Bumblebees retrieve only the ordinal ranking of foraging options when comparing memories obtained in distinct settings
Abstract
Are animals’ preferences determined by absolute memories for options (e.g. reward sizes) or by their remembered ranking (better/worse)? The only studies examining this question suggest humans and starlings utilise memories for both absolute and relative information. We show that bumblebees’ learned preferences are based only on memories of ordinal comparisons. A series of experiments showed that after learning to discriminate pairs of different flowers by sucrose concentration, bumblebees preferred flowers (in novel pairings) with (1) higher ranking over equal absolute reward, (2) higher ranking over higher absolute reward, and (3) identical qualitative ranking but different quantitative ranking equally. Bumblebees used absolute information in order to rank different flowers. However, additional experiments revealed that, even when ranking information was absent (i.e. bees learned one flower at a time), memories for absolute information were lost or could no longer be retrieved after at most 1 hr. Our results illuminate a divergent mechanism for bees (compared to starlings and humans) of learned preferences that may have arisen from different adaptations to their natural environment.
Editor's evaluation
This is a very informative and nicely controlled study showing that, when retrieving the value of a food type, bumblebees guide their choices by remembered ranking of feeders instead of focusing on their absolute rewards.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.78525.sa0Introduction
What do animals remember about items out of context? For example, suppose we learn that different options (e.g. coffee shops) result in different reward outcomes (e.g. waiting time and quality), and later we are presented with a choice between two previously encountered options which we have never experienced side-by-side. What types of values do we remember for those options now presented in a novel context? Do our memories of the subjective values for each option contain absolute information (e.g. delay to reward), remembered ranking (how they compared to previous alternatives), or a weighted combination of both?
In typical studies exploring the economic choices of animals including humans, subjects do not have to use distant memories of the options; they are presented with choices where the objective values (e.g. amount, cost, and status) are concurrently visible and can be directly compared. Under such conditions, a wealth of research shows that animals’ choices can be influenced by the presence of additional options (Hunter and Daw, 2021; Spektor et al., 2021). An example of this phenomenon is frequently used in marketing: when given a choice between popcorn options in different sizes, e.g. $3 for small and $7 for large, most people choose the smaller cheaper option, but when a $6 medium option is added, more people choose the large because it now seems like a good deal. Evidence of contextual effects like this on direct assessments has been found across the animal kingdom, e.g. humans and other primates (Berkowitsch et al., 2014; Parrish et al., 2015; Trueblood et al., 2013), bats (Hemingway et al., 2021), birds (Bateson, 2002; Morgan et al., 2012), frogs (Lea and Ryan, 2015), fish (Reding and Cummings, 2017), bees (Shafir et al., 2002), and worms (Iwanir et al., 2019). However, little is known about the type and degree of information (absolute and/or relative) that is encoded in the remembered subjective values of options.
Only more recently have investigations of absolute and relative information traversed into the realm of reinforcement learning, where value must be inferred from memories. Studies on starlings (Pompilio and Kacelnik, 2010) and humans (Bavard et al., 2018; Bavard et al., 2021; Klein et al., 2017) demonstrated that both absolute memories and remembered ranking are combined in particular ways to give rise to these animals’ preferences. So far, however, no other species have been investigated for the roles played by absolute memories and remembered ranking in learned preferences. Here, we examine this in bumblebees (Bombus terrestris), an invertebrate and a key model for examining the economy of decision-making outside of humans (Real, 1996).
Specifically, we adopt an instrumental learning paradigm that combines a contextual training phase and a transfer-test phase (Palminteri and Lebreton, 2021). This paradigm essentially involves two distinct learning contexts (e.g. AB context and CD context; Figure 1), with each context offering two options of contrasting properties (e.g. A>B and C>D in reward sizes). After training, animals’ learned preferences are tested with a novel combination of options (e.g. B vs C). Note that behavioural tests for transitive inference (Guez et al., 2013; Vasconcelos, 2008) involve a similar task design, which includes several training phases and a test phase of new combinations. However, this method provides overlapped relational premises during training (e.g. A>B, B>C, C>D, and D>E) in order to examine if animals can infer the relationship between a pair of options within the chained sequence which had previously not been experienced together (e.g. B vs D). In contrast, our paradigm provides no direct overlapped training between contexts (e.g. A>B and C>D), and therefore, animals cannot infer the relationship between unchained options (e.g. B vs C). Rather, by providing both absolute and relative information during training, our paradigm is used to assess whether, and in what combination, bumblebees retain and utilise absolute and ranking memories.
