Analogue closed-loop optogenetic modulation of hippocampal pyramidal cells dissociates gamma frequency and amplitude
Abstract
Gamma-band oscillations are implicated in modulation of attention, integration of sensory information and flexible communication among anatomically connected brain areas. How networks become entrained is incompletely understood. Specifically, it is unclear how the spectral and temporal characteristics of network oscillations can be altered on rapid timescales needed for efficient communication. We use closed-loop optogenetic modulation of principal cell excitability in mouse hippocampal slices to interrogate the dynamical properties of hippocampal oscillations. Gamma frequency and amplitude can be modulated bi-directionally, and dissociated, by phase-advancing or delaying optogenetic feedback to pyramidal cells. Closed-loop modulation alters the synchrony rather than average frequency of action potentials, in principle avoiding disruption of population rate-coding of information. Modulation of phasic excitatory currents in principal neurons is sufficient to manipulate oscillations, suggesting that feed-forward excitation of pyramidal cells has an important role in determining oscillatory dynamics and the ability of networks to couple with one another.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.38346.001Introduction
Gamma-band (approximately 30 to 120 Hz) oscillations have been implicated in the modulation of attention and perception, in action initiation, spatial navigation and memory encoding, and have also been proposed to underlie flexible information routing among anatomically connected regions (Akam and Kullmann, 2010; Akam and Kullmann, 2014; Börgers and Kopell, 2003; Fries, 2005; Kirst et al., 2016; Lisman, 2010; Rodriguez et al., 1999; Salinas and Sejnowski, 2001; Schnitzler and Gross, 2005). Central to several of these proposed roles is the ability of gamma oscillations in different areas to enter into, and exit, states of synchrony with one another (Akam et al., 2012; Fries, 2015; Varela et al., 2001). Evidence for behavioral-state-dependent coupling and uncoupling comes from variable oscillatory coherence among distinct components of the visual cortex, correlating with selective stimulus attention (Bosman et al., 2012; Grothe et al., 2012). An earlier study in the rodent hippocampal formation showed that the CA1 subfield can flip between a state of coherence with the medial entorhinal cortex at ~110 Hz and a state of coherence with the CA3 subfield at ~40 Hz, correlating with information flow through the temporo-ammonic and Schaffer collateral pathways, respectively (Colgin et al., 2009). Although several experimental confounds cloud the interpretation of coherence measured from local field potential (LFP) recordings (Buzsáki and Schomburg, 2015), these studies provide some of the most compelling evidence that gamma-band oscillatory entrainment underlies flexible functional connectivity.
Although the cellular mechanisms underlying gamma oscillations have been extensively studied (Bartos et al., 2007; Buzsáki and Wang, 2012), there remain uncertainties over the fundamental determinants of their dynamics and the relative contributions of excitatory and inhibitory signalling. Gamma-band oscillations can be induced in vitro in the presence of blockers of ionotropic glutamate receptors (Whittington et al., 1995), or in vivo by optogenetic stimulation of parvalbumin-positive interneurons (Cardin et al., 2009; Sohal et al., 2009), underlining the importance of fast perisomatic inhibition (Bartos et al., 2002; Fisahn et al., 2004; Mann et al., 2005). Robust population oscillations can also be simulated in exclusively inhibitory networks (Wang and Buzsáki, 1996). These experimental and computational observations emphasize the importance of inhibitory kinetics. Nevertheless, gamma-band oscillations can be entrained by sinusoidal optogenetic stimulation of pyramidal neurons in an in vitro hippocampal slice preparation (Akam et al., 2012). This observation implies that phasic depolarization of principal cells can determine the gamma rhythm and argues against a model where the only role of pyramidal cells is to tonically depolarize a network of reciprocally coupled interneurons (Bartos et al., 2007; Tiesinga and Sejnowski, 2009).
Further insight into the dynamical mechanisms of synchronization between oscillating networks comes from examining the phase response curve (PRC) of the network oscillation, defined as the phase advance or delay produced by a transient stimulation, as a function of the instantaneous phase at which the stimulus is delivered. The finding that gamma in an in vitro hippocampal slice preparation shows a biphasic PRC (Akam et al., 2012) is consistent with the hypothesis that this oscillation can be entrained by appropriately modulated afferent activity. The shape of the PRC is furthermore accurately reproduced with a simple neural mass model (Wilson and Cowan, 1972), where extracellular electrical or optogenetic stimuli are represented as transient perturbations of the instantaneous level of excitation or inhibition (Akam et al., 2012). Recent theoretical work has derived population PRCs for oscillations in spiking network models, providing an insight into how mechanisms of oscillation generation determine entrainment properties (Akao et al., 2018; Kotani et al., 2014). Nevertheless, there remains a large gap between the PRC and understanding the determinants of the oscillatory frequency and interactions between gamma-generating circuits.
