Risk-sensitive learning is a winning strategy for leading an urban invasion
eLife assessment
This important study uses a multi-pronged empirical and theoretical approach to advance our understanding of animal cognition. It presents convincing data on how differences in learning relate to differences in the ways that male versus female animals cope with urban environments, and more generally how reversal learning may benefit animals in urban habitats.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.89315.3.sa0Important: Findings that have theoretical or practical implications beyond a single subfield
- Landmark
- Fundamental
- Important
- Valuable
- Useful
Convincing: Appropriate and validated methodology in line with current state-of-the-art
- Exceptional
- Compelling
- Convincing
- Solid
- Incomplete
- Inadequate
During the peer-review process the editor and reviewers write an eLife Assessment that summarises the significance of the findings reported in the article (on a scale ranging from landmark to useful) and the strength of the evidence (on a scale ranging from exceptional to inadequate). Learn more about eLife Assessments
Abstract
In the unpredictable Anthropocene, a particularly pressing open question is how certain species invade urban environments. Sex-biased dispersal and learning arguably influence movement ecology, but their joint influence remains unexplored empirically, and might vary by space and time. We assayed reinforcement learning in wild-caught, temporarily captive core-, middle-, or edge-range great-tailed grackles—a bird species undergoing urban-tracking rapid range expansion, led by dispersing males. We show, across populations, both sexes initially perform similarly when learning stimulus-reward pairings, but, when reward contingencies reverse, male—versus female—grackles finish ‘relearning’ faster, making fewer choice-option switches. How do male grackles do this? Bayesian cognitive modelling revealed male grackles’ choice behaviour is governed more strongly by the ‘weight’ of relative differences in recent foraging payoffs—i.e., they show more pronounced risk-sensitive learning. Confirming this mechanism, agent-based forward simulations of reinforcement learning—where we simulate ‘birds’ based on empirical estimates of our grackles’ reinforcement learning—replicate our sex-difference behavioural data. Finally, evolutionary modelling revealed natural selection should favour risk-sensitive learning in hypothesised urban-like environments: stable but stochastic settings. Together, these results imply risk-sensitive learning is a winning strategy for urban-invasion leaders, underscoring the potential for life history and cognition to shape invasion success in human-modified environments.
Introduction
Dispersal and range expansion go ‘hand in hand’; movement by individuals away from a population’s core is a pivotal precondition of witnessed growth in species’ geographic limits (Ronce, 2007; Chuang and Peterson, 2016). Because ‘who’ disperses—in terms of sex—varies both within and across taxa (e.g., male-biased dispersal is dominant among fish and mammals, whereas female-biased dispersal is dominant among birds; see Table 1 in Trochet et al., 2016), skewed sex ratios are apt to arise at expanding range fronts, and, in turn, differentially drive invasion dynamics (Miller et al., 2011). Female-biased dispersal, for instance, can ‘speed up’ staged invertebrate invasions by increasing offspring production (Miller and Inouye, 2013). Alongside sex-biased dispersal, cognition is also argued to contribute to species’ colonisation capacity, as novel environments inevitably present novel (foraging, predation, shelter, and social) challenges that newcomers need to surmount in order to settle successfully (Wright et al., 2010; Sutter and Kawecki, 2009). Indeed, a growing number of studies show support for this supposition, at least for those animals thriving in urban environments (recent reviews: Lee and Thornton, 2021; Szabo et al., 2020; Barrett et al., 2019; but see: Vincze and Kovács, 2022). Carefully controlled choice tests show, for example, urban-dwelling individuals will learn novel stimulus-reward pairings more readily than do rural-dwelling counterparts (Batabyal and Thaker, 2019). Similarly, urban-dwellers will more frequently figure out how to overcome experimenter-placed obstacles blocking known food resources, compared to conspecific rural-dwellers (Mazza and Guenther, 2021). And how often such hidden food rewards are successfully accessed positively relates to how long specific subspecies have lived commensually with humans in urban environments (Vrbanec et al., 2021). Taken together, these data in both resident urban and non-urban species as well as urban invasive species generally support the view that urban environments favour particular learning phenotypes. It is perhaps surprising, then, that despite their apparent independent influence on movement ecology, the potential interactive influence of sex-biased dispersal and learning on successful urban invasion remains unexamined empirically (but for related theoretical study, see: Liedtke and Fromhage, 2021b; Liedtke and Fromhage, 2021a), especially as the dynamics of urban environments appear distinctly demanding.
Urban environments are hypothesised to be both stable and stochastic: more specifically, urbanisation is thought to lead to stabilisation in some aspects of biotic structure, including predation pressure, thermal habitat, and resource availability, and to enhanced abiotic disruption, such as anthropogenic noise and light pollution (reviewed in: Shochat et al., 2006; Francis and Barber, 2013; Gaston et al., 2013). Seasonal survey data from (sub)urban British neighborhoods show, for example, 40–75% of households provide supplemental feeding resources for birds (e.g., seed, bread, and peanuts; Cowie and Hinsley, 1988; Davies et al., 2009), the density of which can positively relate to avian abundance within an urban area (Fuller et al., 2008). But such supplemental feeding opportunities are theorised to be traded off against unpredictable and likely fitness-affecting anthropogenic disturbances (e.g., automobile and airplane traffic; as outlined in Frid and Dill, 2002). Experimental data show, for example, the more variable are traffic noise and pedestrian presence, the more negative are such human-driven effects on birds’ sleep (Grunst et al., 2021), mating (Blickley et al., 2012), and foraging behaviour (Fernández-Juricic, 2000). Understanding how and why particular species successfully inhabit seemingly stable but stochastic urban environments remains an open and timely question, as highlighted by recent concerns over (in)vertebrates’ coping ability to current and projected levels of urbanisation (Eisenhauer and Hines, 2021; Li et al., 2022).
