Individual variations in ‘brain age’ relate to early-life factors more than to longitudinal brain change
Abstract
Brain age is a widely used index for quantifying individuals’ brain health as deviation from a normative brain aging trajectory. Higher-than-expected brain age is thought partially to reflect above-average rate of brain aging. Here, we explicitly tested this assumption in two independent large test datasets (UK Biobank [main] and Lifebrain [replication]; longitudinal observations ≈ 2750 and 4200) by assessing the relationship between cross-sectional and longitudinal estimates of brain age. Brain age models were estimated in two different training datasets (n ≈ 38,000 [main] and 1800 individuals [replication]) based on brain structural features. The results showed no association between cross-sectional brain age and the rate of brain change measured longitudinally. Rather, brain age in adulthood was associated with the congenital factors of birth weight and polygenic scores of brain age, assumed to reflect a constant, lifelong influence on brain structure from early life. The results call for nuanced interpretations of cross-sectional indices of the aging brain and question their validity as markers of ongoing within-person changes of the aging brain. Longitudinal imaging data should be preferred whenever the goal is to understand individual change trajectories of brain and cognition in aging.
eLife digest
Scientists who study the brain and aging are keen to find an effective way to measure brain health, which could help identify people at risk for dementia or memory problems. One popular marker is ‘brain age’. This measurement uses a brain scan to estimate a person’s chronological age, then compares the estimated brain age to the person’s actual age to determine whether their brain is aging faster or slower than expected for their age.
However, since brain age relies on one brain scan taken at one point in time, it is not clear whether it really measures brain aging or if it might capture brain differences that have been present throughout the individual’s life. Studies comparing individual brain scans over several years would be necessary to know for sure.
Now, Vidal-Piñeiro et al. show that the brain-age measurement does not reflect faster brain aging. In the experiments, the researchers compared repeated brain scans of thousands of individuals over 40 years of age. The experiments showed that deviations from normative brain age detected in a single scan reflected early life differences more than changes in the brain over time. For example, people with older-looking brains were more likely to have had a low birth weight or to have a combination of genes associated with having an older looking brain.
Vidal-Piñeiro et al. show that brain age mostly reflects a pre-existing brain condition rather than brain aging. The experiments also suggest that genetics and early brain development likely have a strong impact on brain health throughout life. Future studies trying to test or develop brain-aging measurements should use serial measurements to track brain changes over time.
Introduction
The concept of brain age is increasingly used to capture interindividual differences in the structure, function, and neurochemistry of the aging brain (Cole and Franke, 2017). The biological age of the brain is estimated typically by applying machine learning to magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) data to predict chronological age. The difference between predicted brain age and actuackal chronological age (brain age delta) reflects the deviation from the expected norm and is often used to index brain health. Brain age delta has been related to brain, mental, and cognitive health, and proved valuable in predicting outcomes such as mortality (Cole et al., 2018; Cole and Franke, 2017; Elliott et al., 2019). To different degrees, it is assumed that brain age delta reflects past and ongoing neurobiological aging processes (Cole and Franke, 2017; Elliott et al., 2019; Franke and Gaser, 2019; Smith et al., 2020). Hence, it is common to interpret positive brain age deltas as reflecting a steeper rate of brain aging; often dubbed as accelerated aging (here both terms are used interchangeably) (Cole and Franke, 2017; Franke and Gaser, 2019; Smith et al., 2019).
The assumption that brain age delta reflects an ongoing process of faster or slower neurobiological aging implies that there should be a relationship between cross-sectional and longitudinal estimates of brain age. Alternatively, individual deviations from the expected brain age could capture constant interindividual differences in brain structure that remain stable throughout the lifespan, reflecting early genetic and environmental influences (Deary, 2012; Elliott et al., 2019; Walhovd et al., 2016). These perspectives offer fundamentally divergent interpretations of higher brain age (delta) in groups experiencing specific life events, brain disorders, and other medical problems. Here, we tested whether brain age – derived from structural T1-weighted (T1w) morphological features – is related to accelerated brain aging, early-life factors, or a combination of both.
If interindividual variations of brain age reflect variations in rates of ongoing brain aging (Figure 1a), cross-sectional brain age delta should be positively associated with brain decline measured longitudinally. Here, we quantified individual brain change as the annual rate of change of brain age delta (brain age deltalong). In addition, we also assessed brain change with a composite score of structural brain change as obtained using principal component (PC) analysis of change and change in the different raw structural brain features. These analyses were performed in two independent cohorts, both divided into a cross-sectional model generation (training) and a longitudinal, hypothesis testing (test) dataset. If cross-sectional variations in brain age reflect differences in brain structure established early in life, one should observe a relationship between brain age and influences associated with stable, lifelong effects on brain structure. Here, we selected two congenital factors: self-reported birth weight and polygenic scores for brain age (PGS-BA), for which lifelong effects on age-related phenotypes have been shown (Walhovd et al., 2012; Walhovd et al., 2020; Figure 1b). Birth weight reflects normal variation in body (and brain) size as well as prenatal conditions, whereas PGS-BA quantifies genetic liability of having a higher brain age.