The present study investigates the dynamical properties of gamma oscillations by using closed-loop optogenetics to create an artificial feedback loop between the oscillatory network activity (as assessed by the LFP) and excitatory input to the principal cell population. Specifically, we delivered analogue-modulated excitation whose strength was a function of the instantaneous phase and amplitude of the oscillation. This approach is quite distinct from previous closed-loop applications of optogenetics (Grosenick et al., 2015), which have adopted one of four main strategies. First, several studies have used the detection of a change in the state of a network, such as the onset of an electrographic seizure (Krook-Magnuson et al., 2013; Paz et al., 2013) or sharp-wave ripple (Stark et al., 2014), to trigger light delivery and return the network to its ground state. Second, light pulses have been timed according to the phase of a theta oscillation (Siegle and Wilson, 2014), while examining the consequences for behaviour. In the latter example, the theta oscillation itself was not altered. Third, optogenetics has been used to regulate the overall activity of a population of neurons at a desired level (Newman et al., 2015). Fourth, optogenetic depolarization of interneurons, triggered by spikes in an individual principal cell, has been used to simulate a feedback inhibitory loop to interrogate their role in gamma (Sohal et al., 2009; Veit et al., 2017). The goal of the present investigation is qualitatively different: to understand how the spectral characteristics of gamma are affected by rhythmic excitation arriving at different phases. Computational simulations have suggested that closed-loop optogenetics could be used to adjust the phase of gamma (Witt et al., 2013), but whether it can alter its frequency or amplitude remains unclear.
Results
Closed-loop feedback modulation of affects gamma oscillations in CA1
We expressed the red-shifted optogenetic actuator C1V1 (Yizhar et al., 2011) in the mouse hippocampus CA1 under the Camk2a promoter to bias expression to excitatory neurons. The local field potential (LFP) was recorded in the CA1 pyramidal cell layer in acute hippocampal slices. A slowly increasing ramp of light (peak wavelength 590 nm) was delivered via a light-emitting diode (LED) coupled to the epifluorescence port of an upright microscope, eliciting a gamma oscillation (Figure 1a,b), as previously reported in rodents (Adesnik, 2018; Adesnik and Scanziani, 2010; Akam et al., 2012; Butler et al., 2016; Pastoll et al., 2013), cats (Ni et al., 2016) and monkeys (Lu et al., 2015).
-
Figure 1—source data 1
- https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.38346.003
In order to investigate the role of phasic excitation in setting the dynamical properties of gamma, we used the LFP itself to manipulate the optogenetic drive in real time. The LED driver command was multiplied by a simple function of the instantaneous value of the LFP and its time-derivative: (1 + k1 LFP + k2 dLFP/dt), where k1 and k2 are positive or negative constants. These operations were implemented with a field-programmable gate array (FPGA) and applied for a defined duration (typically 1 or 2 s) during the ramp. This yielded a change in the spectral properties of the oscillation, which lasted for the duration of the closed-loop feedback (Figure 1c). Because both the LFP and its time-derivative fluctuated about 0, the ‘gamma clamp’ had little effect on the average illumination intensity relative to an unmodulated ramp. Changes in the oscillation frequency or power could therefore not be attributed to a net increase or decrease in the average optogenetic drive to pyramidal neurons.
We adjusted the clamp function by altering the values of k1 and k2 and asked whether the frequency and/or power of the gamma oscillation can be modulated bidirectionally. Changes in spectral properties were related to the phase difference between the LFP and the LED drive during the clamp, as estimated from the cross-spectrum at maximal magnitude. In-phase modulation, achieved by setting k1 positive and k2 = 0, led to an increase in oscillatory power and frequency (Figure 2a). Modulating the ramp in anti-phase relative to the LFP, by setting k1 negative, led to a decrease in both frequency and power (Figure 2b). Advancing the phase of the clamp by approximately 90°, achieved by setting k1 = 0 and k2 positive, increased the frequency of the oscillation whilst decreasing its power (Figure 2c). Finally, a decrease in frequency and increase in power was achieved by delaying the trough of the clamp modulation relative to the LFP, by setting k2 negative (Figure 2D). Detailed inspection of the ramp command waveform during the clamp shows that it was in some cases distorted relative to the LFP, as expected from its non-sinusoidal shape (Cole and Voytek, 2017) (e.g. Figure 2c,d), and so the LED-LFP phase differences were only approximate.