Great-tailed grackles (Quiscalus mexicanus; henceforth, grackles) are an excellent model for empirical examination of the interplay between sex-biased dispersal, learning, and ongoing urban-targeted rapid range expansion: over the past ∼150 years, they have seemingly shifted their historically human-associated niche to include more variable urban environments (e.g., arid habitat; Summers et al., 2023; Haemig, 1978), moving from their native range in Central America into much of the United States, with several first-sightings spanning as far north as Canada (Dinsmore and Dinsmore, 1993; Wehtje, 2003; Fink et al., 2020). Notably, the record of this urban invasion is heavily peppered with first sightings involving a single or multiple male(s) (41 of 63 recorded cases spanning most of the twentieth century; Dinsmore and Dinsmore, 1993). Moreover, recent genetic data show, when comparing grackles within a population, average relatedness: (i) is higher among females than among males; and (ii) decreases with increasing geographic distance among females; but (iii) is unrelated to geographic distance among males; hence, confirming urban invasion in grackles is male-led via sex-biased dispersal (Sevchik et al., 2022). Considering these life history and genetic data in conjunction with data on grackle wildlife management efforts (e.g., pesticides, pyrotechnics, and sonic booms; Luscier, 2018), it seems plausible that—regardless of their shared human and urban ‘history’—urban invasion might drive differential learning between male and female grackles, potentially resulting in a spatial sorting of the magnitude of this sex difference with respect to population establishment age (i.e., sex-effect: newer population > older population; Phillips et al., 2010). Indeed, irrespective of population membership, such sex differences could come about via differential reliance on learning strategies mediated by an interaction between grackles’ polygynous mating system and male-biased dispersal system (see our in-depth discussion on this point). Population membership might, in turn, differentially moderate the magnitude of any such sex-effect since an edge population, even though urban, could still pose novel challenges—e.g., by requiring grackles to learn novel daily temporal foraging patterns such as when and where garbage is collected (grackles appear to track this food resource: Rodrigo et al., 2021).
Here, we test the hypothesis that sex differences in dispersal are related to sex differences in learning in an urban invader—grackles. Specifically, we examine whether, and, if so, how sex mediates learning across 32 male and 17 female wild-caught, temporarily captive grackles either inhabiting a core (17 males, 5 females), middle (4 males, 4 females), or edge (11 males, 8 females) population of their North American range (based on year-since-first-breeding: 1951, 1996, and 2004, respectively; details in Materials and methods; Figure 1). To do this, we collated, cleaned, and curated existing reinforcement learning data (see Data provenance), wherein novel stimulus-reward pairings are presented (i.e., initial learning), and, once successfully learned via reaching a criterion, these reward contingencies are reversed (i.e., reversal learning). As range expansion should disfavour slow, error-prone learning strategies, we expect male and female grackles to differ across at least two reinforcement learning behaviours: speed (trials to criterion) and choice-option switches (times alternating between available stimuli). Specifically, as documented in our preregistration (see Supplementary file 1), if learning and dispersal relate we expect male—versus female—grackles: (predictions 1 and 2) to be faster to, firstly, learn a novel colour-reward pairing, and secondly, reverse their colour preference when the colour-reward pairing is swapped; and (prediction 3) to make fewer choice-option switches during their colour-reward learning. Finally, we further expect (prediction 4) such sex-mediated differences in learning to be more pronounced in grackles living at the edge, rather than the intermediate and/or core region of their range.
To comprehensively examine links between sex-biased dispersal and learning in urban-invading grackles, we employ a combination of Bayesian computational and cognitive modelling methods, and both agent-based and evolutionary simulation techniques. Specifically, our paper proceeds as follows: (i) we begin by describing grackles’ reinforcement learning and testing our predictions using multi-level Bayesian Poisson models; (ii) we next ‘unblackbox’ candidate learning mechanisms generating grackles’ reinforcement learning using a multi-level Bayesian reinforcement learning model, as ultimately the mechanisms of behaviour should be the target of selection; (iii) we then try to replicate our behavioural data via agent-based forward simulations, to determine if our inferred learning mechanisms sufficiently explain our grackles’ reinforcement learning; and (iv) we conclude by examining the evolutionary implications of variation in these learning mechanisms in hypothesised urban-like (or not) environments—i.e., statistical settings that vary in both stability and stochasticity—via algorithmic optimisation.