Results
Brain age prediction
Chronological age (Figure 1c) was predicted based on regional and global features from structural T1w MRI, including cortical thickness, area, volume, and gray-white matter contrast, as well as subcortical volume and intensity imaging-derived phenotypes (|N| = 365). See a list of the different structural features used in the model in Supplementary files 1 and 2, and Figure 1d for pairwise correlations with age. The model was trained on 38,682 participants (age range = 44.8–82.6 years) with a single MRI from the UK Biobank (Miller et al., 2016) using gradient boosting as implemented in XGBoost (https://xgboost.readthedocs.io) and optimized using 10-fold cross-validation and a randomized hyperparameter search. The trained model (Figure 1e) was then used to predict brain age for an independent test dataset of 1372 participants with two MRIs each (age range = 47.2–80.6 years, mean [SD] follow-up = 2.3 [0.1] years). The predictions – applied to the longitudinal test set – revealed a high correlation between chronological and brain age (r = 0.82) with mean absolute error (MAE) = 3.31 years and root mean squared error (RMSE) = 4.14 years (Figure 1f), comparable to other brain age models using UK Biobank MRI data (Cole, 2020a). We used generalized additive models (GAM) to correct for the brain age bias, that is, the underestimation of brain age in older individuals and vice versa; a regression-to-the-mean bias (Smith et al., 2019). Brain age delta was calculated as the residual from the GAM fit. Brain age delta at baseline and follow-up were strongly correlated (r = 0.81). To establish generalizability, we replicated our results using a different machine learning algorithm – a LASSO-based approach (Cole, 2020a) – and an independent training and test (longitudinal) dataset from the Lifebrain consortium (Walhovd et al., 2018) with up to 11.2 years of follow-up (3292 unique participants, age range = 18.0–94.4 years; technical and biological replication). See Figure 1—figure supplement 1 and Supplementary file 3 for additional demographic information. All the codes used to generate the results are available alongside the article and at https://github.com/LCBC-UiO/VidalPineiro_BrainAge, (Vidal-Piñeiro, 2021; copy archived at swh:1:rev:2044c6ca40e0b8f99c9190c6edfde8ca76b559ac).
Brain age does not strongly relate to the rate of brain aging
First, we tested whether cross-sectional brain age delta was associated with brain age deltalong – that is, annual rate of change in brain age delta – using linear models controlling for age, sex, scanning site, and estimated intracranial volume (eICV). We selected the centercept (brain age delta at mean chronological age), instead of baseline brain age delta, to avoid statistical dependency between indices. Cross-sectional and brain age deltalong were weakly, but negatively, associated in the UK Biobank (β = –0.016 [±0.008] delta/year, t(p) = –2.0 (.04), r2 = 0.002, Figure 2a). Cross-sectional and brain age deltalong were unrelated using a LASSO regression approach (β = –0.003 [±0.006] delta/year, t(p) = –0.5 (.65), r2 = 0.001, Figure 2b), and in the Lifebrain replication sample (β = –0.007 [±0.01] delta/year, t(p) = –0.6 (.53), r2 = 0.001, Figure 2c). Post-hoc equivalence tests showed that positive relationships with β > 0.010 delta/year would be rejected in all three analyses, thus confirming a lack of a meaningful relationship between cross-sectional and longitudinal brain age (Materials and methods and Figure 2—figure supplement 1). UK Biobank (gradient boosting) results remained not significant when brain age delta was derived by time points 1 and 2 as two independent training sets (10-fold cross-validation; uncorrected delta values), thus avoiding potential confounds with age-bias correction (t(p) = 0.3 (.76)). Lifebrain results remained unaffected after including follow-up interval as an additional covariate or restricting the analysis to participants with long follow-up intervals (>4 years; n = 424). The relationship between cross-sectional and brain age deltalong was not significant in both cases (β = –0.008 [±0.01] delta/year, t(p) = –0.7 (.45); β = –0.008 [±0.007] delta/year, t(p) = –1.1 (.26)).
We additionally tested whether cross-sectional and longitudinal brain age delta (brain age deltalong) were associated with a composite measure of longitudinal brain change or with change in any of the structural MRI features. See Materials and methods for details. Cross-sectional brain age delta was unrelated to a principal component of change (β = –0.009 [±0.01] year, t(p) = –0.7 (.46), r2 = 0.001). We did not find a significant relationship when brain age delta was computed with neither a LASSO algorithm nor using the Lifebrain sample (β = –0.02 [±0.01] year, t(p) = −1.7 (0.09), r2 = 0.002; β = 0.007 [±0.006] year, t(p) = 1.3 (0.2), r2 = 0.001). In contrast, brain age deltalong was associated with a principal component of change in the UK Biobank dataset as well as in both replication analyses (all tests p<0.001). See Figure 2—figure supplement 2 for a visual representation. For specific features, cross-sectional brain age delta was significantly related to change – in the expected direction – of features capturing lateral ventricle expansion and white matter hypointensities (p<0.05 Bonferroni-corrected). Brain age deltalong related to change in 45 of the features pertaining to four different modalities. The results were replicated both using the LASSO algorithm and the Lifebrain dataset (Figure 2—figure supplement 3 and Supplementary file 4).
Finally, we estimated the rate of aging effects using a cross-sectional model by estimating the scaling of the size of delta with age as defined in Smith et al., 2019. The scaling (δ) of brain age delta (δ) throughout the datasets’ age range was = 0.14 and 0.09 for the UK Biobank and the Lifebrain datasets. This corresponds to an increase in the spread of brain age delta of |δ| = 0.38 and 0.37 years – when moving from youngest to oldest – in the UK Biobank and the Lifebrain datasets, suggesting that brain age delta only modestly reflects rate of aging effects.