-
Figure 2—source data 1
- https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.38346.005
Attempts to estimate the instantaneous oscillation phase, for instance using a Hilbert transform, and to use this to phase-advance or phase-delay a template of the LFP, compressed or stretched in time, were unsuccessful: the phase jitter and cycle-to-cycle variability in the amplitude and frequency of the gamma oscillation (see LFP traces in Figure 2) prevented accurate estimation of these parameters in the face of closed-loop feedback.
Oscillation clamp is broadly consistent with the phase response curve of gamma
Changes in frequency and power, expressed in relation to the approximate phase difference between the LED command and the LFP, were qualitatively consistent across experiments (Figure 3a–c). Moreover, as the LED-LFP phase difference was rotated through a complete cycle, the effect on the oscillation in the two-dimensional plane defined by the change in oscillation frequency and power also rotated through 360°, such that with the appropriate phase of closed-loop feedback the network oscillation could be pushed in any desired direction in the oscillation frequency-power space (Figure 3c).
-
Figure 3—source data 1
- https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.38346.007
To gain a mechanistic insight, we asked if the characteristic relationship between the frequency change and the LED-LFP phase difference could be explained by the shape of the phase-response curve (PRC) previously reported (Akam et al., 2012). In that study, a brief ‘kick’ was applied on top of the LED ramp command, and the phase advance or delay of subsequent oscillations was related to the phase of the LFP at which the transient occurred. A phase delay was observed when the transient optogenetic stimulus was delivered at the trough of the LFP, when pyramidal neurons are most likely to fire. The maximal phase advance, in contrast, occurred when the stimulus was delivered approximately one third of a cycle after the trough of the LFP. Assuming linear behaviour, the effect of modulating the light intensity in closed loop can be obtained by averaging the product of the phase shift and the LFP over the entire cycle of the oscillation. The circular cross-correlogram between the typical LFP shape and the PRC should then predict the effect of modulating the optogenetic drive by the shape of the LFP itself at arbitrary degrees of phase advance or delay (Figure 3d). In-phase modulation is expected, on the basis of this calculation, to phase-advance the oscillation, and thus to result in an increase in oscillatory frequency over successive cycles. Anti-phase modulation, in contrast, is predicted to phase-delay the oscillation, and thus to decrease its frequency. The circular cross-correlation is, moreover, asymmetrical, broadly consistent with the shape of the relationship between the change in frequency and LED-LFP phase difference observed in the clamp experiments (Figure 3a).
Although the shape of the PRC is consistent with the changes in gamma frequency achieved with closed-loop modulation at different LED-LFP phase differences, on its own it says nothing about changes in power. Power was maximally decreased with a phase advance of the LED command over the LFP around 90°, whilst it was maximally increased with a phase delay around 90° (Figure 3b). The relative phases at which frequency and power were altered are, however, consistent with the behaviour of a normal form description of a super-critical Hopf bifurcation in the vicinity of its limit-cycle. In this scenario, the LFP would approximate an observed variable, and the optogenetic drive would act in the direction of a hidden variable at a +90° angle to the LFP.
A deeper understanding of the characteristic changes in frequency and power of the LFP with different LED-LFP phase differences requires an insight into how neurons spike and ultimately how membrane currents respond to the fluctuations in optogenetic drive. We therefore performed single-cell recordings in parallel with the LFP recordings.
Gamma clamp affects the timing, not rate, of pyramidal neuron firing
Although the average illumination intensity was not altered during the gamma clamp, for certain LED-LFP phase relationships gamma power increased or decreased robustly. Inhibitory currents in principal neurons, rather than spikes or excitatory currents, have previously been shown to be the main determinant of the LFP (Oren et al., 2010), suggesting that the change in power during the clamp is not a direct effect of the optogenetic drive but results instead from a change in pyramidal neuron synchrony or phase, in a reciprocal relationship with the degree and temporal synchrony of interneuron recruitment. To determine how the clamp affects pyramidal neuron firing, we repeated experiments with an additional patch pipette to record from individual pyramidal neurons in cell-attached mode. Individual action potentials were used to align the simultaneously recorded LFP, and to estimate the phase at which they occurred. During an unmodulated ramp, pyramidal cells tended to spike sparsely, close to the trough of the oscillation, consistent with previous studies of pharmacologically induced oscillations (Fisahn et al., 1998). During the clamp, an increase in oscillatory power was associated with a corresponding increase in the degree of synchrony of pyramidal cell firing: the circular dispersion of LFP phase at which pyramidal cells fired decreased relative to the unclamped situation (Figure 4a). Conversely, a decrease in power was accompanied by a relative desynchronization of pyramidal cell firing. This relationship was qualitatively consistent, as indicated by the change in vector length obtained from the circular average of spike phases (Figure 4c). The vector length increased when oscillation power increased (p=0.02, n = 12, sign test), and decreased when oscillation power decreased (p=0.025, n = 5). Strikingly, however, there was no change in the overall firing rate of pyramidal cells when the oscillation power was increased or decreased by the clamp. Changes in power were thus achieved by tightening the synchrony of firing, or by desynchronizing action potentials, rather than by altering the overall activity of pyramidal neurons.
