Mother brain is wired for social moments

  1. Ortal Shimon-Raz  Is a corresponding author
  2. Roy Salomon  Is a corresponding author
  3. Miki Bloch  Is a corresponding author
  4. Gabi Aisenberg Romano  Is a corresponding author
  5. Yaara Yeshurun  Is a corresponding author
  6. Adi Ulmer-Yaniv  Is a corresponding author
  7. Orna Zagoory-Sharon  Is a corresponding author
  8. Ruth Feldman  Is a corresponding author
  1. IDC Herzliya, Israel
  2. Bar Ilan University, Israel
  3. Tel Aviv Sourasky Medical Center, Israel
  4. Tel Aviv University, Israel

Abstract

Reorganization of the maternal brain upon childbirth triggers species-typical maternal social behavior. These brief social moments carry profound effects on the infant's brain and likely have distinct signature in the maternal brain. Utilizing a double-blind, within-subject oxytocin/placebo administration crossover design, mothers' brain was imaged twice using fMRI while observing three naturalistic maternal-infant contexts in the home ecology; 'unavailable', 'unresponsive', and 'social', when mothers engaged in synchronous peek-a-boo play. The social condition elicited greater neural response across the human caregiving network, including amygdala, VTA, hippocampus, insula, ACC, and temporal cortex. Oxytocin impacted neural response primarily to the social condition and attenuated differences between social and non-social stimuli. Greater temporal consistency emerged in the 'social' condition across the two imaging sessions, particularly in insula, amygdala, and TP. Findings describe how mother's brain varies by caregiving experiences and gives salience to moments of social synchrony that support infant social development and brain maturation.

Data availability

We shared raw, subject by subject, anonymized brain data (fMRI); group level data (e.g. unthresholded group maps on MNI template) and raw subject by subject data from the ROI analysis (csv and JASP files). These files are uploaded to our OSF account (https://osf.io/mszqj/?view_only=0daf10c02c984ead8929452edf44e550). We believe that these measures will allow full transparency of the data.

The following data sets were generated

Article and author information

Author details

  1. Ortal Shimon-Raz

    IDC Herzliya, Herzliya, Israel
    For correspondence
    ortalsh@hotmail.com
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  2. Roy Salomon

    Gonda Brain Research Center, Bar Ilan University, Ramat-Gan, Israel
    For correspondence
    royesal@gmail.com
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0002-6688-617X
  3. Miki Bloch

    Department of Psychiatry, Tel Aviv Sourasky Medical Center, Tel Aviv, Israel
    For correspondence
    mikib@tlvmc.gov.il
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  4. Gabi Aisenberg Romano

    Department of Psychiatry, Tel Aviv Sourasky Medical Center, Tel Aviv, Israel
    For correspondence
    gabiar@tlvmc.gov.il
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  5. Yaara Yeshurun

    Psychology, Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv, Israel
    For correspondence
    yaara.yeshurun@gmail.com
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  6. Adi Ulmer-Yaniv

    IDC Herzliya, Herzliya, Israel
    For correspondence
    adi.yaniv@gmail.com
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0001-5865-8305
  7. Orna Zagoory-Sharon

    IDC Herzliya, Herzliya, Israel
    For correspondence
    orna.zagoory@idc.ac.il
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  8. Ruth Feldman

    IDC Herzliya, Herzliya, Israel
    For correspondence
    feldman.ruth@gmail.com
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0001-5048-1381

Funding

Simms/Mann Foundation

  • Ruth Feldman

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Ethics

Human subjects: The study was approved by the Bar-Ilan University's IRB and by the Helsinki committee of the Sourasky medical center, Tel Aviv (Ethical approval no. 0161-14-TLV). All participants signed an informed consent.

Copyright

© 2021, Shimon-Raz et al.

This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License permitting unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.

Metrics

  • 2,759
    views
  • 341
    downloads
  • 19
    citations

Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.

Download links

A two-part list of links to download the article, or parts of the article, in various formats.

Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)

Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)

Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)

  1. Ortal Shimon-Raz
  2. Roy Salomon
  3. Miki Bloch
  4. Gabi Aisenberg Romano
  5. Yaara Yeshurun
  6. Adi Ulmer-Yaniv
  7. Orna Zagoory-Sharon
  8. Ruth Feldman
(2021)
Mother brain is wired for social moments
eLife 10:e59436.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.59436

Share this article

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.59436

Further reading

    1. Neuroscience
    Franziska Auer, Katherine Nardone ... David Schoppik
    Research Article

    Cerebellar dysfunction leads to postural instability. Recent work in freely moving rodents has transformed investigations of cerebellar contributions to posture. However, the combined complexity of terrestrial locomotion and the rodent cerebellum motivate new approaches to perturb cerebellar function in simpler vertebrates. Here, we adapted a validated chemogenetic tool (TRPV1/capsaicin) to describe the role of Purkinje cells — the output neurons of the cerebellar cortex — as larval zebrafish swam freely in depth. We achieved both bidirectional control (activation and ablation) of Purkinje cells while performing quantitative high-throughput assessment of posture and locomotion. Activation modified postural control in the pitch (nose-up/nose-down) axis. Similarly, ablations disrupted pitch-axis posture and fin-body coordination responsible for climbs. Postural disruption was more widespread in older larvae, offering a window into emergent roles for the developing cerebellum in the control of posture. Finally, we found that activity in Purkinje cells could individually and collectively encode tilt direction, a key feature of postural control neurons. Our findings delineate an expected role for the cerebellum in postural control and vestibular sensation in larval zebrafish, establishing the validity of TRPV1/capsaicin-mediated perturbations in a simple, genetically tractable vertebrate. Moreover, by comparing the contributions of Purkinje cell ablations to posture in time, we uncover signatures of emerging cerebellar control of posture across early development. This work takes a major step towards understanding an ancestral role of the cerebellum in regulating postural maturation.

    1. Neuroscience
    Zhujun Shao, Mengya Zhang, Qing Yu
    Research Article

    When holding visual information temporarily in working memory (WM), the neural representation of the memorandum is distributed across various cortical regions, including visual and frontal cortices. However, the role of stimulus representation in visual and frontal cortices during WM has been controversial. Here, we tested the hypothesis that stimulus representation persists in the frontal cortex to facilitate flexible control demands in WM. During functional MRI, participants flexibly switched between simple WM maintenance of visual stimulus or more complex rule-based categorization of maintained stimulus on a trial-by-trial basis. Our results demonstrated enhanced stimulus representation in the frontal cortex that tracked demands for active WM control and enhanced stimulus representation in the visual cortex that tracked demands for precise WM maintenance. This differential frontal stimulus representation traded off with the newly-generated category representation with varying control demands. Simulation using multi-module recurrent neural networks replicated human neural patterns when stimulus information was preserved for network readout. Altogether, these findings help reconcile the long-standing debate in WM research, and provide empirical and computational evidence that flexible stimulus representation in the frontal cortex during WM serves as a potential neural coding scheme to accommodate the ever-changing environment.