Spatiotemporal organization of human sensorimotor beta burst activity

  1. Catharina Zich  Is a corresponding author
  2. Andrew J Quinn
  3. James J Bonaiuto
  4. George O'Neill
  5. Lydia C Mardell
  6. Nick S Ward
  7. Sven Bestmann
  1. University College London, United Kingdom
  2. University of Oxford, United Kingdom
  3. Institut des Sciences Cognitives Marc Jeannerod, France

Abstract

Beta oscillations in human sensorimotor cortex are hallmark signatures of healthy and pathological movement. In single trials, beta oscillations include bursts of intermittent, transient periods of high-power activity. These burst events have been linked to a range of sensory and motor processes, but their precise spatial, spectral, and temporal structure remains unclear. Specifically, a role for beta burst activity in information coding and communication suggests spatiotemporal patterns, or travelling wave activity, along specific anatomical gradients. We here show in human magnetoencephalography recordings that burst activity in sensorimotor cortex occurs in planar spatiotemporal wave-like patterns that dominate along two axes either parallel or perpendicular to the central sulcus. Moreover, we find that the two propagation directions are characterised by distinct anatomical and physiological features. Finally, our results suggest that sensorimotor beta bursts occurring before and after a movement can be distinguished by their anatomical, spectral and spatiotemporal characteristics, indicating distinct functional roles.

Data availability

Data are available via the Open Science Framework (OSF) at https://osf. io/eu6nx. Data are also archived at the Open MEG Archive (OMEGA; Nisoetal.,2016) and may be accessed via http://dx .doi.org/10.23686/ 0015896 (Niso et al.,2018) after registration at https:// www.mcgill.ca/bic/resources/omega.

Article and author information

Author details

  1. Catharina Zich

    Department of Clinical and Movement Neuroscience, University College London, London, United Kingdom
    For correspondence
    catharina.zich@ndcn.ox.ac.uk
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0002-0705-9297
  2. Andrew J Quinn

    Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford, Oxford, United Kingdom
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0003-2267-9897
  3. James J Bonaiuto

    Institut des Sciences Cognitives Marc Jeannerod, Bron, France
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  4. George O'Neill

    Department of Imaging Neuroscience, University College London, London, United Kingdom
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  5. Lydia C Mardell

    Department of Clinical and Movement Neuroscience, University College London, London, United Kingdom
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0003-3180-3239
  6. Nick S Ward

    Department of Clinical and Movement Neuroscience, University College London, London, United Kingdom
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  7. Sven Bestmann

    epartment for Clinical and Movement Neuroscience, Institute of Neurology, University College London, London, United Kingdom
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0002-6867-9545

Funding

Brain Research UK (201718-13)

  • Catharina Zich

Brain Research UK (201617-03)

  • Catharina Zich

Wellcome Trust (098369/Z/12/Z)

  • Andrew J Quinn

Engineering and Physical Sciences Research Council (EP/T001046/1)

  • George O'Neill

Medical Research Council (MR/N013867/1)

  • Lydia C Mardell

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Ethics

Human subjects: The study protocol was in full accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and all participants gave written informed consent after being fully informed about the purpose of the study. The study protocol, participant information, and form of consent, were approved by the UCL Research Ethics Committee (reference number 5833/001).

Copyright

© 2023, Zich et al.

This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License permitting unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.

Metrics

  • 1,469
    views
  • 236
    downloads
  • 13
    citations

Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.

Download links

A two-part list of links to download the article, or parts of the article, in various formats.

Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)

Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)

Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)

  1. Catharina Zich
  2. Andrew J Quinn
  3. James J Bonaiuto
  4. George O'Neill
  5. Lydia C Mardell
  6. Nick S Ward
  7. Sven Bestmann
(2023)
Spatiotemporal organization of human sensorimotor beta burst activity
eLife 12:e80160.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.80160

Share this article

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.80160

Further reading

    1. Neuroscience
    Gergely F Turi, Sasa Teng ... Yueqing Peng
    Research Article

    Synchronous neuronal activity is organized into neuronal oscillations with various frequency and time domains across different brain areas and brain states. For example, hippocampal theta, gamma, and sharp wave oscillations are critical for memory formation and communication between hippocampal subareas and the cortex. In this study, we investigated the neuronal activity of the dentate gyrus (DG) with optical imaging tools during sleep-wake cycles in mice. We found that the activity of major glutamatergic cell populations in the DG is organized into infraslow oscillations (0.01–0.03 Hz) during NREM sleep. Although the DG is considered a sparsely active network during wakefulness, we found that 50% of granule cells and about 25% of mossy cells exhibit increased activity during NREM sleep, compared to that during wakefulness. Further experiments revealed that the infraslow oscillation in the DG was correlated with rhythmic serotonin release during sleep, which oscillates at the same frequency but in an opposite phase. Genetic manipulation of 5-HT receptors revealed that this neuromodulatory regulation is mediated by Htr1a receptors and the knockdown of these receptors leads to memory impairment. Together, our results provide novel mechanistic insights into how the 5-HT system can influence hippocampal activity patterns during sleep.

    1. Neuroscience
    Sven Ohl, Martin Rolfs
    Research Article

    Detecting causal relations structures our perception of events in the world. Here, we determined for visual interactions whether generalized (i.e. feature-invariant) or specialized (i.e. feature-selective) visual routines underlie the perception of causality. To this end, we applied a visual adaptation protocol to assess the adaptability of specific features in classical launching events of simple geometric shapes. We asked observers to report whether they observed a launch or a pass in ambiguous test events (i.e. the overlap between two discs varied from trial to trial). After prolonged exposure to causal launch events (the adaptor) defined by a particular set of features (i.e. a particular motion direction, motion speed, or feature conjunction), observers were less likely to see causal launches in subsequent ambiguous test events than before adaptation. Crucially, adaptation was contingent on the causal impression in launches as demonstrated by a lack of adaptation in non-causal control events. We assessed whether this negative aftereffect transfers to test events with a new set of feature values that were not presented during adaptation. Processing in specialized (as opposed to generalized) visual routines predicts that the transfer of visual adaptation depends on the feature similarity of the adaptor and the test event. We show that the negative aftereffects do not transfer to unadapted launch directions but do transfer to launch events of different speeds. Finally, we used colored discs to assign distinct feature-based identities to the launching and the launched stimulus. We found that the adaptation transferred across colors if the test event had the same motion direction as the adaptor. In summary, visual adaptation allowed us to carve out a visual feature space underlying the perception of causality and revealed specialized visual routines that are tuned to a launch’s motion direction.