-
Figure 4—source data 1
- https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.38346.009
Increases in oscillatory frequency were accompanied by a phase advance of pyramidal cell firing relative to the LFP (Figure 4b,c, p=8×10−7, n = 7, Hotellier test – Zar, 2009). A trend for a phase delay was observed in a small number of experiments where frequency-lowering clamp was tested (n = 3). This observation is consistent with the view that changes in the phase of pyramidal neuron action potentials are causally upstream of changes in gamma frequency, even though the current generators of the LFP itself are dominated by GABAergic signaling (Gulyás et al., 2010; Hájos et al., 2004; Oren et al., 2010).
Excitatory current phase in principal cells determines changes in gamma spectral properties
In the examples illustrated in Figure 4a and b, the optogenetic modulation was applied with a phase advance over the LFP of ~0° and ~45° respectively. Why does in-phase modulation result in an increase in power, and phase-advanced modulation result in an increase in frequency? To gain a mechanistic insight into how gamma clamp operates, we examined the phase of excitation experienced by pyramidal neurons during different clamp regimes.
We repeated experiments as above, but with one pipette used to voltage–clamp a pyramidal neuron at the estimated GABAA reversal potential (approximately –70 mV), and the other pipette to record the LFP. We then measured the inward current at each phase of the gamma oscillation, as defined by the LFP, and repeated this over consecutive cycles to obtain an average time-course (Figure 5a). The minimum (that is, least negative) inward current during the average cycle was subtracted to yield an estimate of the phasic excitatory current, which could then be represented as a vector representing its average phase and amplitude (Figure 5b). During unclamped gamma, the excitatory current was small, and its average phase relative to the LFP varied among experiments, as expected from the very sparse synaptic connectivity among pyramidal neurons in CA1 (Deuchars and Thomson, 1996). Gamma clamp imposed a large phasic inward current (Figure 5b). Subtracting the vector representing the baseline phasic inward current yielded a vector representing the net excitatory current imposed by the gamma clamp (ΔE). This lagged behind the LED modulation, reflecting in part the opsin activation and deactivation kinetics (Figure 5c, and arrows in Figure 5b, right). For the example illustrated in Figure 5 an 83° phase advance of the LED over the LFP resulted in ΔE with mean phase of 247°, where the LFP trough is defined as 0°. This yielded an increase in frequency and decrease in power of the gamma oscillation.
-
Figure 5—source data 1
- https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.38346.011
Comparing across different clamp regimes reveals how gamma frequency and power change in relation to the phasic excitation experienced by principal cells (Figure 6a,b). An increase in gamma frequency was achieved when the average excitatory current phase occurred during the down-stroke of the LFP (~180° to 360°), whilst a decrease in frequency was achieved when excitation was applied during the upstroke (~0° to 180°). An increase in power, on the other hand, was achieved with excitation around the trough (~270° to 90°), and a decrease in power occurred with excitation around the peak (~90° to 270°). Given that, under baseline conditions, pyramidal neurons fire maximally near to the trough of the LFP (0°), these data imply that the increase in frequency occurs because they are brought to firing threshold earlier (see also Figure 4b,c). An increase in power, on the other hand, occurs because pyramidal neurons are synchronized by adding a depolarization when they are most likely to fire (see also Figure 4a,c).
