Quantifying noxious-evoked baseline sensitivity in neonates to optimise analgesic trials

  1. Maria M Cobo
  2. Caroline Hartley
  3. Deniz Gursul
  4. Foteini Andritsou
  5. Marianne van der Vaart
  6. Gabriela Schmidt Mellado
  7. Luke Baxter
  8. Eugene P Duff
  9. Miranda Buckle
  10. Ria Evans Fry
  11. Gabrielle Green
  12. Amy Hoskin
  13. Richard Rogers
  14. Eleri Adams
  15. Fiona Moultrie
  16. Rebeccah Slater  Is a corresponding author
  1. Department of Paediatrics, University of Oxford, United Kingdom
  2. Universidad San Francisco de Quito USFQ, Colegio de Ciencias Biologicas y Ambientales, Ecuador
  3. Wellcome Centre for Integrative Neuroimaging, University of Oxford, United Kingdom
  4. Newborn Care Unit, John Radcliffe Hospital, Oxford University Hospitals NHS Foundation Trust, United Kingdom
  5. Department of Anaesthetics, John Radcliffe Hospital, Oxford University Hospitals NHS Foundation Trust, United Kingdom
5 figures, 1 table and 1 additional file

Figures

Noxious-evoked sensitivity paradigm explained.

Schematic representation of the noxious-evoked sensitivity paradigm components. A brief description of each step is included with additional explanatory notes.

Magnitude of noxious-evoked brain activity in response to experimental noxious stimuli correlates with the response to a clinically required heel lance and can be used as a measure of baseline sensitivity to reduce sample sizes.

(A) The magnitude of noxious-evoked brain activity following mild experimental noxious stimuli and a clinically required heel lance was significantly correlated within-subject (p=0.0025, R2 = 0.77, n = 9, Pearson correlation test, Study 1); grey solid line indicates line of best fit. Dashed lines and their corresponding electroencephalography (EEG) traces indicate three neonates with a range of response magnitudes. The magnitude of the brain activity was quantified using a template of noxious-evoked activity, shown overlaid in red (Hartley et al., 2017). (B-E) In Study 2, we used simulated data to investigate how sample size is altered when the relationship in (A) is considered. (B) For each sample size, 1000 data sets were simulated with a 40% reduction in the response to a clinically required procedure assumed in the Intervention Group. The power (percentage of significant results, p<0.05) to detect a difference between the two groups was calculated for each sample size using a linear regression with (red) and without (blue) accounting for individual differences in baseline sensitivity. (C) The number of neonates required to achieve 95% power with different levels of intervention effect. Simulations were run with increasing numbers of neonates until 95% power was achieved. (D) Percentage reduction in the number of neonates required per group when individual baseline sensitivity is accounted for compared with not accounting for baseline sensitivity (power = 95%). (E) The percentage reduction in the number of neonates required per group with different degrees of correlation (standard deviation of residuals) between the responses to experimental noxious stimuli and clinically required procedure (40% intervention effect, 95% power). The red marker indicates the standard deviation of residuals (SD = 0.37) in (A) (Figure 2—source data 1). The code to produce (B-E) is available from https://gitlab.com/paediatric_neuroimaging/simulating_power_nociceptive_sensitivity.git.

Figure 3 with 1 supplement
Accounting for individual baseline sensitivity in the assessment of efficacy of a gentle touch intervention.

(A) The magnitude of the noxious-evoked brain activity following a mild experimental noxious stimulus compared with the clinically required heel lance for each neonate in the Control Group (n = 15). Solid line indicates line of best fit. (B) (Top) Group average raw electroencephalography (EEG) and (Woody) filtered EEG traces in response to the clinically required heel lance; Control Group (purple) and Intervention Group (neonates received gentle touch at a rate of approximately 3 cm/s for 10 s prior to the heel lance, n = 16) (blue). Dashed lines indicate the point of stimulation; the template of noxious-evoked brain activity is shown overlaid in red. Each neonate’s EEG responses to the experimental noxious stimulus and the heel lance are shown in Figure 3—figure supplement 1. (Bottom) Magnitude of the noxious-evoked brain activity following heel lance in the two groups. Error bars indicate mean ± standard error. (C) Comparison of the stimulus responses for each neonate in the Intervention Group. Gentle touch was not applied prior to the experimental noxious stimuli so that each neonate’s individual baseline sensitivity could be assessed. (D) Confidence intervals of the correlations for the two groups shown overlaid: Control Group (purple), Intervention Group (blue), solid lines indicate line of best fit. The effect of the intervention (gentle touch) is demonstrated by the difference between the two groups’ confidence intervals and is most evident in neonates who have greater baseline sensitivity (i.e. higher responses to the experimental noxious stimulus) (Figure 3—source data 1).

Figure 3—figure supplement 1
Noxious-evoked brain activity in individual neonates in the Control Group and Intervention Group, Study 3.