Brain age delta is associated with congenital factors on brain structure
Next, we tested whether birth weight was associated with brain age delta or change in brain age delta. Linear mixed models were used to fit time (from baseline; years), birth weight, and its interaction on brain age delta using age at baseline, sex, scanning site, and eICV as covariates. Birth weight was significantly related to brain age delta (β = –0.70 [±0.30] year/kg, t(p) = −2.3 (0.02), r2 = 0.009, Figure 3a), but not to delta change (β = 0.02 [±0.09] year/kg, t(p) = 0.3 (0.79), Figure 3c). Birth weights were limited to normal variations at full term (from 2.5 to 4.5 kg; n = 770 unique individuals) but see Figure 3—figure supplement 1 for results with varying cutoffs. The results were not affected by excluding individuals being part of multiple births (p=0.02) and were replicated using the LASSO approach (β = –0.79 [±0.29] year/kg, t(p) = −2.8 (0.006), r2 = 0.009, Figure 3b and d).
Finally, we tested whether PGS-BA related to brain age delta and change in brain age delta (n = 1339). PGS-BA was computed using a mixture-normal model based on a genome-wide association study (GWAS) of the brain age delta phenotype in the UK Biobank training dataset. To test the association, linear mixed models were used as above along with the top 10 genetic PCs to account for population structure. PGS-BA was positively associated with brain age delta (β = 0.54 [±0.09] year, t(p) = 9.4 (<0.001), r2 = 0.02, Figure 4a) and negatively associated with brain age delta change (β = –0.06 [±0.03] year, t(p) = −2.4 (0.02), Figure 4c) in the independent test dataset. Likewise, PGS-BA was associated with brain age delta derived from the LASSO algorithm (β = 0.53 [±0.09] year, t(p) = 10.4 (<0.001), r2 = 0.02, Figure 4b) but not to brain age delta change (β = –0.001 [±.02] year, t(p) = 0.0 (1.0), Figure 4d). See Figure 4—figure supplement 1 for GWAS results. The association between PGS-BA and brain age delta remained significant when using as covariates the top 10 genetic components derived from the full UK Biobank sample (p<0.001 in both analyses).
Discussion
Altogether, these findings do not support the claim that individual variation in the cross-sectional brain age metric capture across-subject differences in the ongoing rate of brain aging. Rather, brain age seems to reflect early-life influences on brain structure, and only to a very modest degree reflects actual rate of brain change in middle and old adulthood. A lack of relationship between brain age and rate of brain aging could potentially be explained – although not investigated in the present study – by the effect of circumscribed events such as isolated insults or detrimental lifestyles that occurred in the past, resulting in higher, but not accelerating, brain age. Yet, variations in brain age could equally reflect congenital and early-life differences and show lifelong stability. Cross-sectional brain age studies are ill-suited to disentangle these sources of variation but are often interpreted in line with the former. This assumes that variation in brain age largely results from the accumulation of damage and insults during the lifespan, with similar starting points for everyone. An exception is Elliott et al., 2019, who found that middle-aged individuals with higher brain age already exhibited poorer cognitive function and brain health at age 3 years. This fits a robust corpus of literature showing effects of lifelong, stable influences as indexed by childhood IQ (Karama et al., 2014), genetics (Walhovd et al., 2020), and neonatal characteristics (Walhovd et al., 2016) on brain and cognitive variation in old age.
It has been argued that at a population level brain age captures modest rate of aging effects because brain age delta spreads with increasing age (Smith et al., 2019). Here, we found a similar degree of delta spreading in our brain age metrics. Likewise, our secondary analyses suggested brain age related to change in a few specific neuroimaging features, that is, ventricular expansion and white matter hypointensities, though not to any composite score. Thus, both results are compatible and converge towards brain age as a real but relatively modest metric for capturing ongoing brain change. The largest part of interindividual variation in brain age delta, instead, largely originates before the sample lower bound (⪝ 18 and 45 years for the Lifebrain and UK Biobank datasets). Also, associations of brain age with other bodily markers of aging or with cognitive decline have yielded mixed support for cross-sectional brain age as a marker of individual differences in brain aging (Cole et al., 2018, p. 201; Elliott et al., 2019; Franke and Gaser, 2012). Other multivariate approaches might be better equipped for capturing the dynamics of the aging brain. Using independent component analysis, a recent study found that – compared to a single brain age score – distinct modes of multimodal brain variation better reflect both the genetic make-up and ongoing aging effects, with a subset of modes showing significant spreading of delta with age (Smith et al., 2020). The degree to which brain age reflects ongoing effects of aging likely depends on the specific features, modalities, and algorithms employed, and is constrained by model properties such as prediction accuracy and homoscedasticity. Yet, without longitudinal imaging, one should not interpret brain age as accelerated aging. Our results align with theoretical claims and empirical observations that covariance structures capturing differences between individuals do not necessarily generalize to covariance structures within individuals (Molenaar, 2004; Schmiedek et al., 2020). From a measurement theory perspective, our results suggest that cross-sectional brain age has low validity as an index of brain aging – despite having high reliability (Franke and Gaser, 2012) – as only a small portion of variance is associated with the trait of interest alone (Zuo et al., 2019). Most variance is rather associated with other factors that vary systematically across individuals, some of which are already present at birth.
The results further showed that birth weight, which reflects differences in genetic propensities and prenatal environment (Gielen et al., 2008), explained a modest portion of the variance in brain age. Subtle variations in birth weight are associated with brain structure early in life and present throughout the lifespan (Walhovd et al., 2016). This association should be considered as proof of concept that the metric of brain age reflects the past more than presently ongoing events in the morphological structure of the brain. This was confirmed by the consistent association between PGS-BA and brain age delta but not with brain age delta change. Since PGS-BA was computed based on cross-sectional brain age delta, this relationship may not be surprising, but still suggests a different genetic foundation for longitudinal brain age. These findings link with evidence that brain development is strongly influenced by a genetic architecture that, in interaction with environmental factors, leads to substantial, longlasting effects on brain structure. By contrast, aging mechanisms seem to be more related to limitations of maintenance and repair functions and have a more stochastic nature (Kirkwood, 2005).