Results
If bumblebees encode and retrieve memories for absolute values, their preference for a particular option should not depend on the context in which that option was learned (Padoa-Schioppa and Assad, 2008). To test this idea, we conducted experiment 1 with a multi-contextual design where certain flowers had the same quality of reward but different ranking. Bees were first trained (individually in all experiments) on two different pairs of coloured flowers (A and B; C and D; Figure 1A, Figure 1—figure supplement 1, and Figure 1—figure supplement 2). The sucrose concentrations of the different pairs of flowers were chosen according to Weber’s Law (Akre and Johnsen, 2014) to represent the same perceived difference, i.e. A:B=45%:30% had the same contrast of incentives as C:D=30%:20% (Figure 1A). Note that for all experiments, the order of training sessions was counterbalanced to ensure that test preferences were not a result of a recency effect. Meanwhile, the focal colours used in the unrewarded test were counterbalanced during training to account for any colour effects (Materials and methods; Figure 1; Figure 1—figure supplement 3). Following sequential training in both contexts (Figure 1—figure supplement 3 and Figure 1—figure supplement 4), we tested each bee’s preference between flower types B and C, which had the same reward quality (30%) during training. Therefore, if bumblebees’ preferences were driven exclusively by their memory for absolute values for options, they should show no preference between B and C in the test. Any significant preference would suggest a contextual effect due to B and C having different rankings during training (B<A while C>D). In an unrewarding test with B and C flowers, bees significantly preferred C over B (generalised linear model [GLM]: 95% CI = [0.08, 0.38], N=40, and p=4.00e-3; Figure 1B and Figure 1—figure supplement 3). Although these results suggest that bees store memories for the relative ranking of flowers, they still may encode absolute information (Table 1), and therefore, we carried out additional experiments.
Bees may still store and use memories for both absolute and relative information in some combined fashion. To test this, in experiment 2, we set the reward quality of flowers B and C to be different (Figure 1C and Figure 1—figure supplement 3). The sucrose concentration of flowers was A>B >> C>D (45%, 30%, 15%, and 10%). After training in both contexts, bees’ preference between B and C flowers was tested. If bumblebees retain and use memories for both ranking and absolute information (and the value weights for these two different types of information are not extremely unbalanced), bees should either prefer B over C because it is much higher in quality, or this value difference would cancel out the ranking difference (B<A while C>D), and their preference for B and C should be equal. On the other hand, a preference for the much lower reward quality flower C would indicate they have access to only ranking memories. Similar to experiment 1, the result of the unrewarding test showed that bees preferred C (GLM: 95% CI = [0.07, 0.28], N=40, and p=3.30e-3; Figure 1D).
Note that a preference for C over B in experiments 1 and 2 may have resulted from remembered ranking or a distorted absolute memory for flower type B due to the presence of and comparison with A during training. Evidence for the latter would manifest as a difference in the preference for C over B in experiment 2 compared to in experiment 1, whereas remembered ranking would predict a similar preference. There was no significant difference in preference for C across experiments 1 and 2 (GLM: 95% CI = [–0.24, 0.13], N=80, and p=0.55; Figure 1B and D). There is thus no evidence that bumblebees’ choices in novel contexts are based on remembered absolute metrics.
If bumblebees rely purely on remembered ranking, then their memories for options should only be ordinal (Vlaev et al., 2011). That is, they can tell one option is better than another but cannot tell how much better. If so, there should be no indication that bumblebees can compare options in quantitative terms. To test this, in experiment 3, we made the reward contrast ratios within the two contexts very different ([A:B] = [1.5:1]=45%:30% and [C:D] = [6:1]=30%:5%; A>B = C>>D; Figure 1E). Here, testing bees’ preference between A and C flowers can answer our question. If bees encode memories for relative differences in quantitative terms, C should be preferred over A, as C had a higher relative value than A. Alternatively, if bees only remember the ordinal rank of options, preference for A and C should be the same because they have the same rank within their training contexts. In the unrewarding test, bees chose A and C equally (GLM: 95% CI = [–0.16, 0.12], N=40, and p=0.79; Figure 1F), suggesting bumblebees store and recall only memories for the qualitative difference between options. Furthermore, bumblebees’ lack of preference for the equally ranked A and C flowers, despite their difference in sugar concentration, demonstrates absolute memories were not remembered (Table 1) and, in combination with experiments 1 and 2, suggests that bumblebees only encode and recall remembered ranking for use in novel contexts.
