Gene Expression: Listen and learn
Imagine trying to master the violin with an instrument that made no sound, or trying to play football with a foot that was numb. Both would seem almost impossible because learning motor skills depends heavily on sensory feedback. As well as supporting learning, sensory information is also required to maintain these acquired motor behaviors. For instance, people who become deaf later in life gradually lose the ability to speak clearly, even though they have done so for years (Lane and Webster, 1991).
The same is true for songbirds (Marler and Doupe, 2000). The highly structured songs of these birds are learned vocalizations that become disrupted if an adult songbird loses the ability to hear (Nordeen and Nordeen, 1992; Okanoya and Yamaguchi, 1997; Woolley and Rubel, 1997). Despite inextricable links between sensory input and motoric output, it is still unclear how sensory loss affects brain circuits and, as a consequence, how it affects any learned behaviors.
Studying the impact of sensory loss can be a challenge, as learned behaviors often involve several regions of the brain. Moreover, different brain regions can harbor diverse sets of cell types that must work together. To complicate matters, these cell types are distributed differently across various brain areas, and the set of genes expressed by each cell type also varies (Figure 1).
Now, in eLife, Bradley Colquitt, Michael Brainard and colleagues at the University of California, San Francisco report new insights into how sensory loss impacts birdsong by measuring gene expression in several major brain regions necessary for song production in Bengalese finches (Colquitt et al., 2023). Similar to what happens in humans, the vocalizations of these birds change over the course of their development, changing from sounds that are unstructured and variable to songs that are highly structured and consistent by adulthood. Nevertheless, auditory input remains crucial, and deafening leads to variations in the songs of adult birds.
In their experiments, Colquitt et al. studied the expression of genes in birds that could hear and birds that were deafened: this involved measuring which genes are expressed in all cells within a brain region, and combining this with the results of prior studies to assess which of these genes were expressed together in individual cells. This enabled the team to identify the brain regions, genes and cell types that were most affected by auditory loss. This is important, because changes in gene expression do not always produce the same effect; for example, a change made to the signaling molecules in inhibitory cells will have different consequences if made to the signaling molecules in excitatory cells.
Colquitt et al. applied this approach to four brain regions necessary for learned birdsong production: HVC, RA, LMAN and Area X (Figure 1). This revealed that these song regions displayed more similar changes in gene expression and were more affected by hearing loss than adjacent non-song regions, suggesting that this neural circuit shares some common features. In hearing birds, the song regions directly connected to each other tended to share a greater number of expressed genes than those that were more distally connected. Deafening reduced this pattern and increased the variability of birdsongs.
Next, Colquitt et al. tested the idea that one brain area can affect gene expression in another by lesioning LMAN, without which the song variability typically observed in adult deafened birds does not occur. When the team lesioned LMAN in hearing birds and assayed gene expression profiles in the other three brain areas, the biggest changes occurred in one of its direct connections, RA (Figure 1). As expected, the patterns of RA gene expression were different than in the deafened birds. This suggests that direct connections between brain regions can alter patterns of gene expression. However, lesions did not lead to any significant changes in birdsong stability, which makes it difficult to interpret the behavioral relevance of these results.
A circuit-level analysis also revealed that RA and Area X were most affected by hearing loss, but why exactly is not fully clear. It is possible that the changes in gene expression in RA could be tied to behavioral alterations, as RA (together with HVC) is part of the motor pathway that birds need to perform their song. Area X, on the other hand, is predominantly active during song acquisition, but is not needed to produce and maintain songs once they have been learned (Sohrabji et al., 1990; Scharff and Nottebohm, 1991); thus, a different explanation is needed as changes in gene expression in this part of the brain would not be expected to change with the songs of adult birds. Another possibility is that because both RA and Area X receive inputs from two areas of the brain circuit (HVC and LMAN) rather than one, they are twice as likely to be ‘hit’ by a disruption in information flow following hearing loss (Nottebohm et al., 1976; Mooney and Konishi, 1991; Vates and Nottebohm, 1995). However, more research is needed to test these theories.