Limitations and technical considerations
We used large training datasets to estimate the brain age models and the PGS scores leading to robust PGS-BA and brain age estimates. Self-reported birth weight (Nilsen et al., 2017) and cross-sectional brain age (Franke and Gaser, 2012) are highly reliable measures; thus, our analyses are well-powered to detect small effects (Zuo et al., 2019). The reliability of brain age deltalong is, however, unknown. Strictly speaking, brain age delta is a prediction error from a model that maximizes the prediction of age in cross-sectional data and thus partially also reflects noise. Given that deltalong is estimated as the difference between two deltacross estimates, it will hence have higher noise than the cross-sectional estimates, reducing the power in identifying potential associations between longitudinal and cross-sectional delta. This may be compounded by the relatively short interscan interval in the UK Biobank (≈2 years). However, our sample size (n > 1200) ensures that the tests performed in this study are well-powered to detect small effects, even if deltalong has mediocre reliability (Zuo et al., 2019). Further, replication of our null results in the Lifebrain sample with more observations and longer follow-up times reduces the likelihood of noise as the main factor behind the lack of relationship. Furthermore, previous studies have found that changes in brain age are partly heritable (Brouwer et al., 2021) and relate to, for instance, cardiometabolic risk factors (Beck, 2021), suggesting that it captures biologically relevant signals (i.e., has predictive validity), although with substantially different origins from cross-sectional brain age. Although the reliability of deltalong needs to be formally tested, the null relationship between deltacross and deltalong does not seem to be a result of a low-powered test.
We speculate that our results partially generalize to other normative and residual-based modeling approaches, as well as to developmental samples. There is considerable evidence in the literature that birth weight and genetic risk for neurodegenerative conditions affect brain structure from early life (Raznahan et al., 2012; Walhovd et al., 2020; Walhovd et al., 2016). Brain age models are related to other models such as normative brain charts (Bethlehem, 2021; Dong et al., 2020) – akin to normative anthropometric charts – the main difference being that brain age models predict, rather than control for, age (Marquand et al., 2019). Both types of models produce normative brain scores, which are uncorrelated with age (Butler et al., 2021). Thus, caution is required when interpreting these scores as indices of brain aging without availability of longitudinal data. Developmental samples may, however, reflect slightly stronger relationships between cross-sectional brain age delta and ongoing brain change as brain changes during early-life development typically occur at a faster pace than in middle or later life. Similarly, for specific disease groups such as Alzheimer’s disease patients (Franke and Gaser, 2012), interindividual brain variation in brain age might reflect to a greater extent prevailing loss of brain structure. Moreover, the variance associated with factors other than ongoing development/aging might be more limited in early than later age since influences leading to interindividual variations in brain structure have a shorter span to accumulate. That is, as time from birth increases, chronological age as a marker of individual development is reduced.
Finally, many genetic and environmental factors relate to lifelong stable differences in brain age beyond birth weight and PGS-BA. However, both variables are congenital and show stable associations through the lifespan (Raznahan et al., 2012; Walhovd et al., 2020) without strong evidence that they relate to brain change after adolescence. Thus, birth weight and PGS-BA are paradigmatic for showing how interindividual differences in brain age emerge early in life. The present study does not provide a systematic understanding of these influences but presents a framework for interpreting the impact such measures may exert on age-related phenotypes.
Conclusions
The results call for caution in interpreting brain-derived indices of aging based on cross-sectional MRI data and underscore the need to rely on longitudinal data whenever the goal is to understand the trajectories of brain and cognition in aging.
Materials and methods
Participants and samples
Request a detailed protocolThe main sample was drawn from the UK Biobank neuroimaging branch (https://www.ukbiobank.ac.uk/ Miller et al., 2016). 38,682 individuals had MRI available at a single time point and were used as the training dataset. 1372 individuals had longitudinal data and were used as the test dataset. The present analyses were conducted under data application number 32048. The Lifebrain dataset (Walhovd et al., 2018) included datasets from five different major European Lifespan cohorts: the Center for Lifespan Changes in Brain and Cognition cohort (LCBC, Oslo; Walhovd et al., 2016), the Cambridge Center for Aging and Neuroscience study (Cam-CAN; Shafto et al., 2014; Taylor et al., 2017), the Berlin Study of Aging-II (Base-II; Bertram et al., 2014), the University of Barcelona cohort (UB; Rajaram et al., 2016; Vidal-Piñeiro et al., 2014), and the BETULA project (Umeå; Nilsson et al., 2010). Furthermore, we included data from the Australian Imaging Biomarkers and Lifestyle flagship study of ageing (AIBL; Ellis et al., 2009). In addition to cohort-specific inclusion and exclusion criteria, individuals aged <18 years, or with evidence of mild cognitive impairment, or Alzheimer’s disease were excluded from the analyses. 1792 individuals with only one available scan were used for the Lifebrain training dataset. 1500 individuals with available follow-up of >0.4 years were included in the test dataset. Individuals had between 2 and 8 available scans each. Sample demographics for the UK Biobank and the Lifebrain samples are provided in Supplementary file 3. See also Figure 1c and Figure 1—figure supplement 1 for a visual representation of the age distribution in the UK Biobank and the Lifebrain datasets. UK Biobank (North West Multi-Center Research Ethics Committee [MREC]; see also https://www.ukbiobank.ac.uk/the-ethics-and-governance-council) and the different cohorts of the Lifebrain replication dataset (Supplementary file 5) have ethical approval from the respective regional ethics committees. All participants provided informed consent.