-
Figure 6—source data 1
- https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.38346.013
Gamma clamp affects inhibitory currents
Finally, we asked how inhibitory currents in a subset of pyramidal neurons are altered by the closed-loop optogenetic manipulation, by voltage-clamping them around the glutamate reversal potential (0 mV). The mean phase and amplitude of outward inhibitory currents were calculated in a similar way, by subtracting the minimal current from the circular average of the outward GABAA-receptor-mediated current (Figure 7). In contrast to excitatory currents, phasic inhibitory currents under baseline conditions were large, consistent with the major role of feedback interneurons in gamma (Figure 7a,b). Changes in inhibitory currents (ΔI) were relatively smaller than for excitatory currents and were dominated by effects on the power of the oscillation. Thus, for a +83° LED-LFP phase advance, which led to a decrease in gamma power and increase in frequency (same cell as in Figure 5), there was little change in the average phase of the inhibitory current, although it was decreased in amplitude (Figure 7c). Aligning the average currents by the LFP across different trials whilst the cell was held at −70 mV or 0 mV, and during unclamped and clamped periods, yielded an insight into the relationship between excitatory and inhibitory conductances during the oscillatory cycle (Figure 7d). For the example shown in Figure 5 and Figure 7a–c, the effective cycle changed from one dominated by phasic inhibition to one with similar relative amplitudes of phasic inhibition and excitation, with excitation leading inhibition in both cases (Figure 7d, left). In contrast, for a 133° LED-LFP phase delay, which led to a decrease in frequency and increase in power, the inhibitory current increased (Figure 7d, right). Similar results were obtained in eight cells.
-
Figure 7—source data 1
- https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.38346.015
Discussion
The present study shows that closed-loop optogenetic manipulation of principal cells allows predictable, bidirectional and dissociable changes in the power and frequency of gamma oscillations. We observed a broad consistency between the frequency manipulation achieved with closed-loop optogenetic feedback and that predicted from the phase response behavior previously observed with intermittent optogenetic stimuli (Akam et al., 2012). Optogenetically and pharmacologically induced gamma also exhibited similar dynamical properties in that study, implying that the principles uncovered in the present work are not specific to the way gamma oscillations were elicited.
Previous studies have stressed the importance of fast-spiking parvalbumin-positive (PV+) interneurons in gamma (Cardin et al., 2009; Sohal et al., 2009) (but see (Veit et al., 2017)). PV+ basket cells tend to fire with very little phase dispersion, close to one-to-one with each cycle of the oscillation in vitro (Bartos et al., 2007; Gulyás et al., 2010). Our attempts to achieve gamma clamp by targeting interneurons rather than pyramidal cells have thus far been unsuccessful because their out of phase recruitment powerfully suppresses the oscillation (data not shown). The weaker phase-locking of pyramidal than PV+ cell firing to gamma oscillations, together with their sparse firing on successive cycles of gamma (Csicsvari et al., 2003; Gulyás et al., 2010; Tukker et al., 2007), may however confer a broader dynamic range over which they can influence the phase, frequency and amplitude of the oscillation. Taken together with previous evidence that open-loop sinusoidal optogenetic stimulation of principal cells can entrain a gamma oscillation (Akam et al., 2012), the present data underline the importance of action potential timing in principal cells in the spectral and temporal properties of hippocampal gamma, notwithstanding the evidence that the LFP itself is dominated by inhibitory currents in principal cells (Oren et al., 2010), and argue against a model where the function of principal cells is only to depolarize a population of reciprocally connected interneurons.
Closed-loop manipulations have been applied previously in the context of network oscillations, using either electrical and optogenetic stimuli delivered at specific phases of theta or gamma oscillations, in order to probe the mechanisms of long-term plasticity induction (Huerta and Lisman, 1995; Pavlides et al., 1988) or sharp-wave ripple generation (Stark et al., 2014), or to test the theta phase-dependence of memory encoding and retrieval (Siegle and Wilson, 2014). A similar strategy has been used to interrupt experimental thalamocortical seizures (Berényi et al., 2012). However, these studies have not aimed at modulating the amplitude or frequency of an on-going oscillation.
We have focused on gamma because a local circuit is sufficient to generate the oscillation, and we have previously shown that the phase response behaviour of hippocampal gamma is well described by a simple dynamical model (Akam et al., 2012). The circuits underlying theta and other oscillations either involve longer-range connections in the brain or are poorly defined. They are therefore less likely to be amenable to local optogenetic manipulation. This does not exclude the possibility that, for instance, theta oscillations in the hippocampus could be manipulated by closed-loop modulation of excitability in the basal forebrain.
The ability to alter the amplitude and frequency of gamma suggests a versatile tool to test the roles of gamma in information routing and other high-level brain functions, both in health and in disease states such as schizophrenia (Uhlhaas and Singer, 2010). Hitherto, most experimental manipulations of oscillations have relied on periodic stimulation, which can entrain network oscillations (Akam et al., 2012) or evoke oscillations in an otherwise asynchronous network (Cardin et al., 2009; Sohal et al., 2009). Transcranial stimulation designed to entrain oscillations in vivo can bias perception (Neuling et al., 2012; Romei et al., 2010; Thut et al., 2011) and bidirectionally affect performance in motor (Joundi et al., 2012) and working memory (Polanía et al., 2012) tasks. However, external periodic stimulation is not well suited to desynchronize network activity or to suppress oscillatory dynamics. Furthermore, if periodic stimulation is used, the desired change in amplitude or frequency is achieved at the cost of imposing an externally determined phase on the oscillation. This will prevent the oscillation from entraining to endogenous periodic signals such as those arising from other oscillating networks or periodic sensory stimuli.