Average noxious-evoked brain activity to the experimental noxious stimuli and the corresponding brain activity following heel lancing within the 31 neonates in Study 3. The template of noxious-evoked brain activity (Hartley et al., 2017) is overlaid in red and the grey dashed lines indicate the point of stimulation. Neonates in the Intervention Group (electroencephalography [EEG] traces in blue) received gentle brushing before the heel lancing.

Figure 4 with 1 supplement
Effect of gentle touch on reflex responses.

(A) The magnitude of the reflex withdrawal following a mild experimental noxious stimulus (baseline reflex sensitivity) compared with the clinically required heel lance for each neonate in the Control Group (n = 18). Solid line indicates line of best fit. (B) Average electromyography (EMG) traces (top) for neonates in the Control Group (purple) and Intervention Group (blue, n = 15) where neonates were gently brushed at a rate of approximately 3 cm/s for 10 s prior to the heel lance. Dashed lines indicate the point of stimulation. Each neonate’s EMG responses to the experimental noxious stimulus and the heel lance are shown in Figure 4—figure supplement 1. (Bottom) Magnitude of the reflex withdrawal response in the two groups. Error bars indicate mean of the root mean square (RMS) of the reflex withdrawal ± standard error. (C) The magnitude of the reflex withdrawal following a mild experimental noxious stimulus (baseline reflex sensitivity) compared with the clinically required heel lance for each neonate in the Intervention Group (gentle touch) (Figure 4—source data 1).

Figure 4—figure supplement 1
Reflex withdrawal activity in individual neonates in the Control group and Intervention Group, Study 3.

Average reflex withdrawal in response to experimental noxious stimulation and reflex withdrawal evoked by heel lancing in the 31 neonates included in Study 3. Neonates in the Intervention Group (EMG traces in blue) received gentle brushing before the heel lancing.

Figure 5 with 3 supplements
Paracetamol significantly reduces noxious-evoked brain activity following immunisation.

(A) Experimental design of Study 4. Electroencephalography (EEG) was recorded during immunisations in neonates before (Control Group, n = 16) and after the guideline change (Intervention Group, n = 16, received paracetamol 1 hr prior to immunisation). A subset of neonates in the Intervention Group (n = 11 of 16) received experimental noxious stimuli before and approximately 1 hr after paracetamol administration. (B) Average (Woody) filtered EEG following immunisations are displayed (top); for the Control Group in purple and Intervention Group in teal, the template of noxious-evoked brain activity is shown overlaid in red. Dashed lines indicate the point of contact of the needle with the skin. The use of the template of noxious-evoked brain activity to quantify the magnitude of noxious-evoked brain activity from immunisation applied to the thigh was validated: Figure 5—figure supplement 1 and Figure 5—figure supplement 2. Individual neonate’s EEG responses to the immunisation are shown in Figure 5—figure supplement 3. (Bottom) Magnitude of the noxious-evoked brain activity following immunisations in the two groups (Control Group n = 15, Intervention Group n = 14), error bars indicate mean ± standard error (linear mixed effects regression model − without accounting for baseline sensitivity, t = 3.61, **p<0.001). (C) Magnitude of the noxious-evoked brain activity following the experimental noxious stimulus prior to paracetamol administration (baseline sensitivity) compared with the noxious-evoked brain activity to immunisation (which was approximately 1 hr after paracetamol administration) for each neonate in the Intervention Group subset (n = 9, markers in teal). For comparison, the confidence interval of the Control Group correlation in Study 3 (i.e. the correlation between the response to experimental noxious stimuli and a heel lance) is shown in grey. (D) Magnitude of the noxious-evoked brain activity to experimental noxious stimuli prior to paracetamol administration (baseline sensitivity) compared with the noxious-evoked brain activity to experimental noxious stimuli applied approximately 1 hr after paracetamol administration for each neonate in the Intervention Group subset (n = 9, markers in teal). For comparison, the confidence interval of the Control Group correlation in Study 3 (i.e. the correlation between the response to experimental noxious stimuli and a heel lance) is shown in grey (Figure 5—source data 1).

Figure 5—figure supplement 1
Latency of the noxious-evoked brain activity in response to stimulation on the foot, thigh, and hand.

(A) Raw average and Woody filtered electroencephalography (EEG) following experimental noxious stimuli of the foot and during background brain activity. (B) Principal component analysis (PCA) was conducted in the time window 400–700 ms after the stimulus and the second principal component weights were significantly higher to noxious stimulation of the foot compared with in the background brain activity (paired t-test, *p<0.05). (C) The second principal component waveform (black) was highly correlated with the previously described template of noxious-evoked brain activity (red). (D) Raw average and Woody filtered EEG following experimental noxious stimuli of the hand and during background brain activity. (E) PCA was conducted in the time window 200–500 ms after the stimulus and the second principal component weights were significantly higher to noxious stimulation of the hand compared with in the background brain activity (**p<0.001). (F) The second principal component waveform (black) was highly correlated with the previously described template of noxious-evoked brain activity (red). (G) Raw average and Woody filtered EEG following experimental noxious stimuli of the thigh and during background brain activity. (H) PCA was conducted in the time window 300–600 ms after the stimulus and the second principal component weights were significantly higher to noxious stimulation of the thigh compared with in the background brain activity (**p<0.001). (I) The second principal component waveform (black) was highly correlated with the previously described template of noxious-evoked brain activity (red). (J) Schematic representation of the latency of the noxious-evoked brain activity with stimuli applied across different locations. In panels (A,D,G) dashed lines show the timepoint of the stimulation. Pink shading shows the time window of the identified noxious response cluster and grey shading shows the time window where the PCA was performed.