Results
Reinforcement learning behaviour
We do not observe credible population-level differences in grackles’ reinforcement learning (Supplementary file 2a and b). As such, we compare male and female grackles’ reinforcement learning across populations. Both sexes start out as similar learners, finishing initial learning in comparable trial numbers (mean trials-to-finish: males, 35; females, 36; Figure 2A and Supplementary file 2a), and with comparable counts of choice-option switches (mean switches-at-finish: males, 14; females, 14; Figure 2 and Supplementary file 2b). Indeed, the male-female (M-F) posterior contrasts for both behaviours centre around zero, evidencing no sex-effect (Figure 2C). Once reward contingencies reverse, however, male—versus female—grackles finish this ‘relearning’ faster by taking fewer trials (mean trials-to-finish: males, 67; females, 81; Figure 2B and Supplementary file 2a), and by making fewer choice-option switches (mean switches-at-finish: males, 25; females, 36; Figure 2B and Supplementary file 2b). The M-F posterior contrasts, which lie almost entirely below zero, clearly capture this sex-effect (Figure 2F and Supplementary file 2a and b). Environmental unpredictability, then, dependably directs disparate reinforcement learning trajectories between male and female grackles, supporting our overall expectation of sex-mediated differential learning in urban-invading grackles.
Reinforcement learning mechanisms
Because (dis)similar behaviour can result from multiple latent processes (McElreath, 2018), we next employ computational methods to delimit reinforcement learning mechanisms. Specifically, we adapt a standard multi-level Bayesian reinforcement learning model (from Deffner et al., 2020; but see also: Farrell and Lewandowsky, 2018; McElreath et al., 2005; Sutton and Barto, 2018), which we validate a priori via agent-based simulation (see Materials and methods and Supplementary file 1), to estimate the contribution of two core latent learning parameters to grackles’ reinforcement learning: the information-updating rate φ (How rapidly do learners ‘revise’ knowledge?) and the risk-sensitivity rate λ (How strongly do learners ‘weight’ knowledge?). Both learning parameters capture individual-level internal response to incurred reward-payoffs, but they differentially direct any reward sensitivity back on choice behaviour due to their distinct definitions (full mathematical details in Materials and methods). For φ, stronger reward sensitivity (bigger values) means faster internal updating about stimulus-reward pairings, which translates behaviourally into faster learning about ‘what to choose’. For λ, stronger reward sensitivity (bigger values) means stronger internal determinism about seeking the non-risk foraging option (i.e., the one with the higher expected payoffs based on prior experience), which translates behaviourally into less choice-option switching i.e., ‘playing it safe’. In sum, conditional on our reinforcement learning model, we reverse engineer which values of our learning parameters most likely produce grackles’ choice behaviour—an analytical advantage over less mechanistic methods (McElreath, 2018).
Looking at our reinforcement learning model’s estimates between populations to determine replicability, we observe, in initial learning, the information-updating rate φ of core- and edge-inhabiting male grackles is largely lower than that of female counterparts (M-F posterior contrasts lie more below zero; Figure 2G and Supplementary file 2c), with smaller sample size likely explaining the middle population’s more uncertain estimates (M-F posterior contrasts centre widely around zero; Figure 2G and Supplementary file 2c); while in reversal learning, the information-updating rate φ of both sexes is nearly identical irrespective of population membership, with females dropping to the reduced level of males (M-F posterior contrasts centre closely around zero; Figure 2H and Supplementary file 2c). Therefore, the information-updating rate φ across male and female grackles is initially different (males < females), but converges downwards over reinforcement learning phases (across-population M-F posterior contrasts first lie mostly below, and then, tightly bound zero; Figure 2G and H and Supplementary file 2c).
These primary mechanistic findings are, at first glance, perplexing: if male grackles generally outperform female grackles in reversal learning (Figure 2D–F), why do all grackles ultimately update information at matched, dampened pace? This apparent conundrum, however, in fact highlights the potential for multiple latent processes to direct behaviour. Case in point: the risk-sensitivity rate λ is distinctly higher in male grackles, compared to female counterparts, regardless of population membership and learning phase (M-F posterior contrasts lie more, if not mostly, above zero; Figure 2I and J and Supplementary file 2d), outwith the middle population in initial learning likely due to sample size (M-F posterior contrasts centre broadly around zero; Figure 2I and Supplementary file 2d). In other words, choice behaviour in male grackles is more strongly governed by relative differences in predicted reward-payoffs, as spotlighted by across-population M-F posterior contrasts that lie mostly above zero in initial learning, and entirely above zero in reversal learning (Figure 2I and J and Supplementary file 2d). Thus, these combined mechanistic data reveal, when reward contingencies reverse, male—versus female—grackles ‘relearn’ faster via pronounced risk-sensitive learning.
Agent-based forward simulations and replication of reinforcement learning
To determine definitively whether our estimated learning parameters are sufficient to generate grackles’ observed reinforcement learning, we conduct agent-based forward simulations. Agent-based forward simulations are posterior predictions, and they provide insight into the implied model dynamics and overall usefulness of our reinforcement learning model. Specifically, agent-based forward simulations allow us to ask—what would a ‘new’ grackle ‘do’, given our reinforcement learning model parameter estimates? It is important to ask this question because, in parameterising our model, we may have overlooked a critical contributing mechanism to grackles’ reinforcement learning. Such an omission is invisible in the raw parameter estimates; it is only betrayed by the estimates in actu. The simulation thus assigns ‘birds’ random information-updating rate φ and risk-sensitivity rate λ draws (whilst maintaining their correlation structure), and tracks their reinforcement learning. By comparing these synthetic data to our real data, we gain valuable insight into the learning and choice behaviour implied by our reinforcement learning model results. If no critical contributing mechanism(s) went overlooked, simulated ‘birds’ should behave similar to our real birds. A disparate mapping between simulated ‘birds’ and our real birds, however, would mean more work is needed with respect to model parameterisation that captures the causal, mechanistic dynamics behind real birds’ reinforcement learning (for an example of this happening in the human reinforcement learning literature, see Deffner et al., 2020).