It may be that bumblebees can store and recall absolute information, but any ranking information available overshadows absolute memory. To test this, in experiment 4, we trained bees so that ranking information was absent. Bees experienced a temporally separated training procedure (Materials and methods; Figure 2A) whereby they first foraged on one flower with either 45% or 30% sugar solution, and 1 hr later they were trained on a different flower with either 30% or 45%, respectively. In the subsequent (1 hr later) unrewarded test, bees’ preferences were assessed. Because the two flower types were separated physically and temporally, bees had no ranking information available to putatively overshadow absolute information. Therefore, any preferences formed would be a result of remembered absolute information. However, bees had no significant preferences for either option (GLM: 95% CI = [–0.15, 0.22], N=20, and p=0.69; Figure 2B), suggesting that bees’ lack of absolute memory in the tests of experiments 1–3 was unlikely to be a result of absolute memories being less salient and overshadowed by ranking memories.
In order to discriminate any two sequentially visited flowers and indeed to rank them, bumblebees must utilise absolute information for at least a brief amount of time. During training in experiments 1–3, bumblebees could see flowers of both types simultaneously. Given the results of experiment 4, we therefore asked whether bumblebees could utilise absolute information long enough to rank flowers that were temporally close together but visually separated. To do this, in experiment 5, we trained bees on one flower for each bout, i.e. each visit to the arena (Materials and methods; Figure 2C). This way, both flower types were never in a bee’s visual field at the same time. During the unrewarded test, bees preferred the option that had offered the higher sugar concentration in training (GLM: 95% CI = [0.23, 0.87], N=10, and p=8.29e-3; Figure 2D), suggesting that absolute information about a flower learned in visual isolation can be retained for at least a couple of minutes but, as experiment 4 highlights, is eventually lost or not able to be recalled later. Experiment 4 and 5 thus collectively indicate that bumblebees can use absolute information to compare options only in their short-term memories (range of minutes) but not in their mid-term memories (range of hours; Menzel, 2001).
Our results highlight the potential disadvantage of using only remembered ranking, i.e. preferring to search for a less rewarding flower. How might our results speak to bumblebees’ behaviour in more ‘natural’ settings? In the wild, bumblebees often forage in situations whereby when drinking from one flower type they cannot see other flower types in their field of view, i.e. on the other side of a bush or many meters away. Yet, the distance between any two flowers within a bumblebee’s foraging range can be traversed within minutes (Osborne et al., 1999), i.e. within the range of bumblebees’ short-term memories (Menzel, 2001). Therefore, we would expect that bumblebees in the wild would not be susceptible to learning preferences for a less rewarding flower despite being unable to retain absolute information for very long. In an attempt to formally test this idea, in experiment 6, we trained bees in a multi-contextual semi-realistic foraging situation. Here, in a divided arena, bumblebees learned to forage from flowers of four different colours (Figure 2E and Figure 1—figure supplement 3) but could only see flowers of two colours at any one time, i.e. could only see A and B (45% and 30%) flowers from one side of the arena and could only see C and D (30% and 20%) flowers from the other side (Figure 2E). In the unrewarding test with all four options available, bees preferred option A (GLM: 95% CI = [0.94, 1.68], N=20, and p=1.21e-6; Figure 2F), indicating that the short retention/utilisation of absolute memories that bumblebees showed in our lab-created situations are likely not to be disadvantageous to bumblebees in the wild, given their natural behaviour and ecological niche.
Discussion
Our results suggest that bumblebees are only able to make use of ordinal ranking memories to guide foraging choices outside their original learning contexts. Although absolute information is essential for any animal to compare any two options initially, our findings show that only ordinal ranking can be recalled by bumblebees later in new contexts. Our results suggest that temporal adjacency is necessary for absolute information of different options to be compared by bees. That is, once bees experience one option, in order to utilise the absolute information, they must experience a second option within several minutes. Both our proof-of-concept experiment (experiment 6) and other previous multi-choice semi-realistic foraging tasks (e.g. Greggers and Menzel, 1993) show that with short inter-flower visits, bees can utilise absolute information in order to compare and rank flowers.