MRI acquisition and preprocessing
Request a detailed protocolSee https://biobank.ctsu.ox.ac.uk/crystal/crystal/docs/brain_mri.pdf for details on the UK Biobank T1w MRI acquisition. UK Biobank and Lifebrain MRI data were acquired with 3 and 10 different scanners, respectively. T1w MRI acquisition parameters for both the Lifebrain and the UK Biobank are summarized in Supplementary file 6.
We used summary regional and global metrics derived from T1w data. For UK Biobank, we used the imaging-derived phenotypes developed centrally by UK Biobank researchers (Miller et al., 2016) and distributed via the data showcase (http://biobank.ctsu.ox.ac.uk/crystal/index.cgi). See preprocessing details in https://biobank.ctsu.ox.ac.uk/crystal/crystal/docs/brain_mri.pdf. This procedure yielded 365 structural MRI features, partitioned in 68 features of cortical thickness, area, and gray-white matter contrast, 66 features of cortical volume, 41 features of subcortical intensity, and 54 features of subcortical volume. See the list of features in Supplementary files 1 and 2. Lifebrain data were processed on the Colossus processing cluster, University of Oslo. Similar to the UK Biobank pipeline, we used the fully automated longitudinal FreeSurfer v.6.0. pipeline (Reuter et al., 2012) for cortical reconstruction and subcortical segmentation of the structural T1w data (http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/fswiki Dale et al., 1999; Fischl et al., 1999; Fischl and Dale, 2000) and used similar atlases for structural segmentation and feature extraction.
Birth weight
Request a detailed protocolWe used birth weight (kg) from the UK Biobank (field #20022). Participants were asked to enter their birth weight at the initial assessment visit, the first repeat assessment visit, or the first imaging visit. In the case of multiple birth weight instances, we used the latest available input. n = 894 participants from the test dataset had available data on birth weight. The main analysis was constrained to normal variations in birth weight between 2.5 and 4.5 kg (n = 770; Walhovd et al., 2012) due to lower reliability of extreme scores and to tentatively remove participants potentially with severe medical complications associated with prematurity.
Genetic preprocessing
Request a detailed protocolDetailed information on genotyping, imputation, and quality control was published by Bycroft et al., 2018. For genetic analyses, we only included participants with both genotypes and MRI scans. Following the recommendations from the UK Biobank website, we excluded individuals with failed genotyping, who had abnormal heterozygosity status, or who withdrew their consents. We also removed participants who were genetically related – up to the third degree – to at least another participant as estimated by the kinship coefficients as implemented in PLINK (Chang et al., 2015). For the GWAS we used 38,163 individuals from the training dataset. Polygenic risk scores were computed using the test dataset consisting of 1339 individuals with longitudinal MRI.
Genome-wide association study (GWAS)
Request a detailed protocolWe performed GWAS analysis on the training dataset and the brain age delta-semi-corrected phenotype using the imputed UK Biobank genotypes. To control for subtle effects of population stratification in the dataset, we computed the top 10 PCs using the PLINK command –pca on a decorrelated set of autosome single-nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs). The set of SNPs (n = 101,797) were generated by using the PLINK command, --maf 0.05, --hwe 1e–6, --indep-pairwise 100 50 0.1. The –glm function from PLINK was used to perform GWAS on about 9 million autosomal SNPs, including age, sex, and the top 10 PCs as covariates. See Manhattan and quantile-quantile (QQ) plots in Figure 4—figure supplement 1. Note that our results corroborated the same association region reported in Jonsson et al., 2019 with a smaller sample.
Polygenic scores (PGS)
Request a detailed protocolThe GWAS results for the training dataset were used to compute PGS (PGS-BA) in the independent test dataset (n = 1339 participants). We used the recently developed method PRS-CS (Ge et al., 2019) to estimate the posterior effect sizes of SNPs that were shown to have high quality in the HapMap data (International HapMap 3 Consortium et al., 2010). Rather than estimating the polygenicity of brain age delta from our data, we assumed a highly polygenic architecture for brain age delta by setting the parameter --phi = 0.01 (Boyle et al., 2017). The remaining parameters of PRS-CS were set to the default values. PGS was based on 654,725 SNPs and was computed on the independent test data using the --score function from PLINK. SNPs were aligned with HapMap 3 SNPs (autosome only as provided by PRC-CS) and posterior effects were estimated. We also computed the population structures PCs’ in the test dataset using the same procedure as in the training dataset.
Statistical analyses
Request a detailed protocolAll statistical analyses were run with R version 3.6.3 https://www.r-project.org/. We used the UK Biobank as the main sample and the Lifebrain cohort for independent replication. The main description refers to the UK Biobank pipeline, though Lifebrain replication followed identical steps unless otherwise stated. For replication across machine learning pipelines, we used a LASSO regression approach for age prediction, adapted from (Cole, 2020b). See more details in Cole, 2020a. The correlation between LASSO-based and Gradient Boosting-based brain age deltas was 0.80.
