Memory recall involves a transient break in excitatory-inhibitory balance

  1. Renée S Koolschijn  Is a corresponding author
  2. Anna Shpektor
  3. William T Clarke
  4. I Betina Ip
  5. David Dupret
  6. Uzay E Emir
  7. Helen C Barron  Is a corresponding author
  1. University of Oxford, United Kingdom
  2. Purdue University, United States

Abstract

The brain has a remarkable capacity to acquire and store memories that can later be selectively recalled. These processes are supported by the hippocampus which is thought to index memory recall by reinstating information stored across distributed neocortical circuits. However, the mechanism that supports this interaction remains unclear. Here, in humans, we show that recall of a visual cue from a paired associate is accompanied by a transient increase in the ratio between glutamate and GABA in visual cortex. Moreover, these excitatory-inhibitory fluctuations are predicted by activity in the hippocampus. These data suggest the hippocampus gates memory recall by indexing information stored across neocortical circuits using a disinhibitory mechanism.

Data availability

The data and code used in this study are available via the MRC BNDU Data Sharing PlatformThe data is available here:https://data.mrc.ox.ac.uk/data-set/fmri-fmrs-inferenceDOI: https://doi.org/10.5287/bodleian:vmJOOm7KDThe code is available here:https://data.mrc.ox.ac.uk/data-set/frms-codeDOI: https://doi.org/10.5287/bodleian:8JwYayQmD

Article and author information

Author details

  1. Renée S Koolschijn

    University of Oxford, Oxford, United Kingdom
    For correspondence
    renee.koolschijn@keble.ox.ac.uk
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0001-9553-4213
  2. Anna Shpektor

    University of Oxford, Oxford, United Kingdom
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  3. William T Clarke

    University of Oxford, Oxford, United Kingdom
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  4. I Betina Ip

    University of Oxford, Oxford, United Kingdom
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0003-3544-0711
  5. David Dupret

    University of Oxford, Oxford, United Kingdom
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  6. Uzay E Emir

    school of health science, Purdue University, West Lafayette, United States
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0001-5376-0431
  7. Helen C Barron

    University of Oxford, Oxford, United Kingdom
    For correspondence
    helen.barron@merton.ox.ac.uk
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.

Funding

Engineering and Physical Sciences Research Council (EP/L016052/1)

  • Renée S Koolschijn

Royal Society Dorothy Hodgkin Research Fellowship

  • I Betina Ip

Wellcome Trust (203836/Z/16/Z)

  • Anna Shpektor

Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council (BB/N0059TX/1)

  • David Dupret

Medical Research Council (MC_UU_12024/3)

  • David Dupret

John Fell Fund, University of Oxford (153/046)

  • Helen C Barron

Wellcome Centre for Integrative Neuroimaging (Seed grant)

  • Helen C Barron

Merton College, University of Oxford (JRF)

  • Helen C Barron

Medical Research Council (MC_UU_12024/3)

  • Helen C Barron

Wellcome Trust (203139/Z/16/Z)

  • Renée S Koolschijn
  • Anna Shpektor
  • William T Clarke
  • I Betina Ip
  • Helen C Barron

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Reviewing Editor

  1. Muireann Irish, University of Sydney, Australia

Ethics

Human subjects: All participants gave informed written consent.All experiments were approved by the University of Oxford ethics committee (reference number R43594/RE001).

Version history

  1. Preprint posted: November 27, 2020 (view preprint)
  2. Received: May 5, 2021
  3. Accepted: September 7, 2021
  4. Accepted Manuscript published: October 8, 2021 (version 1)
  5. Version of Record published: October 14, 2021 (version 2)

Copyright

© 2021, Koolschijn et al.

This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License permitting unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.

Metrics

  • 3,380
    views
  • 446
    downloads
  • 16
    citations

Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.

Download links

A two-part list of links to download the article, or parts of the article, in various formats.

Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)

Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)

Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)

  1. Renée S Koolschijn
  2. Anna Shpektor
  3. William T Clarke
  4. I Betina Ip
  5. David Dupret
  6. Uzay E Emir
  7. Helen C Barron
(2021)
Memory recall involves a transient break in excitatory-inhibitory balance
eLife 10:e70071.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.70071

Share this article

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.70071

Further reading

    1. Cell Biology
    2. Neuroscience
    Mariana I Tsap, Andriy S Yatsenko ... Halyna R Shcherbata
    Research Article Updated

    Mutations in Drosophila Swiss cheese (SWS) gene or its vertebrate orthologue neuropathy target esterase (NTE) lead to progressive neuronal degeneration in flies and humans. Despite its enzymatic function as a phospholipase is well established, the molecular mechanism responsible for maintaining nervous system integrity remains unclear. In this study, we found that NTE/SWS is present in surface glia that forms the blood-brain barrier (BBB) and that NTE/SWS is important to maintain its structure and permeability. Importantly, BBB glia-specific expression of Drosophila NTE/SWS or human NTE in the sws mutant background fully rescues surface glial organization and partially restores BBB integrity, suggesting a conserved function of NTE/SWS. Interestingly, sws mutant glia showed abnormal organization of plasma membrane domains and tight junction rafts accompanied by the accumulation of lipid droplets, lysosomes, and multilamellar bodies. Since the observed cellular phenotypes closely resemble the characteristics described in a group of metabolic disorders known as lysosomal storage diseases (LSDs), our data established a novel connection between NTE/SWS and these conditions. We found that mutants with defective BBB exhibit elevated levels of fatty acids, which are precursors of eicosanoids and are involved in the inflammatory response. Also, as a consequence of a permeable BBB, several innate immunity factors are upregulated in an age-dependent manner, while BBB glia-specific expression of NTE/SWS normalizes inflammatory response. Treatment with anti-inflammatory agents prevents the abnormal architecture of the BBB, suggesting that inflammation contributes to the maintenance of a healthy brain barrier. Considering the link between a malfunctioning BBB and various neurodegenerative diseases, gaining a deeper understanding of the molecular mechanisms causing inflammation due to a defective BBB could help to promote the use of anti-inflammatory therapies for age-related neurodegeneration.

    1. Neuroscience
    Mohsen Sadeghi, Reza Sharif Razavian ... Dagmar Sternad
    Research Article

    Natural behaviors have redundancy, which implies that humans and animals can achieve their goals with different strategies. Given only observations of behavior, is it possible to infer the control objective that the subject is employing? This challenge is particularly acute in animal behavior because we cannot ask or instruct the subject to use a particular strategy. This study presents a three-pronged approach to infer an animal’s control objective from behavior. First, both humans and monkeys performed a virtual balancing task for which different control strategies could be utilized. Under matched experimental conditions, corresponding behaviors were observed in humans and monkeys. Second, a generative model was developed that represented two main control objectives to achieve the task goal. Model simulations were used to identify aspects of behavior that could distinguish which control objective was being used. Third, these behavioral signatures allowed us to infer the control objective used by human subjects who had been instructed to use one control objective or the other. Based on this validation, we could then infer objectives from animal subjects. Being able to positively identify a subject’s control objective from observed behavior can provide a powerful tool to neurophysiologists as they seek the neural mechanisms of sensorimotor coordination.