Temporally delayed linear modelling (TDLM) measures replay in both animals and humans

  1. Yunzhe Liu  Is a corresponding author
  2. Raymond J Dolan
  3. Cameron Higgins
  4. Hector Penagos
  5. Mark W Woolrich
  6. H Freyja Ólafsdóttir
  7. Caswell Barry
  8. Zeb Kurth-Nelson
  9. Timothy E Behrens
  1. University College London, United Kingdom
  2. University of Oxford, United Kingdom
  3. Massachusetts Institute of Technology, United States
  4. Donders Institute for Brain Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University, Netherlands
  5. DeepMind, United Kingdom

Abstract

There are rich structures in off-task neural activity which are hypothesised to reflect fundamental computations across a broad spectrum of cognitive functions. Here, we develop an analysis toolkit – Temporal Delayed Linear Modelling (TDLM) for analysing such activity. TDLM is a domain-general method for finding neural sequences that respect a pre-specified transition graph. It combines nonlinear classification and linear temporal modelling to test for statistical regularities in sequences of task-related reactivations. TDLM is developed on the non-invasive neuroimaging data and is designed to take care of confounds and maximize sequence detection ability. Notably, as a linear framework, TDLM can be easily extended, without loss of generality, to capture rodent replay in electrophysiology, including in continuous spaces, as well as addressing second-order inference questions, e.g., its temporal and spatial varying pattern. We hope TDLM will advance a deeper understanding of neural computation and promote a richer convergence between animal and human neuroscience.

Data availability

No new data is used or generated in the current paper.Data relevant for current paper is available at https://github.com/YunzheLiu/TDLM. This dataset is from "Ólafsdóttir, H. F., Carpenter, F., & Barry, C. (2016). Coordinated grid and place cell replay during rest. Nature neuroscience, 19(6), 792-794."

Article and author information

Author details

  1. Yunzhe Liu

    Wellcome Centre for Human Neuroimaging, University College London, London, United Kingdom
    For correspondence
    yunzhe.liu.16@ucl.ac.uk
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared.
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0003-0836-9403
  2. Raymond J Dolan

    The Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, University College London, London, United Kingdom
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared.
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0001-9356-761X
  3. Cameron Higgins

    Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford, Oxford, United Kingdom
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared.
  4. Hector Penagos

    Picower Institute for Learning and Memory; Center for Brains, Minds and Machines; Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, United States
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared.
  5. Mark W Woolrich

    Oxford Centre for Human Brain Activity (OHBA), Wellcome Centre for Integrative NeuroImaging, Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford, Oxford, United Kingdom
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared.
  6. H Freyja Ólafsdóttir

    Donders Institute for Brain Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University, Nijmegen, Netherlands
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared.
  7. Caswell Barry

    Cell and Developmental Biology, University College London, London, United Kingdom
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared.
  8. Zeb Kurth-Nelson

    Neuroscience Team, DeepMind, London, United Kingdom
    Competing interests
    Zeb Kurth-Nelson, Zeb Kurth-Nelson is affiliated with DeepMind. The author has no other competing interests to declare..
  9. Timothy E Behrens

    Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, University of Oxford, Oxford, United Kingdom
    Competing interests
    Timothy E Behrens, Senior editor, eLife.
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0003-0048-1177

Funding

Wellcome (098362/Z/12/Z)

  • Raymond J Dolan

Wellcome (104765/Z/14/Z)

  • Timothy E Behrens

Wellcome (219525/Z/19/Z)

  • Timothy E Behrens

James S. McDonnell Foundation (JSMF220020372)

  • Timothy E Behrens

Wellcome (212281/Z/18/Z)

  • Caswell Barry

max planck

  • Yunzhe Liu

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Ethics

Human subjects: The human dataset used in this study was reported in Liu et al 2019. All participants were recruited from the UCL Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience subject pool, had a normal or corrected-to-normal vision, no history of psychiatric or neurological disorders, and had provided written informed consent prior to the start of the experiment, which was approved by the Research Ethics Committee at University College London (UK), under ethics number 9929/002.

Copyright

© 2021, Liu et al.

This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License permitting unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.

Metrics

  • 4,605
    views
  • 855
    downloads
  • 29
    citations

Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.

Download links

A two-part list of links to download the article, or parts of the article, in various formats.

Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)

Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)

Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)

  1. Yunzhe Liu
  2. Raymond J Dolan
  3. Cameron Higgins
  4. Hector Penagos
  5. Mark W Woolrich
  6. H Freyja Ólafsdóttir
  7. Caswell Barry
  8. Zeb Kurth-Nelson
  9. Timothy E Behrens
(2021)
Temporally delayed linear modelling (TDLM) measures replay in both animals and humans
eLife 10:e66917.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.66917

Share this article

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.66917

Further reading

    1. Neuroscience
    Mehrdad Kashefi, Sasha Reschechtko ... J Andrew Pruszynski
    Research Article

    Real-world actions often comprise a series of movements that cannot be entirely planned before initiation. When these actions are executed rapidly, the planning of multiple future movements needs to occur simultaneously with the ongoing action. How the brain solves this task remains unknown. Here, we address this question with a new sequential arm reaching paradigm that manipulates how many future reaches are available for planning while controlling execution of the ongoing reach. We show that participants plan at least two future reaches simultaneously with an ongoing reach. Further, the planning processes of the two future reaches are not independent of one another. Evidence that the planning processes interact is twofold. First, correcting for a visual perturbation of the ongoing reach target is slower when more future reaches are planned. Second, the curvature of the current reach is modified based on the next reach only when their planning processes temporally overlap. These interactions between future planning processes may enable smooth production of sequential actions by linking individual segments of a long sequence at the level of motor planning.

    1. Neuroscience
    Raeed H Chowdhury
    Insight

    When carrying out a sequence of movements, humans can plan several steps in advance to make the movement smooth.