Brain age prediction
Request a detailed protocolWe used machine learning to estimate each individuals’ brain age based on a set of regional and global features extracted from T1w sequences. We estimated brain age using gradient tree boosting (https://xgboost.readthedocs.io). We used participants with only one MRI scan for the training dataset (n = 36,682) and participants with longitudinal data as test dataset (n = 1372). All variables were scaled prior to any analyses using the training dataset metrics as reference.
The model was optimized in the training set using a 10-fold cross-validation randomized hyperparameter search (50 iterations). The hyperparameters explored were number of estimators [seq(100:600, by = 50)], learning rate (0.01, 0.05, 0.1, 0.15, 0.2), maximum depth [seq(2:8, by = 1)], gamma regularization parameter [seq(0.5:1.5, by = 0.5)], and min child weight [seq(1:4, by = 1)]. The remaining parameters were left to default. The optimal parameters were number of estimators = 500, learning rate = 0.1, maximum depth = 5, gamma = 1, and min child weight = 4 predicting r2 = 0.68 variance in chronological age with MAE = 3.41 and RMSE = 4.29. See visual representation in Figure 1f.
Next, we recomputed the machine learning model using the entire training dataset and the optimal hyperparameters and used it to predict brain age for the test dataset (Figure 1e). These metrics are similar or better than other brain age models using UK Biobank MRI data (Cole, 2020a; de Lange et al., 2019) and the cross-validation diagnostics. We used GAM to correct for the brain age bias estimation (Smith et al., 2019); r = –0.54 for the test dataset. Note that we used GAM fittings as estimated in the training dataset so delta values in the test dataset are not centered to 0. Brain age delta was estimated as the GAM residual. The correlation between brain age delta corrected based on the training vs. the test fit was r > 0.99. Also, GAM-based bias correction led to similar brain age delta estimations to linear and quadratic-based corrections (r > 0.99). The diagnostics for the LASSO-based model were as follows: variance explained (r2) = 0.69/0.69; MAE = 3.36/3.28; RMSE = 4.21/4.04; age bias = –0.56/–0.52 for the training and predicted datasets. See representation of the brain age prediction in Figure 2—figure supplement 2.
Higher level analysis
Relationship between cross-sectional and longitudinal brain age
Request a detailed protocolFor each participant, we computed the mean brain age delta across the two MRI time points and the yearly rate of change (brain age deltalong). We selected mean, instead of baseline brain age delta, to avoid statistical dependency between both indices (Rogosa and Willett, 1985; Wainer, 2000). Brain age deltalong was fitted by mean brain age delta using a linear regression model, which accounted for age, sex, site, and eICV. We used mean eICV across both time points.
Relationship between brain age delta and change in brain features
Request a detailed protocolFor each participant, we computed the yearly rate of change in all the raw neuroimaging features and tested whether change was significantly different from 0 (one-sample t-test, <0.05, Bonferroni-corrected; Figure 2—figure supplement 3, Supplementary file 4). Features with significant change over time were fed into a PC analysis (uncentered). The first component, explaining ≃20% of the variance both in the UK Biobank and the Lifebrain datasets, was selected for further analysis. Although it did not qualitatively affect the results, we removed two and three extreme outliers from the UK Biobank and Lifebrain datasets (score >10). See Supplementary file 4 for component weights. Finally, we tested whether cross-sectional and brain age deltalong predicted brain change as quantified both by the first component analysis and change in each of the raw neuroimaging features (p<0.05, Bonferroni-corrected) using the same models described above.
Spreading of brain age delta with age
Request a detailed protocolFurther, we estimated the degree to which brain age delta reflects rate of aging using a cross-sectional model proposed by Smith et al., 2019, which estimates the scaling of brain age delta through the datasets’ age range. The scaling is estimated by λ in δ = δ0(1 +λY0), where δ is brain age delta, Y0 is a linear mapping of chronological age into the range 0:1, and |δ0| relates to brain age delta distribution in the youngest participants. The spread of brain age delta throughout the datasets’ age range can then be expressed as |δ0|λ (years).
Relationship between brain age PGS and cross-sectional and longitudinal brain age
Request a detailed protocolThis association was tested using linear mixed models with time from baseline (years), PGS-BA, and its interaction on brain age delta. Age at baseline, sex, site, eICV, and the 10 first PCs for population structure were used as covariates. The PCs of population structure were added to minimize false positives associated with any form of relatedness within the sample.
Effects of birth weight on brain age
Request a detailed protocolLinear mixed models were used to fit time, birth weight, and its interaction on brain age delta, using age at baseline, sex, site, and eICV as covariates. We explored the consistency of the results by modifying the birth weight limits in a grid-like fashion [0.5, 2.7, 0.025] and [4.2, 6.5, 0.025] for minimum and maximum birth weight (Figure 3—figure supplement 1). Self-reported birth weight is a reliable estimate of actual birth weight. However, extreme values are either misestimated or reflect profound gestational abnormalities (Nilsen et al., 2017; Tehranifar et al., 2009).
Equivalence tests
Request a detailed protocolPost-hoc equivalence tests were performed to test for the absence of a relationship between cross-sectional and brain age deltalong (Lakens et al., 2018). Specifically, we used inferiority tests to test whether a null hypothesis of an effect at least as large as Δ (in years/delta) could be rejected. We reran the three main models assessing a relationship between cross-sectional and longitudinal brain age delta (UK Biobank trained with boosting gradient, UK Biobank trained with LASSO, and Lifebrain trained with boosting gradient) varying the right-hand-side test (Δ) [–0.02, 0.05, 0.001] (p<0.05, one-tailed; Figure 2—figure supplement 1).