Another potential mechanism worth discussing which could potentially explain bumblebees’ preferences is state-dependent valuation learning (SDVL; for a review see McNamara et al., 2012; Table 1). If bees’ choices were a result of SDVL instead of remembered ordinal ranking, then their preferences should be quantitative, i.e. larger values should be assigned with larger differences between internal state and flower experience. However, the results of experiment 3 speak against this interpretation because bees were indifferent between equally ranked A and C even though they were experienced in a richer and poorer environment, respectively.
Why would bumblebees have evolved to use only memories for ordinal comparisons while humans and starlings evolved to retain and recall both absolute and ranking memories? Breadth of diet has been suggested to play a role in the evolution of cognition (Hemingway et al., 2017; MacLean et al., 2014; Simons and Tibbetts, 2019). Humans and starlings forage on a range of different foods, whereas adult bumblebees feed almost exclusively on nectar and pollen from flowers. Perhaps a varied diet, whereby one might need a common currency across vastly different food types (Chib et al., 2009; Levy and Glimcher, 2012) may have forced some animals to retain and use absolute memories, whereas a limited diet and limited dimensionality of reward might have favoured memories for ordinal relationships alone.
There is no reason yet to suggest a bee’s brain lacks the neural substrates to retain and recall absolute memories for options for later use in novel contexts. In fact, recent electrophysiological recordings on the gustatory neurons of bumblebees demonstrated that the spiking rates of these peripheral sensory neurons increased as a function of sucrose concentration (Miriyala et al., 2018). These gustatory neuronal signals could theoretically be used by higher centres of an animal’s brain to encode an option’s utility as a cardinal (absolute) value (Schultz, 2015). However, our results suggest that bumblebees do not access or utilise any potentially stored absolute memories when it would be of benefit in novel contexts. Given our results, it would be intriguing to determine the differences in the underlying neural mechanisms responsible for the utilisation of absolute properties and ordinal ranking memories. Whatever the ultimate and proximate causes of bumblebees’ ordinal-only memories for options’ values, our findings demonstrate a fundamental difference in the mechanisms underlying learned preferences between those of bumblebees and those of starlings and humans.
Materials and methods
Animals and setup
Request a detailed protocolBumblebee (Bombus terrestris) colonies were obtained from the Chinese branch of the Biobest Group (Biobest Belgium N.V., Westerlo, Belgium) and housed in wooden nest boxes (28 cm × 16 cm × 11 cm). Although there are no current requirements regarding insect care and use in research, experimental design and procedures were guided by the 3Rs principles (replacement, reduction, and refinement; Russell and Burch, 1959). The behavioural tests were non-invasive, and the types of manipulations used (sucrose and water) are all experienced by bumblebees during their natural foraging life in the wild. The bumblebees were cared for on a daily basis by trained and competent staff, which included routine monitoring of welfare and provision of correct and adequate food during the experimental period. A foraging arena (40 cm × 59 cm × 41 cm; Figure 1—figure supplement 1) was connected to the nest boxes via an acrylic tunnel with sliding doors, allowing experimenters to control bees’ access to the arena. Individual bees were marked with number tags (Opalithplättchen, Warnholz & Bienenvoigt, Ellerau, Germany), which were super-glued to the bees’ thorax. To ensure the sucrose solution concentration used in experiments was motivating, colonies were fed outside of experiments with 5–15% sucrose solution (w/w). They were also provided with ~3 g pollen every day. Illumination was provided by daylight fluorescent tubes (MASTER TL-D 90 DeLuxe 36 W/965, Philips, Eindhoven, the Netherlands) and near-UV fluorescent tubes (TL-D 36 W BLB, Philips) with high-frequency electronic ballasts (EB-Ci 1–2 36 W/1–4 18 W, 42–60 kHz, Philips) to generate a flicker frequency beyond the bumblebee’s flicker-fusion frequency. Coloured acrylic squares (25 mm × 5 mm × 5 mm) set on top of opaque glass cylinders as (artificial) flowers were placed in the arena with a different random spatial arrangement each trial. The spectral reflectance (Figure 1—figure supplement 2A) of all flower colours used in experiments was measured with a wavelength range of 300–700 nm and with 1 nm increments, using a spectrophotometer (Ocean Optics USB 2000+; Shanghai, China) and a deuterium/halogen light source. The perceptual positions of the colours in the bee colour hexagon space (Figure 1—figure supplement 2B) were calculated using the spectral reflectance measurements and the published Bombus terrestris spectral sensitivity functions of their three photoreceptors (Chittka, 1992; Skorupski et al., 2007). The minimal perceptual distance of all the colour combinations used in this study was above 0.14 hexagon units, which can be well differentiated by bumblebees (Dyer and Chittka, 2004). Furthermore, neurophysiological (Miriyala et al., 2018) and behavioural evidence (Whitney et al., 2008) verify that the sugar concentration differences used in this study can be readily discriminated by bumblebees.