Ten thousand synthetic reinforcement learning trajectories (5000 ‘males’ and 5000 ‘females’), together, compellingly show our ‘birds’ behave just like our grackles: ‘males’ outpace ‘females’ in reversal but not in initial learning (mean trials-to-finish in initial and reversal learning: ‘males’, 35 and 71; ‘females’, 38 and 91; respectively; Figure 2K and N); and ‘males’ make fewer choice-option switches in initial but not in reversal learning, compared to ‘females’ (mean switches-at-finish in initial and reversal learning: ‘males’, 13 and 23; ‘females’, 14 and 35; respectively; Figure 2L and O). Figure 2M and P show, respectively, synthetic initial and reversal learning trajectories by three average ‘males’ and three average ‘females’ (i.e., simulants informed via learning parameter estimates that average over our posterior distribution), for the reader interested in representative individual-level reinforcement learning dynamics. Such quantitative replication confirms our reinforcement learning model results sufficiently explain our behavioural sex-difference data.
Selection of reinforcement learning mechanisms under urban-like environments
Learning mechanisms in grackles obviously did not evolve to be successful in the current study; instead, they likely reflect selection pressures and/or adaptive phenotypic plasticity imposed by urban environments (Blackburn et al., 2009; Sol et al., 2013; Lee and Thornton, 2021; Vinton et al., 2022; Caspi et al., 2022). Applying an evolutionary algorithm model (Figure 3A), we conclude by examining how urban environments might favour different information-updating rate φ and risk-sensitivity rate λ values, by estimating optimal learning strategies in settings that differ along the two ecological axes thought to represent urban environmental characteristics (see Introduction): environmental stability u (How often does optimal behaviour change?) and environmental stochasticity s (How often does optimal behaviour fail to payoff?). In reality, much additional (a)biotic and life history information is relevant to thoroughly testing such urban eco-evolutionary theory. But our evolutionary model is a necessary and useful first step towards addressing targeted research questions about the interplay between learning phenotype and apparent environmental characteristics.
Strikingly, under hypothesised urban-like (i.e., stable but stochastic) environments, our evolutionary model shows the learning parameter constellation robustly exhibited by males grackles in our study—low information-updating rate φ and high risk-sensitivity rate λ—should be favoured by natural selection (darker and lighter squares in, respectively, the left and right plots in Figure 3B). These results imply, in seemingly urban and other statistically similar environments, learners benefit by averaging over prior experience (i.e., gradually updating ‘beliefs’), and by informing behaviour based on this experiential history (i.e., proceeding with ‘caution’), highlighting the adaptive value of strategising risk-sensitive learning in such settings. We note that, for most rows of environmental stochasticity s, intermediate levels of environmental stability u generated the highest risk-sensitivity rate λ values, while risk-sensitivity rate λ values were slightly lower for both extremes of environmental stability u. The most plausible explanation for this pattern is that when the environment changes at intermediate levels, it is advantageous for learners to be fairly risk-sensitive (like our grackles), so that behaviour can be both effective and effectively adjusted. By contrast, in environments with either minimal or maximal environmental change, selection should be weaker for, or favouring lower degrees of, risk-sensitive learning because optimal behaviour is either more-or-less constant or constantly changing, respectively.
Discussion
Mapping a full pathway from behaviour to mechanisms through to selection and adaptation, we show risk-sensitive learning is a viable strategy to help explain how male grackles—the dispersing sex—currently lead their species’ remarkable North American urban invasion. Specifically, in wild-caught, temporarily captive core-, middle-, or edge-range grackles, we show: (i) irrespective of population membership, male grackles outperform female counterparts on stimulus-reward reversal reinforcement learning, finishing faster and making fewer choice-option switches; (ii) they achieve their superior reversal learning performance via pronounced risk-sensitive learning (low φ and high λ), as ‘unblackboxed’ by our mechanistic model; (iii) these learning mechanisms sufficiently explain our sex-difference behavioural data: because we replicate our results using agent-based forward simulations; and (iv) risk-sensitive learning—i.e., low φ and high λ—appears advantageous in hypothesised urban-like environments (stable but stochastic settings), according to our evolutionary model. These results set the scene for future comparative research.
