Convergence of cortical types and functional motifs in the human mesiotemporal lobe

  1. Casey Paquola  Is a corresponding author
  2. Oualid Benkarim
  3. Jordan DeKraker
  4. Sara Lariviere
  5. Stefan Frässle
  6. Jessica Royer
  7. Shahin Tavakol
  8. Sofie Valk
  9. Andrea Bernasconi
  10. Neda Bernasconi
  11. Ali Khan
  12. Alan C Evans
  13. Adeel Razi
  14. Jonathan Smallwood
  15. Boris Bernhardt  Is a corresponding author
  1. McGill University, Canada
  2. University of Western Ontario, Canada
  3. ETH Zurich, Switzerland
  4. Max Planck Institute Leipzig, Germany
  5. Monash University, Australia
  6. University of York, United Kingdom

Abstract

The mesiotemporal lobe (MTL) is implicated in many cognitive processes, is compromised in numerous brain disorders, and exhibits a gradual cytoarchitectural transition from six-layered parahippocampal isocortex to three-layered hippocampal allocortex. Leveraging an ultra-high-resolution histological reconstruction of a human brain, our study showed that the dominant axis of MTL cytoarchitectural differentiation follows the iso-to-allocortical transition and depth-specific variations in neuronal density. Projecting the histology-derived MTL model to in-vivo functional MRI, we furthermore determined how its cytoarchitecture underpins its intrinsic effective connectivity and association to large-scale networks. Here, the cytoarchitectural gradient was found to underpin intrinsic effective connectivity of the MTL, but patterns differed along the anterior-posterior axis. Moreover, while the iso-to-allocortical gradient parametrically represented the multiple-demand relative to task-negative networks, anterior-posterior gradients represented transmodal versus unimodal networks. Our findings establish that the combination of micro- and macrostructural features allow the MTL to represent dominant motifs of whole-brain functional organization.

Data availability

Code and data related to this specific project are openly available under https://github.com/MICA-MNI/micaopen/tree/master/cortical_confluence, BigBrain related information are openly available under https://bigbrain.loris.ca/main.php. The human connectome project dataset is available under https://db.humanconnectome.org/.

Article and author information

Author details

  1. Casey Paquola

    Neurology and Neurosurgery, McGill University, Montreal, Canada
    For correspondence
    casey.paquola@mcgill.ca
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0002-0190-4103
  2. Oualid Benkarim

    Neurology and Neurosurgery, McGill University, Montreal, Canada
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  3. Jordan DeKraker

    Brain and Mind Institute, University of Western Ontario, London, Canada
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  4. Sara Lariviere

    Neurology and Neurosurgery, McGill University, Montreal, Canada
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0001-5701-1307
  5. Stefan Frässle

    Biomedical, ETH Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0002-8011-2226
  6. Jessica Royer

    Neurology and Neurosurgery, McGill University, Montreal, Canada
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  7. Shahin Tavakol

    Neurology and Neurosurgery, McGill University, Montreal, Canada
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  8. Sofie Valk

    Cognitive Neurogenetics, Max Planck Institute Leipzig, Leipzig, Germany
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  9. Andrea Bernasconi

    Neurology and Neurosurgery, McGill University, Montreal, Canada
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  10. Neda Bernasconi

    Neurology and Neurosurgery, McGill University, Montreal, Canada
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  11. Ali Khan

    Brain and Mind Institute, University of Western Ontario, London, Canada
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  12. Alan C Evans

    Montreal Neurological Institute and Hospital, McGill University, Montreal, Canada
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  13. Adeel Razi

    Monash University, Melbourne, Australia
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0002-0779-9439
  14. Jonathan Smallwood

    Department of Psychology, University of York, York, United Kingdom
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  15. Boris Bernhardt

    Neurology and Neurosurgery, McGill University, Montreal, Canada
    For correspondence
    boris.bernhardt@mcgill.ca
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0002-9536-7862

Funding

Canadian HIV Trials Network, Canadian Institutes of Health Research (CIHR FDN-154298)

  • Boris Bernhardt

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Reviewing Editor

  1. Morgan Barense, University of Toronto, Canada

Ethics

Human subjects: Participants gave informed consent and the study was approved by the local Research Ethics Board of the Montreal Neurological Institute and Hospital (2018-3469).

Version history

  1. Received: July 3, 2020
  2. Accepted: November 3, 2020
  3. Accepted Manuscript published: November 4, 2020 (version 1)
  4. Version of Record published: November 17, 2020 (version 2)

Copyright

© 2020, Paquola et al.

This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License permitting unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.

Metrics

  • 2,379
    views
  • 274
    downloads
  • 52
    citations

Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.

Download links

A two-part list of links to download the article, or parts of the article, in various formats.

Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)

Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)

Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)

  1. Casey Paquola
  2. Oualid Benkarim
  3. Jordan DeKraker
  4. Sara Lariviere
  5. Stefan Frässle
  6. Jessica Royer
  7. Shahin Tavakol
  8. Sofie Valk
  9. Andrea Bernasconi
  10. Neda Bernasconi
  11. Ali Khan
  12. Alan C Evans
  13. Adeel Razi
  14. Jonathan Smallwood
  15. Boris Bernhardt
(2020)
Convergence of cortical types and functional motifs in the human mesiotemporal lobe
eLife 9:e60673.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.60673

Share this article

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.60673

Further reading

    1. Computational and Systems Biology
    2. Neuroscience
    Sara Ibañez, Nilapratim Sengupta ... Christina M Weaver
    Research Article

    Normal aging leads to myelin alterations in the rhesus monkey dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC), which are positively correlated with degree of cognitive impairment. It is hypothesized that remyelination with shorter and thinner myelin sheaths partially compensates for myelin degradation, but computational modeling has not yet explored these two phenomena together systematically. Here, we used a two-pronged modeling approach to determine how age-related myelin changes affect a core cognitive function: spatial working memory. First, we built a multicompartment pyramidal neuron model fit to monkey dlPFC empirical data, with an axon including myelinated segments having paranodes, juxtaparanodes, internodes, and tight junctions. This model was used to quantify conduction velocity (CV) changes and action potential (AP) failures after demyelination and subsequent remyelination. Next, we incorporated the single neuron results into a spiking neural network model of working memory. While complete remyelination nearly recovered axonal transmission and network function to unperturbed levels, our models predict that biologically plausible levels of myelin dystrophy, if uncompensated by other factors, can account for substantial working memory impairment with aging. The present computational study unites empirical data from ultrastructure up to behavior during normal aging, and has broader implications for many demyelinating conditions, such as multiple sclerosis or schizophrenia.

    1. Neuroscience
    Nicholas GW Kennedy, Jessica C Lee ... Nathan M Holmes
    Research Article

    How is new information organized in memory? According to latent state theories, this is determined by the level of surprise, or prediction error, generated by the new information: a small prediction error leads to the updating of existing memory, large prediction error leads to encoding of a new memory. We tested this idea using a protocol in which rats were first conditioned to fear a stimulus paired with shock. The stimulus was then gradually extinguished by progressively reducing the shock intensity until the stimulus was presented alone. Consistent with latent state theories, this gradual extinction protocol (small prediction errors) was better than standard extinction (large prediction errors) in producing long-term suppression of fear responses, and the benefit of gradual extinction was due to updating of the conditioning memory with information about extinction. Thus, prediction error determines how new information is organized in memory, and latent state theories adequately describe the ways in which this occurs.