Figure 5—figure supplement 2
Validation of the template for use in immunisation studies.

(A) t-Statistics from the comparison of the noxious-evoked brain activity following immunisations and during background brain activity in 15 neonates from the Control Group in Study 4. Dashed lines indicate the t-statistic threshold for cluster significance, set as the 97.5 percentile of the permuted data. The grey bars indicate time periods outside of the t-statistic threshold and the significant time window identified with the cluster analysis is illustrated by the grey shading area. (B) Principal component analysis was conducted in the time window 300–600 ms after the immunisations and the first principal component weights were significantly higher following immunisations compared with in the background brain activity (*p<0.01). (C) The first principal component waveform (black) was highly correlated with the previously described template of noxious-evoked brain activity (red).

Figure 5—figure supplement 3
Noxious-evoked brain activity following immunisations in individual neonates in the Control Group and Intervention Group, Study 4.

Average noxious-evoked brain activity in individual neonates following immunisations in the 29 neonates in Study 4. The template of noxious-evoked brain activity (Hartley et al., 2017) is overlaid in red and the grey dashed lines indicate the point of stimulation. Neonates in the Intervention Group (electroencephalography [EEG] traces in teal) received paracetamol approximately 1 hr prior to the immunisations.

Tables

Table 1
Participant demographics.

Values given are median (lower quartile, upper quartile) or number (%). * Indicates missing data for one neonate.

Study 1Study 3Study 4
Control groupIntervention groupControl groupIntervention groupTemplate validation
Number of neonates91820151416
Gestational age (GA) at birth (weeks)40.7 (40.3, 41)40 (37.1, 40.7)39.1 (37.1, 40.6)27.6 (25.6, 28.8)27.3 (26.3, 28.3)40.6 (40, 41)
Postmenstrual age (PMA) at time of study (weeks)41 (40.9, 41.7)40.5 (37.6, 40.9)39.5 (37.8, 41.1)38 (37.2, 39.4)37.2 (36.2, 38.1)40.7 (40.1, 41.4)
Postnatal age (PNA) at time of study (days)2 (2, 4)1.5 (1, 3.5)4 (2, 5)64 (59, 90)64 (62, 70.8)2 (1, 3)
Birthweight (g)4140 (3725, 4320)3675 (3021, 3881)3390 (3051, 3908)1040 (705, 1268)880 (708, 1031)3520 (3103, 3786)
Sex
Male4 (44)11 (61)9 (45)9 (60)9 (36)12 (75)
Female5 (56)7 (39)11 (55)6 (40)5 (64)4 (25)
Mode of delivery
NVD (normal vaginal delivery)2 (22)6 (33)7 (35)6 (40)3 (21.4)8 (50)
Assisted vaginal ventouse/forceps6 (67)3 (17)4 (20)0 (0)1 (7.1)4 (25)
Emergency C-section0 (0)6 (33)5 (25)7 (47)7 (50)4 (25)
Elective C-section1 (11)3 (17)4 (20)2 (13)3 (21.4)0 (0)
Apgar score at 1 min9 (8, 10)9 (8, 10)9 (7, 10)5 (4, 6)*7 (3, 8)*9 (9, 10)
Apgar score at 5 min10 (10, 10)10 (10, 10)10 (9, 10)8 (7, 9)*8 (7, 10)*10 (10, 10)
Estimated cumulative prior pain exposure4 (1, 4)*2 (2, 6)5 (3, 8)*31 (28, 451)78 (58, 219)0 (0, 0)

Additional files

Download links

A two-part list of links to download the article, or parts of the article, in various formats.

Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)

Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)

Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)

  1. Maria M Cobo
  2. Caroline Hartley
  3. Deniz Gursul
  4. Foteini Andritsou
  5. Marianne van der Vaart
  6. Gabriela Schmidt Mellado
  7. Luke Baxter
  8. Eugene P Duff
  9. Miranda Buckle
  10. Ria Evans Fry
  11. Gabrielle Green
  12. Amy Hoskin
  13. Richard Rogers
  14. Eleri Adams
  15. Fiona Moultrie
  16. Rebeccah Slater
(2021)
Quantifying noxious-evoked baseline sensitivity in neonates to optimise analgesic trials
eLife 10:e65266.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.65266