The term ‘behavioural flexibility’—broadly defined as some ‘attribute’, ‘cognition’, ‘characteristic’, ‘feature’, ‘trait’, and/or ‘quality’ that enables animals to adapt behaviour to changing circumstances (Coppens et al., 2010; Audet and Lefebvre, 2017; Barrett et al., 2019; Lea et al., 2020)—has previously been hypothesised to explain invasion success (Wright et al., 2010), including that of grackles (Summers et al., 2023). But as eloquently argued elsewhere (Audet and Lefebvre, 2017), this term is conceptually uninformative, given the many ways in which it is applied and assessed. Of these approaches, reversal learning and serial—multiple back-to-back—reversal learning tasks are the most common experimental assays of behavioural flexibility (non-exhaustive examples of each assay in: bees; Strang and Sherry, 2014; Raine and Chittka, 2012; birds; Bond et al., 2007; Morand-Ferron et al., 2022; fish; Lucon-Xiccato and Bisazza, 2014; Bensky and Bell, 2020; frogs; Liu et al., 2016; Burmeister, 2022; reptiles; Batabyal and Thaker, 2019; Gaalema, 2011; primates; Cantwell et al., 2022; Lacreuse et al., 2018; rodents; Rochais et al., 2021; Boulougouris et al., 2007). We have shown, however, at least for our grackles, faster reversal learning is governed primarily by risk-sensitive learning, so: firstly, these go-to reversal learning assays do not necessarily measure the unit they claim to measure (a point similarly highlighted in: Aljadeff and Lotem, 2021; Federspiel et al., 2017); and secondly, formal models based on the false premise that variation in learning speed relates to variation in behavioural flexibility require reassessment (Lea et al., 2020; Blaisdell et al., 2021; Logan et al., 2022; Lukas et al., 2024; Logan et al., 2023a; Logan et al., 2023b). Indeed, perhaps unsurprisingly (Figure 3B), grackles that learn how to learn in serial reversal experiments (i.e., high and low environments) do so primarily through shifts up in information-updating rates (Lukas et al., 2024), though this consecutive relearning task does not resemble the foraging scenarios or the apparent environmental dynamics grackles navigate day-to-day in urban environments (Fronimos et al., 2011; Rodrigo et al., 2021; Shochat et al., 2006; Francis and Barber, 2013; Gaston et al., 2013). Heeding previous calls (Dukas, 1998; McNamara and Houston, 2009; Fawcett et al., 2013), our study provides an analytical solution to facilitate productive research on proximate and ultimate explanations of seemingly flexible (or not) behaviour: because we publicly provide step-by-step code to examine individual decision-making, two core underlying learning mechanisms, and their theoretical selection and benefit (see GitHub, copy archived at Breen, 2024), which can be tailored to specific research questions. The reinforcement learning model, for example, generalises to, in theory, a variety of choice-option paradigms (Barrett, 2023), and these learning models can be extended to estimate asocial and social influence on individual decision-making (e.g., McElreath et al., 2005; Aplin et al., 2017; Barrett et al., 2017; Deffner et al., 2020; Chimento et al., 2022), facilitating insight into the multi-faceted feedback process between individual cognition and social systems (Tump et al., 2024). Our open-access analytical resource thus allows researchers to dispense with the umbrella term behavioural flexibility, and to biologically inform and interpret their science—only then can we begin to meaningfully examine the functional basis of behavioural variation across taxa and/or contexts.
Ideas and speculation
Related to this final point, it is useful to outline how additional drivers outwith sex-biased risk-sensitive learning might contribute towards urban-invasion success in grackles, too. Grackles exhibit a polygynous mating system, with territorial males attracting multiple female nesters (Johnson et al., 2000). Recent learning ‘style’ simulations show the sex with high reproductive skew approaches pure individual learning, while the other sex approaches pure social learning (Smolla et al., 2019). During population establishment, then, later-arriving female grackles could rely heavily on vetted information provided by male grackles on ‘what to do’ (Wright et al., 2010), as both sexes ultimately face the same urban-related challenges. Moreover, risk-sensitive learning in male grackles should help reduce the elevated risk associated with any skew towards acquiring knowledge through individual learning. And as the dispersing sex this process would operate independently of their proximity to a range front—a pattern suggestively supported by our mechanistic data (i.e., risk-sensitivity: males > females; Figure 2G and H). As such, future research on potential sex differences in social learning propensity in grackles seem particularly prudent, alongside systematic surveying of population-level environmental and fitness components across spatially (dis)similar populations.
The lack of spatial replicates in the existing data set used herein inherently poses limitations on inference. Nevertheless, the currently available data do not show meaningful population-level behavioural or mechanistic differences in grackles’ reinforcement learning, and we should thus be cautious about speculating on between-population variation. But it is worth noting that phenotypic filtering by invasion stage is not a compulsory signature of successful (urban) invasion. Instead, phenotypic plasticity and/or inherent species trait(s) may be facilitators (Blackburn et al., 2009; Sol et al., 2013; Lee and Thornton, 2021; Vinton et al., 2022; Caspi et al., 2022). For urban-invading grackles, both of these biological explanations seem strongly plausible, given: firstly, grackles’ highly plastic foraging and nesting ecology (Selander and Giller, 1961; Davis and Arnold, 1972; Wehtje, 2003); secondly, grackles’ apparent historic and current realised niche being—albeit in present day more variable—urban and human-associated environments, a consistent habit preference that cannot be explained by changes in habitat availability or habitat connectivity (Summers et al., 2023); and finally, our combined behavioural, mechanistic, and evolutionary modelling results showing environments apparently approaching grackles’ general species niche—human and urban environments—select for particular traits that exist across grackle populations (here, sex-biased risk-sensitive learning). Admittedly, our evolutionary model is not a complete representation of urban ecology dynamics. Relevant factors—e.g., spatial dynamics and realistic life histories—are missing. These omissions are tactical ones. Our evolutionary model solely focuses on the response of reinforcement learning parameters to hypothesised urban-like (or not) environmental statistics, providing a baseline for future studies to build on; for example, it would be interesting to investigate such selection on learning parameters of ‘true’ invaders and not their descendants, a logistically tricky but nonetheless feasible research possibility (e.g., Duckworth and Badyaev, 2007).