Finally, the experimental design was not able to distinguish between the following three potential relationships between gene expression and behavior. One option is that the loss of sensory input caused changes in gene expression, which then drove the variations in birdsong. It could also be possible that the modifications in the vocalizations itself led to changes in gene expression, which are only then indirectly associated with deafening. A third possibility is that a combination of these mechanisms is at play during different phases of behavioral changes, or in different brain areas and cell types. Defining these relationships will provide additional insight into the neural properties required to acquire and sustain learned behavior.
References
-
Speech deterioration in postlingually deafened adultsJournal of the Acoustical Society of America 89:859–866.https://doi.org/10.1121/1.1894647
-
Auditory feedback is necessary for the maintenance of stereotyped song in adult zebra finchesBehavioral and Neural Biology 57:58–66.https://doi.org/10.1016/0163-1047(92)90757-u
-
Central control of song in the canary, Serinus canariusJournal of Comparative Neurology 165:457–486.https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.901650405
-
Adult Bengalese finches (Lonchura striata var. domestica) require real‐time auditory feedback to produce normal song syntaxJournal of Neurobiology 33:343–356.
-
Selective impairment of song learning following lesions of a forebrain nucleus in the juvenile zebra finchBehavioral and Neural Biology 53:51–63.https://doi.org/10.1016/0163-1047(90)90797-a
Article and author information
Author details
Publication history
Copyright
© 2023, London
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
-
- 351
- views
-
- 26
- downloads
-
- 0
- citations
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.
Download links
Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)
Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)
Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)
Further reading
-
- Chromosomes and Gene Expression
- Neuroscience
Pathogenic variants in subunits of RNA polymerase (Pol) III cause a spectrum of Polr3-related neurodegenerative diseases including 4H leukodystrophy. Disease onset occurs from infancy to early adulthood and is associated with a variable range and severity of neurological and non-neurological features. The molecular basis of Polr3-related disease pathogenesis is unknown. We developed a postnatal whole-body mouse model expressing pathogenic Polr3a mutations to examine the molecular mechanisms by which reduced Pol III transcription results primarily in central nervous system phenotypes. Polr3a mutant mice exhibit behavioral deficits, cerebral pathology and exocrine pancreatic atrophy. Transcriptome and immunohistochemistry analyses of cerebra during disease progression show a reduction in most Pol III transcripts, induction of innate immune and integrated stress responses and cell-type-specific gene expression changes reflecting neuron and oligodendrocyte loss and microglial activation. Earlier in the disease when integrated stress and innate immune responses are minimally induced, mature tRNA sequencing revealed a global reduction in tRNA levels and an altered tRNA profile but no changes in other Pol III transcripts. Thus, changes in the size and/or composition of the tRNA pool have a causal role in disease initiation. Our findings reveal different tissue- and brain region-specific sensitivities to a defect in Pol III transcription.
-
- Biochemistry and Chemical Biology
- Chromosomes and Gene Expression
The mRNA 5'-cap structure removal by the decapping enzyme DCP2 is a critical step in gene regulation. While DCP2 is the catalytic subunit in the decapping complex, its activity is strongly enhanced by multiple factors, particularly DCP1, which is the major activator in yeast. However, the precise role of DCP1 in metazoans has yet to be fully elucidated. Moreover, in humans, the specific biological functions of the two DCP1 paralogs, DCP1a and DCP1b, remain largely unknown. To investigate the role of human DCP1, we generated cell lines that were deficient in DCP1a, DCP1b, or both to evaluate the importance of DCP1 in the decapping machinery. Our results highlight the importance of human DCP1 in decapping process and show that the EVH1 domain of DCP1 enhances the mRNA-binding affinity of DCP2. Transcriptome and metabolome analyses outline the distinct functions of DCP1a and DCP1b in human cells, regulating specific endogenous mRNA targets and biological processes. Overall, our findings provide insights into the molecular mechanism of human DCP1 in mRNA decapping and shed light on the distinct functions of its paralogs.