Assumptions were checked for the main statistical tests using plot diagnostics. Variance explained for single terms refers to unique variance (UVE), which is defined as the difference in explained variance between the full model and the model without the term of interest. For linear mixed models, UVE was estimated as implemented in the MuMIn r-package.
Lifebrain-specific steps
Features
Request a detailed protocolThe Lifebrain cohort included |N| = 372 features. It included eight new features compared to the UK Biobank dataset, whereas one feature was excluded (new features: left and right temporal pole area volume and thickness, cerebral white matter volume, cortex volume; excluded feature: ventricle choroid). See age variance explained in each feature in Supplementary files 1 and 2 as estimated with GAMs.
Quality control
Request a detailed protocolPrior to any analysis, we tentatively removed observations for which > 5% of the features fell above or below 5 SD from the sample mean. The application of this arbitrary high threshold led to the removal of 10 observations. We considered these MRI data to be extreme outliers and likely to be artifactual and/or contaminated by important sources of noise. Also, before brain prediction, we tentatively removed variance associated with the different scanners using generalized additive mixed models (GAMM) and controlling for age as a smooth factor and a subject identifier as random intercept. This correction was performed due to differences in age distribution by scanner and lack of across scanner calibration.
Hyperparameter search and model diagnostics
Request a detailed protocolThe optimal parameters for the Lifebrain replication sample were number of estimators = 600, learning rate = 0.05, maximum depth = 4, gamma = 1.5, and min child weight = 1. Using cross-validation, the model predicted r2 = 0.92 of the age variance with MAE = 4.75 and RMSE = 6.31. Brain age was underestimated in older age (bias r = –0.33).
Model prediction
Request a detailed protocolThe age variance explained by brain age was r2 = 0.90 with MAE = 4.68 and RMSE = 6.06. Brain age was underestimated in older age (bias r = –0.25; Figure 1—figure supplement 2).
Higher level analysis
Request a detailed protocolFor each individual, mean brain age delta was considered as the grand mean brain age delta across the different MRI time points. To compute brain age deltalong , we set for each participant a linear regression model with observations equal to the number of time points that fitted brain age delta by time since the initial visit. Slope indexed change in brain age delta/year. The relationship between mean and brain age deltalong was tested using linear mixed models controlling for age, sex, and eICV as fixed effects, and using a site identifier as a random intercept. Likewise, linear mixed models were used to test the relationship between brain age delta and change in brain features. Note that eICV was identical across time points as a result of being estimated through the longitudinal FreeSurfer pipeline. We could not obtain the required information on genetics and birth weight to replicate the analyses supporting the early-life account.
Data and code availability
Request a detailed protocolThe raw data were gathered from the UK Biobank, the Lifebrain cohort, and the AIBL. Raw data requests are specific to each cohort. UK Biobank and AIBL data are available upon application to UK Biobank and at https://aibl.csiro.au upon corresponding approvals. For the Lifebrain cohorts, requests for raw MRI data should be submitted to the corresponding principal investigator. See contact details in Supplementary file 5. MRI data is not openly available as participants did not consent to share publicly their data. Access to data is available upon reasonable requests and transfer agreements. Different sample agreements are required for each dataset.Statistical analyses in this article are available alongside the article and will be available at https://github.com/LCBC-UiO/VidalPineiro_BrainAge. All analyses were performed in R 3.6.3. The scripts were run on the Colossus processing cluster, University of Oslo. UK Biobanks’ data acquisition, MRI preprocessing, and feature generation pipelines are freely available (https://www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/ukbiobank). For the Lifebrain cohorts, the image acquisition details are summarized in Supplementary file 6. MRI preprocessing and feature generation scripts were performed with the freely available FreeSurfer software (https://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/). For bash-sourcing scripts, please contact the corresponding author.
Data availability
The raw data were gathered from the UK Biobank, the Lifebrain cohort, and the AIBL. Raw data requests are specific to each cohort. UK Biobank and AIBL data are available upon application to UK Biobank and at https://aibl.csiro.au upon corresponding approvals. For the Lifebrain cohorts, requests for raw MRI data should be submitted to the corresponding principal investigator. See contact details in Supplementary File 5. Different agreements are required for each dataset. Statistical analyses in this manuscript are available alongside the manuscript and will be made available at https://github.com/LCBC-UiO/VidalPineiro_BrainAge, (copy archived at swh:1:rev:2044c6ca40e0b8f99c9190c6edfde8ca76b559ac). All analyses were performed in R 3.6.3. The scripts were run on the Colossus processing cluster, University of Oslo. UK Biobanks' data acquisition, MRI preprocessing, and feature generation pipelines are freely available (https://www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/ukbiobank). For the Lifebrain cohorts, the image acquisition details are summarized in Supplementary File 6. MRI preprocessing and feature generation scripts were performed with the freely available FreeSurfer software (https://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/).