Experimental protocol
All bees were individually pre-trained on eight transparent artificial flowers and were allowed to collect a full crop of sucrose solution from those flowers. Once a bee had successfully foraged for at least three consecutive bouts, she was moved to the training phase of an experiment.
Experiments 1, 2, and 3
Request a detailed protocolBees (N=40 for each experiment) were trained to forage from eight flowers, four of each of two colours (Figure 1—figure supplement 1 and Figure 1—figure supplement 3). In experiment 1, one group of bees (n=10) learned (individually) that green flowers contained 45% sucrose solution and that yellow flowers contained 30% sucrose solution. Once training on these two flower types was complete (more detail below), this group of bees learned (individually) that orange flowers contained 30% sucrose solution and that blue flowers contained 20% sucrose solution. Training for bees in experiments 2 and 3 was the same except the sucrose concentrations were different. Training sequences and colour combinations were equally counterbalanced across bees in each experiment (Figure 1—figure supplement 3). Flower colours and sequences for all experiments are listed in Figure 1—figure supplement 3. Initially during training, all bees would land and drink from both flowers. As bees learned the colour-reward contingency, they began to land on the less rewarded flowers but did not drink. Once this behaviour was observed, during subsequent bouts, we removed and replaced one higher-ranking flower with one lower-ranking flower. By doing so, and because none of the flowers were refilled during training bouts, bees would again visit the lower-ranking flowers and drink the sugar water droplet. This method helped us to ensure that bees experienced drinking sucrose solution from both options an equal number of times. Training was completed when a bee collected 50 of the 20 μl aliquots of sugar reward from flowers of both colours. Note that we verified, in our setup, that bees could learn to discriminate between flowers of different colours and sucrose concentrations (Figure 1—figure supplement 4). After training, each bee was individually tested with two types of flowers (four of each type) offering 20 μl droplets of unrewarding water, and all flower visits were recorded for 2 min. Note that in all tests, the two flowers types presented had not been experienced previously together in the same arena by the bees.
Experiment 4
Request a detailed protocolBees (N=20) were trained in the same way as the other experiments except that during training only one flower type was in the arena at any one time, i.e. either all of the flowers offered 45% sucrose solution or all flowers offered 30% sucrose solution (Figure 1—figure supplement 3; Figure 2A). Also, the inter-session interval for experiment 4 was 1 hr. Each session ended when the bee completed 50 flower visits. After training and after another 1 hr interval, bees underwent an unrewarded test with the two flower types they had been trained on, presented simultaneously in the arena. Both the colour/sucrose concentration contingency and the order of training sessions (which type of flower was trained last) were counterbalanced across bees (Figure 1—figure supplement 3; Figure 2B).
Experiment 5
Request a detailed protocolBees (n=10) were trained similarly to experiment 4 in which only one flower type was in the arena at any one time (Figure 1—figure supplement 3; Figure 2C). However, instead of two long sessions, bees experienced each flower in alternating bouts (visits to the arena to fill their crop). Once bees experienced 50 flower visits on each flower type, training ended and the bees underwent an unrewarded preference test with both flower types in the arena (Figure 1—figure supplement 3; Figure 2D).