Closed-loop stimulation, in which signals recorded from a network are used in real time to bias its state, in principle provides an alternative way of manipulating network oscillations, and has been used to interfere with pathological rhythms in models of Parkinson’s disease (Rosin et al., 2011), to suppress Parkinsonian tremor (Brittain et al., 2013), and in a model of thalamocortical epilepsy (Butt et al., 2005). This approach relies on an artificial feedback loop which either counteracts or amplifies the endogenous feedback responsible for synchronizing the network (Rosenblum and Pikovsky, 2004). Importantly, optogenetics has the advantage over electrical stimulation that the modulation can be distributed across a population of neurons. We have, moreover, shown that closed-loop manipulation of a gamma oscillation can be achieved without a net increase or decrease in the average firing rate of neurons, implying that it would not necessarily perturb information represented as an average firing rate code.
Extrapolating from in vitro gamma to the brain in situ presents several technical challenges, including the need for optical fibers to illuminate the tissue and the potential for photoelectrical artifacts. Moreover, oscillations are generally less prominent because the current generators from multiple oscillating and non-oscillating populations overlap, complicating the evaluation of phase and frequency. Nevertheless, the present study identifies some general principles to guide attempts to achieve bidirectional and dissociable modulation of oscillatory frequency and power in vivo. This should allow a definitive test of the causal role of gamma in functions such as attention modulation and information routing (Sohal, 2016).
Materials and methods
All procedures followed the Animals (Scientific Procedures) Act, 1986, and were reviewed by the UCL Institute of Neurology Animal Welfare and Ethical Review Body. P21 male C57 mice were anesthetized with isoflurane and placed in a stereotaxic frame (Kopf Instruments). A suspension of AAV5-CaMKIIa-C1V1(E122T/E162T)-TS-eYFP (UNC Vector Core, titre 5 × 1012 IU/ml) was injected at a rate of 100 nl/min into four sites in both hippocampi (injection volume: 300–500 nl per site). The antero-posterior injection coordinate was taken as 2/3 of the distance from bregma to lambda. The lateral coordinates were 3.0 mm from the midline, and the ventral coordinates were 3.5, 3.0, 2.5 and 2.0 mm from the surface of the skull.
Hippocampal slices were prepared at least 4 weeks later. Animals were sacrificed by pentobarbitone overdose and underwent transcardiac perfusion with an oxygenated solution containing (in mM): 92 N-methyl-D-glucamine-Cl, 2.5 KCl, 1.25 NaH2PO4, 20 HEPES, 30 NaHCO3, 25 glucose, 10 MgCl2, 0.5 CaCl2, 2 thiourea, 5 Na-ascorbate and 3 Na-pyruvate, with sucrose added to achieve an osmolality of 315 mOsm/L. Brain slices (400 μm thick) were prepared at room temperature and then incubated at 37°C for 12 min in the same solution. They were subsequently stored at room temperature, in a solution containing (in mM): 126 NaCl, 3 KCl, 1.25 NaH2PO4, 2 MgSO4, 2 CaCl2, 24 NaHCO3, 10 glucose, shielded from light, before being transferred to the stage of an upright microscope (Olympus BX51WI or Scientifica SliceScope), where they were perfused on both sides with the same solution at 32° C. Expression of C1V1 in CA1 was verified by epifluorescence, and CA3 was ablated to focus on local gamma-generating mechanisms.
Epifluorescence imaging and C1V1 stimulation were achieved with LEDs (OptoLED, Cairn Instruments, or assembled from Thorlabs components using an M590L2 590 nm LED and a DC2100 high-power LED driver). The light source was coupled to the epifluorescence illuminator of the microscope, with a silver mirror in the place of a dichroic cube. Wide-field illumination was delivered via a 20x, 0.5 NA water immersion objective. The current delivered to the LED was kept in the linear input-output range, and the irradiance was <5 mW/mm2. Light ramps typically lasting 8 s were delivered every 30 – 45 s.