References
-
Cohort Profile: The Berlin Aging Study II (BASE-II)†Ternational Journal of Epidemiology 43:703–712.https://doi.org/10.1093/ije/dyt018
-
Predicting Age Using Neuroimaging: Innovative Brain Ageing BiomarkersTrends in Neurosciences 40:681–690.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2017.10.001
-
Charting brain growth in tandem with brain templates at school ageScience Bulletin 65:1924–1934.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.scib.2020.07.027
-
Equivalence Testing for Psychological Research: A TutorialAdvances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science 1:259–269.https://doi.org/10.1177/2515245918770963
-
Conceptualizing mental disorders as deviations from normative functioningMolecular Psychiatry 24:1415–1424.https://doi.org/10.1038/s41380-019-0441-1
-
Multimodal population brain imaging in the UK Biobank prospective epidemiological studyNature Neuroscience 19:1523–1536.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4393
-
Validity of Self-Reported Birth Weight: Results from a Norwegian Twin SampleTwin Research and Human Genetics 20:406–413.https://doi.org/10.1017/thg.2017.44
-
Betula: A Prospective Cohort Study on Memory, Health and AgingAging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition 11:134–148.https://doi.org/10.1080/13825580490511026
-
Validity of Self-reported Birth Weight by Adult Women: Sociodemographic Influences and Implications for Life-Course StudiesAmerican Journal of Epidemiology 170:910–917.https://doi.org/10.1093/aje/kwp205
-
SoftwareVidalPineiro_BrainAge, version swh:1:rev:2044c6ca40e0b8f99c9190c6edfde8ca76b559acSoftware Heritage.
-
The centercept: an estimable and meaningful regression parameterPsychological Science 11:434–436.https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00284
-
Harnessing reliability for neuroscience researchNature Human Behaviour 3:768–771.https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-019-0655-x
Article and author information
Author details
Funding
H2020 European Research Council (732592)
- Kristine Beate Walhovd
H2020 European Research Council (283634 725025)
- Anders Fjell
H2020 European Research Council (313440)
- Kristine Beate Walhovd
Norges Forskningsråd
- Anders Fjell
Max Planck Institute for Dynamics of Complex Technical Systems Magdeburg
- Andreas M Brandmaier
María de Maeztu Unit of Excellence (Institute of Neurosciences,University of Barcelona) (MDM-2017-0729)
- Barbara Segura
- Carme Junqué
- David Bartres-Faz
European Research Council (677804)
- Simone Kühn
UK Medical Research Council (G1001354)
- Sana Suri
- Enikő Zsoldos
Charitable Trust (1117747)
- Enikő Zsoldos
- Sana Suri
Alzheimer’s Research UK (441)
- Sana Suri
NIHR Biomedical Research Centre, Oxford
- Sana Suri
Knut and Alice Wallenberg Foundation
- Lars Nyberg
ICREA Academia Award
- David Bartres-Faz
Norges Forskningsråd (324882)
- Didac Vidal-Pineiro
Medical Research Council (SUAG/046 G101400)
- Richard N Henson
The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.
Acknowledgements
BASE-II has been supported by the German Federal Ministry of Education and Research under grant numbers 16SV5537/16SV5837/16SV5538/16SV5536K/01UW0808/01UW0706/01GL1716A/01GL1716B. Part of the computation was performed on the Norwegian high-performance computation resources, sigma2, through the project no. nn9769k. The Wellcome Centre for Integrative Neuroimaging is supported by core funding from award 203139/Z/16/Z from the Wellcome Trust. Data used in the preparation of this article were partially obtained from the AIBL funded by the Commonwealth Scientific and Industrial Research Organisation (CSIRO), which was made available at the ADNI database (http://www.loni.usc.edu/ADNI). UK Biobank is generously supported by its founding funders the Wellcome Trust and UK Medical Research Council, as well as the Department of Health, Scottish Government, the Northwest Regional Development Agency, British Heart Foundation and Cancer Research UK. The organization has over 150 dedicated members of staff, based in multiple locations across the UK.
Ethics
UK Biobank (North West Multi-Center Research Ethics Committee [MREC]; see also https://www.ukbiobank.ac.uk/the-ethics-and-governance-council) and the different cohorts of the Lifebrain replication dataset (see Pseudo-Table below) have ethical approval from the respective regional ethics committees. All participants provided informed consent. LCBC Norwegian Regional Committee for Medical and Health Research Ethic, Regional Ethical Committee of South Norway, BETULA Regional Ethical Vetting Board at Umeå University, BASE-II Ethics committee of the Charité-Universitätsmedizin Berlin Cam-CAN, Cambridgeshire 2 Research Ethics Committee, UB Comisión de Bioética de la Universidad de Barcelona and Hospital Clinic AIBL Institutional ethics committees of Austin Health, StVincent's Health Hollywood Private Hospital and Edith Cowan University.
Copyright
© 2021, Vidal-Pineiro et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
-
- 6,674
- views
-
- 841
- downloads
-
- 108
- citations
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.
Download links
Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)
Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)
Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)
Further reading
-
- Neuroscience
Time estimation is an essential prerequisite underlying various cognitive functions. Previous studies identified ‘sequential firing’ and ‘activity ramps’ as the primary neuron activity patterns in the medial frontal cortex (mPFC) that could convey information regarding time. However, the relationship between these patterns and the timing behavior has not been fully understood. In this study, we utilized in vivo calcium imaging of mPFC in rats performing a timing task. We observed cells that showed selective activation at trial start, end, or during the timing interval. By aligning long-term time-lapse datasets, we discovered that sequential patterns of time coding were stable over weeks, while cells coding for trial start or end showed constant dynamism. Furthermore, with a novel behavior design that allowed the animal to determine individual trial interval, we were able to demonstrate that real-time adjustment in the sequence procession speed closely tracked the trial-to-trial interval variations. And errors in the rats’ timing behavior can be primarily attributed to the premature ending of the time sequence. Together, our data suggest that sequential activity maybe a stable neural substrate that represents time under physiological conditions. Furthermore, our results imply the existence of a unique cell type in the mPFC that participates in the time-related sequences. Future characterization of this cell type could provide important insights in the neural mechanism of timing and related cognitive functions.