Conclusions
By revealing across-population interactive links between the dispersing sex and risk-sensitive learning in an urban invader (grackles), these analytically replicable insights, coupled with our finding that hypothesised urban-like environments favour pronounced risk-sensitivity, imply risk-sensitive learning is a winning strategy for urban-invasion leaders. Our modelling methods, which we document in-depth and make freely available, can now be comparatively applied, establishing a biologically meaningful analytical approach for much-needed study on (shared or divergent) drivers of geographic and phenotypic distributions (Somveille et al., 2018; Bro-Jørgensen et al., 2019; Lee and Thornton, 2021; Breen et al., 2021; Breen, 2021; Deffner et al., 2022).
Materials and methods
Data provenance
Request a detailed protocolThe current study uses data from two types of sources: publicly archived data at the Knowledge Network for Biocomplexity (Logan, 2018; Logan et al., 2021); or privately permissed access to AJB of (at the time) unpublished data by Grackle Project principal investigator Corina Logan, who declined participation on this study. We note these shared data are now also available at the Knowledge Network for Biocomplexity (Logan et al., 2023c). Finally, we note the cleaned versions of at least part of these data (see Reinforcement learning criterion) are available at our GitHub repository.
Data contents
Request a detailed protocolThe data used herein chart colour-reward reinforcement learning performance from 32 male and 17 female wild-caught, temporarily captive grackles inhabiting one of three study sites that differ in their range expansion demographics i.e., defined as a core, middle, or edge population (based on time-since-settlement population growth dynamics, as outlined in Chuang and Peterson, 2016). Specifically: (i) Tempe, Arizona (17 males and 5 females)—herein, the core population (estimated to be breeding since 1951, by adding the average time between first sighting and first breeding to the year first sighted; Wehtje, 2003; Wehtje, 2004); (ii) Santa Barbara, California (4 males and 4 females)—herein, the middle population (known to be breeding since 1996; Lehman, 2020); and (iii) Greater Sacramento, California (11 males and 8 females)—herein, the edge population (known to be breeding since 2004; Hampton, 2004).
Experimental protocol
Request a detailed protocolA step-by-step description of the experimental protocol carried out by the original experimenters is reported elsewhere (Blaisdell et al., 2021). As such, below we detail only the protocol for the colour-reward reinforcement learning test that we analysed herein.
Reinforcement learning test
Request a detailed protocolThe reinforcement learning test consists of two experimental phases (Figure 1): (i) stimulus-reward initial learning and (ii) stimulus-reward reversal learning. In both experimental phases, two different coloured tubes are used: for Santa Barbara grackles, gold and grey; for all other grackles, light and dark grey. Each tube consists of an outer and inner diameter of 26 mm and 19 mm, respectively; and each is mounted to two pieces of plywood attached at a right angle (entire apparatus: 50 mm wide×50 mm tall×67 mm deep); thus resulting in only one end of each coloured tube being accessible (Figure 1).
In initial learning, grackles are required to learn that only one of the two coloured tubes contains a food reward (e.g., dark grey); this colour-reward pairing is counterbalanced across grackles within each study site. Specifically, the rewarded and unrewarded coloured tubes are placed—either on a table or on the floor—in the centre of the aviary run (distance apart: table, 2 feet; floor, 3 feet), with the open tube-ends facing, and perpendicular to, their respective aviary side wall. Which coloured tube is placed on which side of the aviary run (left or right) is pseudorandomised across trials. A trial begins at tube placement, and ends when a grackle has either made a tube choice or the maximum trial time has elapsed (8 min). A tube choice is defined as a grackle bending down to examine the contents (or lack thereof) of a tube. If the chosen tube contains food, the grackle is allowed to retrieve and eat the food, before both tubes are removed and the rewarded coloured tube is rebaited out of sight (for the grackle). If a chosen tube does not contain food, both tubes are immediately removed. Each grackle is given, first, up to 3 min to make a tube choice, after which a piece of food is placed equidistant between the tubes to entice participation; and then, if no choice has been made, an additional 5 min maximum, before both tubes are removed. All trials are recorded as either correct (choosing the rewarded coloured tube), incorrect (choosing the unrewarded coloured tube), or incomplete (no choice made). To successfully finish initial learning, a grackle must meet the learning criterion, detailed below.
In reversal learning, grackles are required to learn that the colour-reward pairing has been swapped; that is, the previously unrewarded coloured tube (e.g., light grey) now contains a food reward (Figure 1). The protocol for this second and final experimental phase is identical to that, described above, of initial learning.
Reinforcement learning criterion
Request a detailed protocolFor all grackles in the current study, we apply the following learning criterion: to successfully finish their respective learning phase, grackles must make a correct choice in 17 of the most recent 20 trials. Therefore, the earliest a grackle can successfully finish initial or reversal learning in the current study is at trail 17. This applied learning criterion is the most compatible with respect to previous learning criteria used by the original experimenters. Specifically, Logan, 2018, and Logan et al., 2021, used a fixed-window learning criterion for core- and middle-population grackles, in which grackles were required to make 17 out of the last 20 choices correctly, with a minimum of eight and nine correct choices across the last two sets of 10 trials, assessed at the end of each set. If a core- or middle-population grackle successfully satisfied the fixed-window learning criterion, the grackle was assigned by Logan or colleagues the final trial number for that set (e.g., 20, 30, 40), which is problematic because this trial did not always coincide with the true passing trial (by a maximum of two additional trials; see below).