Experiment 6
Request a detailed protocolBees (N=20) were trained and tested similarly to experiments 1–3 except that the two sets of flowers were in the arena at the same time and accessible to the bee during each bout. However, flowers were also separated by an opaque wall to prevent the bees from seeing both sets simultaneously (Figure 1—figure supplement 3; Figure 2E). The wall extended to cover most but not the full length of the arena, and there was space at the entrance and the far end of the arena so that bees could freely choose to visit flowers on either side of the wall. No matter where the bee was in the arena, she could not see both pairs of flowers at the same time. In addition, flowers were refilled after the bees emptied one and began drinking on another. Each group (n=10) was trained with either the yellow or green flower as the high concentration flower. The training was deemed complete when either a bee performed 200 landings or when a bee only landed exclusively on any one option for three consecutive bouts (note that bees never landed exclusively on the lower rewarding option for three bouts consecutively). After training, the bees were individually tested with all four flowers (two of each type) presented in the same arena with the opaque barrier removed (Figure 1—figure supplement 3; Figure 2F).
Statistical analyses
Request a detailed protocolR v.3.6.1 was used to perform all GLMs with quasibinomial distribution and logit link function. For all models, the response variable was the proportion of choices for the flower that had been associated with high concentration sugar water. Fixed factors for models for results of experiments were (i) colony of each bee, (ii) flower colour, and (iii) training sequence (whether the higher concentration sugar reward was used during the first or second training session). Significance of fixed effects was tested using likelihood ratio tests, and none were found to have a significant effect on bees’ preferences during the tests in any experiment. When comparing preference for flower C across experiments 1 and 2, experiment was set as a fixed factor.
Data availability
All data generated or analysed during this study are available on the Dryad Digital Repository: https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.rr4xgxdb9.
-
Dryad Digital RepositoryBumblebees retrieve only the ordinal ranking of foraging options when comparing memories obtained in distinct settings.https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.rr4xgxdb9
References
-
Psychophysics and the evolution of behaviorTrends in Ecology & Evolution 29:291–300.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tree.2014.03.007
-
Context-dependent foraging choices in risk-sensitive starlingsAnimal Behaviour 64:251–260.https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.2002.3059
-
Rigorously testing multialternative decision field theory against random utility modelsJournal of Experimental Psychology. General 143:1331–1348.https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035159
-
Evidence for a common representation of decision values for dissimilar goods in human ventromedial prefrontal cortexThe Journal of Neuroscience 29:12315–12320.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2575-09.2009
-
The colour hexagon: a chromaticity diagram based on photoreceptor excitations as a generalized representation of colour opponencyJournal of Comparative Physiology A 170:533–543.https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00199331
-
Biological significance of distinguishing between similar colours in spectrally variable illumination: bumblebees (Bombus terrestris) as a case studyJournal of Comparative Physiology. A, Neuroethology, Sensory, Neural, and Behavioral Physiology 190:105–114.https://doi.org/10.1007/s00359-003-0475-2
-
Memory dynamics and foraging strategies of honeybeesBehavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 32:17–29.https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00172219
-
Transitive or not: A critical appraisal of transitive inference in animalsEthology : Formerly Zeitschrift Fur Tierpsychologie 119:703–726.https://doi.org/10.1111/eth.12124
-
Rationality in decision-making in the fringe-lipped bat, trachops cirrhosusBehavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 71:94.https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-017-2321-5
-
Context-dependent preferences in wild fruit batsAnimal Behaviour 179:65–72.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2021.06.016
-
Context-sensitive valuation and learningCurrent Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 41:122–127.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2021.05.001
-
The root of all value: a neural common currency for choiceCurrent Opinion in Neurobiology 22:1027–1038.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2012.06.001
-
The ecological rationality of state-dependent valuationPsychological Review 119:114–119.https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025958
-
Searching for the memory trace in a mini-brain, the honeybeeLearning & Memory 8:53–62.https://doi.org/10.1101/lm.38801
-
Burst firing in bee gustatory neurons prevents adaptationCurrent Biology 28:1585–1594.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2018.03.070
-
Context-dependent decisions among options varying in a single dimensionBehavioural Processes 89:115–120.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2011.08.017
-
A landscape-scale study of bumble bee foraging range and constancy, using harmonic radarJournal of Applied Ecology 36:519–533.https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1365-2664.1999.00428.x
-
