Balancing true and false detection of intermittent sensory targets by adjusting the inputs to the evidence accumulation process

  1. Anna Catharina Geuzebroek  Is a corresponding author
  2. Hannah Craddock
  3. Redmond G O'Connell
  4. Simon P Kelly  Is a corresponding author
  1. University College Dublin, Ireland
  2. Trinity College Dublin, Ireland

Abstract

Decisions about noisy stimuli are widely understood to be made by accumulating evidence up to a decision bound that can be adjusted according to task demands. However, relatively little is known about how such mechanisms operate in continuous monitoring contexts requiring intermittent target detection. Here, we examined neural decision processes underlying detection of 1-second coherence-targets within continuous random dot motion, and how they are adjusted across contexts with Weak, Strong, or randomly Mixed Weak/Strong targets. Our prediction was that decision bounds would be set lower when Weak targets are more prevalent. Behavioural hit and false alarm rate patterns were consistent with this, and were well-captured by a bound-adjustable leaky accumulator model. However, Beta-band EEG signatures of motor preparation contradicted this, instead indicating lower bounds in the Strong-target context. We thus tested two alternative models in which decision bound dynamics were constrained directly by Beta measurements, respectively featuring leaky accumulation with adjustable leak, and non-leaky accumulation of evidence referenced to an adjustable sensory-level criterion. We found that the latter model best explained both behaviour and neural dynamics, highlighting novel means of decision policy regulation and the value of neurally-informed modelling.

Data availability

Code to recreated the Random Dot Motion task utilising Psychtoolbox is publicly available at https://github.com/AnnaCGeuzebroek/Context-Dependent-Detection. All code to recreated the behavioural and EEG data analysis as well as the modelling code can be found at https://github.com/AnnaCGeuzebroek/Continuous-Behavioural-Modelling. Pre-processed anonymised EEG and behavioural data is uploaded at OSFhttps://osf.io/yjvku/?view_only=7ed5aee5d09a4d5ca13de1ba169b0588

Article and author information

Author details

  1. Anna Catharina Geuzebroek

    School of Electrical and Electronic Engineering, University College Dublin, Dublin, Ireland
    For correspondence
    anna.geuzebroek@ucd.ie
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared.
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0002-8287-2990
  2. Hannah Craddock

    School of Electrical and Electronic Engineering, University College Dublin, Dublin, Ireland
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared.
  3. Redmond G O'Connell

    Institute of Neuroscience, Trinity College Dublin, Dublin, Ireland
    Competing interests
    Redmond G O'Connell, Reviewing editor, eLife.
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0001-6949-2793
  4. Simon P Kelly

    School of Electrical and Electronic Engineering, University College Dublin, Dublin, Ireland
    For correspondence
    simon.kelly@ucd.ie
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared.
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0001-9983-3595

Funding

Science Fundation Ireland (15/CDA/3591)

  • Anna Catharina Geuzebroek
  • Simon P Kelly

Wellcome Trust (219572/Z/19/Z)

  • Anna Catharina Geuzebroek
  • Simon P Kelly

Horizon 2020 European Research Council Consolidator Grant Ind (865474)

  • Redmond G O'Connell

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Reviewing Editor

  1. Valentin Wyart, Inserm, France

Ethics

Human subjects: All participants gave written consent prior to their participation and were compensated for their time with €25. The UCD Human Research Ethics Committee for Life Sciences approved all experimental procedures in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki (LS-16-76-Craddock).

Version history

  1. Received: August 26, 2022
  2. Preprint posted: September 3, 2022 (view preprint)
  3. Accepted: August 29, 2023
  4. Accepted Manuscript published: August 30, 2023 (version 1)
  5. Version of Record published: October 3, 2023 (version 2)

Copyright

© 2023, Geuzebroek et al.

This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License permitting unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.

Metrics

  • 322
    views
  • 53
    downloads
  • 1
    citations

Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.

Download links

A two-part list of links to download the article, or parts of the article, in various formats.

Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)

Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)

Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)

  1. Anna Catharina Geuzebroek
  2. Hannah Craddock
  3. Redmond G O'Connell
  4. Simon P Kelly
(2023)
Balancing true and false detection of intermittent sensory targets by adjusting the inputs to the evidence accumulation process
eLife 12:e83025.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.83025

Share this article

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.83025

Further reading

    1. Biochemistry and Chemical Biology
    2. Neuroscience
    Maximilian Nagel, Marco Niestroj ... Marc Spehr
    Research Article

    In most mammals, conspecific chemosensory communication relies on semiochemical release within complex bodily secretions and subsequent stimulus detection by the vomeronasal organ (VNO). Urine, a rich source of ethologically relevant chemosignals, conveys detailed information about sex, social hierarchy, health, and reproductive state, which becomes accessible to a conspecific via vomeronasal sampling. So far, however, numerous aspects of social chemosignaling along the vomeronasal pathway remain unclear. Moreover, since virtually all research on vomeronasal physiology is based on secretions derived from inbred laboratory mice, it remains uncertain whether such stimuli provide a true representation of potentially more relevant cues found in the wild. Here, we combine a robust low-noise VNO activity assay with comparative molecular profiling of sex- and strain-specific mouse urine samples from two inbred laboratory strains as well as from wild mice. With comprehensive molecular portraits of these secretions, VNO activity analysis now enables us to (i) assess whether and, if so, how much sex/strain-selective ‘raw’ chemical information in urine is accessible via vomeronasal sampling; (ii) identify which chemicals exhibit sufficient discriminatory power to signal an animal’s sex, strain, or both; (iii) determine the extent to which wild mouse secretions are unique; and (iv) analyze whether vomeronasal response profiles differ between strains. We report both sex- and, in particular, strain-selective VNO representations of chemical information. Within the urinary ‘secretome’, both volatile compounds and proteins exhibit sufficient discriminative power to provide sex- and strain-specific molecular fingerprints. While total protein amount is substantially enriched in male urine, females secrete a larger variety at overall comparatively low concentrations. Surprisingly, the molecular spectrum of wild mouse urine does not dramatically exceed that of inbred strains. Finally, vomeronasal response profiles differ between C57BL/6 and BALB/c animals, with particularly disparate representations of female semiochemicals.

    1. Neuroscience
    Kenta Abe, Yuki Kambe ... Tatsuo Sato
    Research Article

    Midbrain dopamine neurons impact neural processing in the prefrontal cortex (PFC) through mesocortical projections. However, the signals conveyed by dopamine projections to the PFC remain unclear, particularly at the single-axon level. Here, we investigated dopaminergic axonal activity in the medial PFC (mPFC) during reward and aversive processing. By optimizing microprism-mediated two-photon calcium imaging of dopamine axon terminals, we found diverse activity in dopamine axons responsive to both reward and aversive stimuli. Some axons exhibited a preference for reward, while others favored aversive stimuli, and there was a strong bias for the latter at the population level. Long-term longitudinal imaging revealed that the preference was maintained in reward- and aversive-preferring axons throughout classical conditioning in which rewarding and aversive stimuli were paired with preceding auditory cues. However, as mice learned to discriminate reward or aversive cues, a cue activity preference gradually developed only in aversive-preferring axons. We inferred the trial-by-trial cue discrimination based on machine learning using anticipatory licking or facial expressions, and found that successful discrimination was accompanied by sharper selectivity for the aversive cue in aversive-preferring axons. Our findings indicate that a group of mesocortical dopamine axons encodes aversive-related signals, which are modulated by both classical conditioning across days and trial-by-trial discrimination within a day.