For edge-population grackles, Logan et al., 2023c, used a sliding-window learning criterion, in which grackles were required to again make 17 out of the last 20 choices correctly, with the same minimum correct-choice counts for the previous two 10-trial sets, except that this criterion was assessed at every trial (from 20 onwards) rather than at the end of discrete sets. This second method is also problematic because a grackle can successfully reach criterion via a shift in the sliding-window before making a choice. For example, a grackle could make three wrong choices followed by 17 correct choices (i.e., 7/10 correct and 10/10 correct in the last two sets of 10 trials), and at the start of the next trial, the grackle will reach criterion because the summed choices now consist of 8/10 correct and at least 9/10 correct in the last two sets of 10 trials no matter their subsequent choice—see initial learning performance by bird ‘Kel’ for a real example (row 1816 in GitHub; as well as in Logan et al., 2023c). Moreover, the use of different learning criteria (fixed- and sliding-window) by Logan and colleagues in different populations represents a confound when populations are compared. Thus, our applied 17/20 learning criterion ensures our assessment of grackles’ reinforcement learning is informative, straightforward, and consistent.
As a consequence of applying our 17/20 learning criterion, grackles can remain in initial and/or reversal learning beyond reaching criterion. These extra learning trials, however, already exist for some core- and middle-population grackles originally assessed via the fixed-window learning criterion ( grackles in initial learning [range: 1–2 extra trials]; grackles in reversal learning [range: 1–2 extra trials]), as explained above. And our cleaning of the original data (see our Data_Processing.R script at GitHub) detected additional cases where grackles remained in-test despite meeting the applied criterion (fixed-window: grackle in reversal learning for 11 extra trials; sliding-window: grackles in initial learning [range: 1–10 extra trials]; grackles in reversal learning [range: 1–14 extra trials]), presumably due to original experimenter oversight. Similarly, our data cleaning detected two grackles in the middle population that were passed by the original experimenters despite not meeting the assigned fixed-window learning criterion; instead, both chose 7/10 and 10/10 correct choices in the last two sets of 10 trials. Moreover, our data cleaning detected an additional four grackles in the core population that did not in fact meet the fixed-window learning criterion because of incorrect trail numbers entered by the original experimenters e.g., skipping trial 24. In any case, in our study we verified: (i) our 17/20 learning criterion results in a similar proportion of male and female grackles experiencing extra initial learning trials (females, 15/17; males, 30/32); and (ii) our learning parameter estimations during initial learning remain relatively unchanged irrespective of whether we exclude or include extra initial learning trails (Figure 2—figure supplement 1). Thus, we are confident that any carryover effect of extra initial learning trials on grackles’ reversal learning in our study is negligible if not nonexistent, and we therefore excluded extra learning trials.
Statistical analyses
Request a detailed protocolWe analysed, processed, and visually present our data using, respectively, the ‘rstan’ (Stan Development Team, 2020), ‘rethinking’ (McElreath, 2018), and ‘tidyverse’ (Wickham et al., 2019) packages in R (R Development Core Team, 2021). We note our reproducible code is available at GitHub. We further note our reinforcement learning model, defined below, does not exclude cases—two males in the core, and one male in the middle population—where a grackle was dropped (due to time constraints) early on from reversal learning by the original experimenters: because individual-level φ and λ estimates can still be estimated irrespective of trial number; the certainty around the estimates will simply be wider (McElreath, 2018). Our Poisson models, however, do exclude these three cases for our modelling of reversal learning, to conserve estimation. Finally, we note that in Bayesian statistics, there is no strict lower limit of required sample size as the inferences do not rely on asymptotic assumptions. With inferences remaining valid in principle, low sample size will of course be reflected in rather uncertain posterior estimates. We further note all of our multi-level models use partial pooling on individuals (the random-effects structure), which is a regularisation technique that especially improves inference in case of low sample sizes (see Ch. 13 in McElreath, 2018). The full output from each of our models, which use weakly informative priors, is available in Supplementary file 2a–d, including posterior means and 89% highest posterior density intervals that convey the most likely values of our parameters over this range (McElreath, 2018).
Poisson models
Request a detailed protocolFor our behavioural assay of reinforcement learning finishing trajectories, we used a multi-level Bayesian Poisson regression to quantify the effect(s) of sex and learning phase (initial versus reversal) on grackles’ recorded number of trials to successfully finish each phase. This model was performed at both the population and across-population level, and accounted for individual differences among birds through the inclusion of individual-specific varying (i.e., random) effects.
For our behavioural assay of reinforcement learning choice-option switching, we used an identical Poisson model to that described above, to predict the total number of switches between the rewarded and unrewarded coloured tubes.
Reinforcement learning model
Request a detailed protocolWe employed an adapted (from Deffner et al., 2020) multi-level Bayesian reinforcement learning model, to examine the influence of sex on grackles’ initial and reversal learning. Our reinforcement learning model, defined below, allows us to link observed coloured tube choices to latent individual-level attraction updating, and to translate the influence of latent attractions (i.e., expected payoffs) into individual tube choice probabilities. As introduced above, we can reverse engineer which values of our two latent learning parameters—the information-updating rate φ and the risk-sensitivity rate λ—most likely produce grackles’ choice behaviour, by formulating our scientific model as a statistical model. Therefore, this computational method facilitates mechanistic insight into how multiple latent learning parameters simultaneously guide grackles’ reinforcement learning (McElreath, 2018).
