft dendrites in cortical layer 1, which are unique in their connectivity and biophysical properties, may be a key site of learning-induced plasticity. We used both two-photon and SCAPE microscopy to longitudinally track tuft-wide calcium spikes in apical dendrites of layer 5 pyramidal neurons in barrel cortex as mice learned a tactile behavior. Mice were trained to discriminate two orthogonal directions of whisker stimulation. Reinforcement learning, but not repeated stimulus exposure, enhanced tuft selectivity for both directions equally, even though only one was associated with reward. Selective tufts emerged from initially unresponsive or low-selectivity populations. Animal movement and choice did not account for changes in stimulus selectivity. Enhanced selectivity persisted even after rewards were removed and animals ceased performing the task. We conclude that learning produces long-lasting realignment of apical dendrite tuft responses to behaviorally relevant dimensions of a task.