Context-dependent outcome encoding in human reinforcement learningCurrent Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 41:144–151.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2021.06.006
-
Rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) exhibit the decoy effect in a perceptual discrimination taskAttention, Perception & Psychophysics 77:1715–1725.https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-015-0885-6
-
Paradox, performance, and the architecture of decision-making in animalsAmerican Zoologist 36:518–529.https://doi.org/10.1093/icb/36.4.518
-
Neuronal reward and decision signals: from theories to dataPhysiological Reviews 95:853–951.https://doi.org/10.1152/physrev.00023.2014
-
Context-dependent violations of rational choice in honeybees (Apis mellifera) and gray jays (Perisoreus canadensis)Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 51:180–187.https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-001-0420-8
-
Insects as models for studying the evolution of animal cognitionCurrent Opinion in Insect Science 34:117–122.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cois.2019.05.009
-
Photoreceptor spectral sensitivity in island and mainland populations of the bumblebee, Bombus terrestrisJournal of Comparative Physiology. A, Neuroethology, Sensory, Neural, and Behavioral Physiology 193:485–494.https://doi.org/10.1007/s00359-006-0206-6
-
The elusiveness of context effects in decision makingTrends in Cognitive Sciences 25:843–854.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2021.07.011
-
Not just for consumers: context effects are fundamental to decision makingPsychological Science 24:901–908.https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612464241
-
Transitive inference in non-human animals: an empirical and theoretical analysisBehavioural Processes 78:313–334.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2008.02.017
-
Does the brain calculate value?Trends in Cognitive Sciences 15:546–554.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2011.09.008
Article and author information
Author details
Funding
National Natural Science Foundation of China (31700988)
- Fei Peng
National Natural Science Foundation of China (31970994)
- Fei Peng
Key-Area Research and Development Program of Guangdong Province, China (2018B030340001)
- Fei Peng
Templeton World Charity Foundation (TWCF-2020-0539)
- Andrew B Barron
China Scholarship Council (202008440515)
- Yonghe Zhou
The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.
Copyright
© 2022, Solvi, Zhou et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
-
- 1,521
- views
-
- 264
- downloads
-
- 6
- citations
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.
Download links
Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)
Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)
Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)
Further reading
-
- Ecology
For the first time in any animal, we show that nocturnal bull ants use the exceedingly dim polarisation pattern produced by the moon for overnight navigation. The sun or moon can provide directional information via their position; however, they can often be obstructed by clouds, canopy, or the horizon. Despite being hidden, these bodies can still provide compass information through the polarised light pattern they produce/reflect. Sunlight produces polarised light patterns across the overhead sky as it enters the atmosphere, and solar polarised light is a well-known compass cue for navigating animals. Moonlight produces an analogous pattern, albeit a million times dimmer than sunlight. Here, we show evidence that polarised moonlight forms part of the celestial compass of navigating nocturnal ants. Nocturnal bull ants leave their nest at twilight and rely heavily on the overhead solar polarisation pattern to navigate. Yet many foragers return home overnight when the sun cannot guide them. We demonstrate that these bull ants use polarised moonlight to navigate home during the night, by rotating the overhead polarisation pattern above homing ants, who alter their headings in response. Furthermore, these ants can detect this cue throughout the lunar month, even under crescent moons, when polarised light levels are at their lowest. Finally, we show the long-term incorporation of this moonlight pattern into the ants’ path integration system throughout the night for homing, as polarised sunlight is incorporated throughout the day.
-
- Ecology
Climatic warming can shift community composition driven by the colonization-extinction dynamics of species with different thermal preferences; but simultaneously, habitat fragmentation can mediate species’ responses to warming. As this potential interactive effect has proven difficult to test empirically, we collected data on birds over 10 years of climate warming in a reservoir subtropical island system that was formed 65 years ago. We investigated how the mechanisms underlying climate-driven directional change in community composition were mediated by habitat fragmentation. We found thermophilization driven by increasing warm-adapted species and decreasing cold-adapted species in terms of trends in colonization rate, extinction rate, occupancy rate and population size. Critically, colonization rates of warm-adapted species increased faster temporally on smaller or less isolated islands; cold-adapted species generally were lost more quickly temporally on closer islands. This provides support for dispersal limitation and microclimate buffering as primary proxies by which habitat fragmentation mediates species range shift. Overall, this study advances our understanding of biodiversity responses to interacting global change drivers.