LFPs were recorded in the CA1 pyramidal layer using patch pipettes filled with extracellular solution and a Multiclamp 700B amplifier (Molecular Devices), and band-pass filtered between 1 and 200 or 500 Hz. A linear LED ramp command was generated via a multifunction data acquisition card (National Instruments PCI-6221) and, together with the LFP, was digitized using a real-time controller (National Instruments cRIO-9022) with a Xilinx Virtex-5 FPGA (cRIO-9133) operating at a loop rate of 10 kHz. The ramp was multiplied by (1 + k1LFP + k2dLFP/dt), stepping through different values of k in a pseudo-random order for successive trials. dLFP/dt was calculated as the difference between successive digitization values in the FPGA, averaged over successive 2 ms intervals to minimize high-frequency noise. The output of the FPGA/real-time controller was sent to the LED driver, and digitized in parallel with the LFP at 10 kHz on the data acquisition PC.
To study the phase relationship of action potentials and the LFP oscillation, a cell-attached recording was obtained using a second patch pipette held in voltage clamp mode, low-pass filtered at 10 kHz and digitized in parallel with the LFP and LED command signal. The phasic excitatory or inhibitory current was recorded in the same way, but using a whole-cell pipette containing (in mM): K-gluconate (145), NaCl (8), KOH-HEPES (10), EGTA (0.2), Mg-ATP (2) and Na 3 -GTP (0.3); pH 7.2; 290 mOsm. Phasic conductances (Figure 7d) were estimated from Ohm’s law, assuming a driving force of 70 mV.
Off-line analysis was performed in LabVIEW (National Instruments) and R. Time-frequency spectrograms were calculated using a Morlet wavelet transform and are displayed as heat maps. Because the gamma oscillation was non-stationary, its frequency was estimated by calculating the short-term Fourier transform and then averaging the mean instantaneous frequency for successive overlapping intervals. The power of the oscillation was estimated in the same way, by averaging the power at the mean instantaneous frequency.
Spikes were identified using threshold crossing. The instantaneous oscillation phase was estimated by passing a 200 ms segment of the LFP centered on the spike through a Hanning window, and then calculating its phase and frequency using the Extract Single Tone VI in LabVIEW.
To estimate the phase relationship between spikes or membrane currents and the gamma oscillation, we first identified successive troughs of the LFP using the WA Multiscale Peak Detection VI in LabVIEW. Gamma cycles that deviated more than 20% from the modal period were rejected. The membrane current waveform between successive troughs was then expressed as a function of instantaneous phase and averaged over all accepted cycles in the interval. The minimal (least negative) inward current recorded at −70 mV was subtracted to yield the average phasic excitatory current waveform. To estimate the phasic inhibitory current waveform, the minimal outward current was subtracted whilst holding cells at 0 mV.
Data availability
Source data files have been provided for Figures 1-7.
References
-
Layer-specific excitation/inhibition balances during neuronal synchronization in the visual cortexThe Journal of Physiology 596:1639–1657.https://doi.org/10.1113/JP274986
-
Oscillatory dynamics in the Hippocampus support dentate gyrus–CA3 couplingNature Neuroscience 15:763–768.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3081
-
Oscillatory multiplexing of population codes for selective communication in the mammalian brainNature Reviews Neuroscience 15:111–122.https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3668
-
Synaptic mechanisms of synchronized gamma oscillations in inhibitory interneuron networksNature Reviews Neuroscience 8:45–56.https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2044
-
Tremor suppression by rhythmic transcranial current stimulationCurrent Biology 23:436–440.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2013.01.068
-
What does gamma coherence tell us about inter-regional neural communication?Nature Neuroscience 18:484–489.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3952
-
Mechanisms of gamma oscillationsAnnual Review of Neuroscience 35:203–225.https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-neuro-062111-150444
-
Brain oscillations and the importance of waveform shapeTrends in Cognitive Sciences 21:137–149.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2016.12.008
-
A mechanism for cognitive dynamics: neuronal communication through neuronal coherenceTrends in Cognitive Sciences 9:474–480.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2005.08.011
-
Switching neuronal inputs by differential modulations of gamma-band phase-coherenceJournal of Neuroscience 32:16172–16180.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0890-12.2012
-
Dynamic information routing in complex networksNature Communications 7:11061.https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms11061
-
Working memory: the importance of theta and gamma oscillationsCurrent Biology 20:R490–R492.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2010.04.011
-
Optogenetically induced spatiotemporal gamma oscillations and neuronal spiking activity in primate motor cortexJournal of Neurophysiology 113:3574–3587.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00792.2014
-
Hippocampal gamma-frequency oscillations: from interneurones to pyramidal cells, and backThe Journal of Physiology 562:55–63.https://doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.2004.078758
-
Controlling synchronization in an ensemble of globally coupled oscillatorsPhysical Review Letters 92:114102.https://doi.org/10.1103/PhysRevLett.92.114102
-
Correlated neuronal activity and the flow of neural informationNature Reviews Neuroscience 2:539–550.https://doi.org/10.1038/35086012
-
Normal and pathological oscillatory communication in the brainNature Reviews Neuroscience 6:285–296.https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1650
-
How close are we to understanding what (if anything) γ oscillations do in cortical circuits?Journal of Neuroscience 36:10489–10495.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0990-16.2016
-
Cell type-specific tuning of hippocampal interneuron firing during gamma oscillations in vivoJournal of Neuroscience 27:8184–8189.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1685-07.2007
-
Abnormal neural oscillations and synchrony in schizophreniaNature Reviews Neuroscience 11:100–113.https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2774
-
The brainweb: phase synchronization and large-scale integrationNature Reviews Neuroscience 2:229–239.https://doi.org/10.1038/35067550
-
Cortical gamma band synchronization through somatostatin interneuronsNature Neuroscience 20:951–959.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4562
-
Gamma oscillation by synaptic inhibition in a hippocampal interneuronal network modelThe Journal of Neuroscience 16:6402–6413.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-20-06402.1996
Article and author information
Author details
Funding
Wellcome (095580/Z/11/Z)
- Dimitri Michael Kullmann
Wellcome (WT104033AIA)
- Dimitri Michael Kullmann
The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.
Acknowledgements
We are grateful to Francis Carpenter, Flora Lee and Iris Oren for pilot experiments, and to Karl Deisseroth for sharing C1V1. This work was supported by the Wellcome Trust.
Ethics
Animal experimentation: This study was performed in accordance with the Animals (Scientific Procedures) Act, 1986, and were reviewed by the UCL Institute of Neurology Animal Welfare and Ethical Review Body.
Copyright
© 2018, Nicholson et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
-
- 2,027
- views
-
- 350
- downloads
-
- 15
- citations
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.
Download links
Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)
Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)
Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)
Further reading
-
- Neuroscience
Time estimation is an essential prerequisite underlying various cognitive functions. Previous studies identified ‘sequential firing’ and ‘activity ramps’ as the primary neuron activity patterns in the medial frontal cortex (mPFC) that could convey information regarding time. However, the relationship between these patterns and the timing behavior has not been fully understood. In this study, we utilized in vivo calcium imaging of mPFC in rats performing a timing task. We observed cells that showed selective activation at trial start, end, or during the timing interval. By aligning long-term time-lapse datasets, we discovered that sequential patterns of time coding were stable over weeks, while cells coding for trial start or end showed constant dynamism. Furthermore, with a novel behavior design that allowed the animal to determine individual trial interval, we were able to demonstrate that real-time adjustment in the sequence procession speed closely tracked the trial-to-trial interval variations. And errors in the rats’ timing behavior can be primarily attributed to the premature ending of the time sequence. Together, our data suggest that sequential activity maybe a stable neural substrate that represents time under physiological conditions. Furthermore, our results imply the existence of a unique cell type in the mPFC that participates in the time-related sequences. Future characterization of this cell type could provide important insights in the neural mechanism of timing and related cognitive functions.
-
- Neuroscience
Granule cells of the cerebellum make up to 175,000 excitatory synapses on a single Purkinje cell, encoding the wide variety of information from the mossy fibre inputs into the cerebellar cortex. The granule cell axon is made of an ascending portion and a long parallel fibre extending at right angles, an architecture suggesting that synapses formed by the two segments of the axon could encode different information. There are controversial indications that ascending axon (AA) and parallel fibre (PF) synapse properties and modalities of plasticity are different. We tested the hypothesis that AA and PF synapses encode different information, and that the association of these distinct inputs to Purkinje cells might be relevant to the circuit and trigger plasticity, similar to the coincident activation of PF and climbing fibre inputs. Here, by recording synaptic currents in Purkinje cells from either proximal or distal granule cells (mostly AA and PF synapses, respectively), we describe a new form of associative plasticity between these two distinct granule cell inputs. We show for the first time that synchronous AA and PF repetitive train stimulation, with inhibition intact, triggers long-term potentiation (LTP) at AA synapses specifically. Furthermore, the timing of the presentation of the two inputs controls the outcome of plasticity and induction requires NMDAR and mGluR1 activation. The long length of the PFs allows us to preferentially activate the two inputs independently, and despite a lack of morphological reconstruction of the connections, these observations reinforce the suggestion that AA and PF synapses have different coding capabilities and plasticity that is associative, enabling effective association of information transmitted via granule cells.