Our reinforcement learning model consists of two equations:
Equation 1 expresses how attraction to choice-option changes for an individual across time () based on their prior attraction to that choice-option () plus their recently experienced choice reward-payoffs (), whilst accounting for the relative influence of recent reward-payoffs (). As increases in value, so, too, does the rate of individual-level attraction updating based on reward-payoffs. Here, then, represents the information-updating rate. We highlight that the indexing (here and elsewhere) denotes that we estimate separate φ parameters for each population (=1 for core; = 2 for middle; = 3 for edge) and for each learning phase ( = 1 for females/initial, = 2 for females/reversal; = 3 for males/initial; = 4 for males/reversal).
Equation 2 is a softmax function that expresses the probability p that choice-option i is selected in the next choice-round () as a function of the attractions and the parameter , which governs how much relative differences in attraction scores guide individual choice behaviour. In the reinforcement learning literature, the λ parameter is known by several names—e.g., ‘inverse temperature’, ‘exploration’, or ‘risk-appetite’ (Sutton and Barto, 2018; Chimento et al., 2022)—since the higher its value the more deterministic the choice behaviour of an individual becomes (note generates random choice). In line with risk-sensitive foraging theory—which focuses on how animals evaluate and choose between distinct food options, and how such foraging decisions are influenced by pay-off variance i.e., risk associated with alternative foraging options (seminal reviews: Bateson, 2002; Kacelnik and Bateson, 1996)—we call λ the risk-sensitivity rate, where higher values of λ imply foragers are more sensitive to risk, seeking higher expected payoffs based on their prior experience, instead of randomly sampling alternative options.
From the above reinforcement learning model, then, we generate inferences about the effect of sex on and from at least 1000 effective samples of the posterior distribution, at both the population- and across-population level. We note our reinforcement learning model also includes bird as a random effect (to account for repeated measures within individuals); however, for clarity, this parameter is omitted from our equations (but not our code: GitHub). Our reinforcement learning model does not, on the other hand, include trials where a grackle did not make a tube choice, as this measure cannot clearly speak to individual learning—e.g., satiation rather than any learning of ‘appropriate’ colour tube choice could be invoked as an explanation in such cases. Indeed, there are, admittedly, a number of intrinsic and extrinsic factors (e.g., temperament and temperature, respectively) that might bias grackles’ tube choice behaviour, and, in turn, the output from our reinforcement learning model (Webster and Rutz, 2020). But the aim of such models is not to replicate the entire study system. Finally, we further note, while we exclude extra learning trials from all of our analyses (see above), our reinforcement learning model initiates estimation of and during reversal learning, based on individual-level attractions encompassing all previous choices. This parameterisation ensures we precisely capture grackles’ attraction scores up to the point of stimulus-reward reversal (for details, see our RL_Execution.R script at GitHub).
Agent-based simulations: pre- and post-study
Request a detailed protocolPrior to analysing our data, we used agent-based simulations to validate our reinforcement learning model (full details in our preregistration–see Supplementary file 1). In brief, the tube choice behaviour of simulants is governed by a set of rules identical to those defined by Equations 1 and 2, and we apply the exact same learning criterion for successfully finishing both learning phases. Crucially, this a priori model vetting verifies our reinforcement learning model can (i) detect simulated sex-effects and (ii) accurately recover simulated parameter values in both extreme and more realistic scenarios.
After model fitting, we used the same agent-based approach to forward simulate—i.e., simulate via the posterior distribution—synthetic learning trajectories by ‘birds’ via individual-level parameter estimates generated from our across-population reinforcement learning model. Specifically, maintaining the correlation structure among sex- and phase-specific learning parameters, we draw samples from the full or averaged random-effects multivariate normal distribution describing the inferred population of grackles. We use these post-study forward simulations to gain a better understanding of the implied consequences of the estimated sex differences in grackles’ learning parameters (see Figure 2 and associated main text; for an example of this approach in a different context, see Deffner et al., 2020). We note agent-based forward simulation does not assess goodness-of-fit—we assessed the fit of our model a priori in our preregistration (see Supplementary file 1)—but it does assess whether one did a comprehensive job of uncovering the mechanistic basis of target behaviour(s), as detailed in the main text.
Evolutionary model
Request a detailed protocolTo investigate the evolutionary significance of strategising risk-sensitive learning, we used algorithmic optimisation techniques (Yu and Gen, 2010; Otto and Day, 2011). Specifically, we construct an evolutionary model of grackle learning, to estimate how our learning parameters—the information-updating rate φ and the risk-sensitivity rate λ—evolve in environments that systematically vary across two ecologically relevant (see main text) statistical properties: the rate of environmental stability u and the rate of environmental stochasticity s. The environmental stability parameter u represents the probability that behaviour leading to a food reward changes from one choice to the next. If is small, individuals encounter a world where they can expect the same behaviour to be adaptive for a relatively long time. As becomes larger, optimal behaviour can change multiple times within an individual’s lifetime. The environmental stochasticity parameter s describes the probability that, on any given day, optimal behaviour may not result in a food reward due to external causes specific to this day. If is small, optimal behaviour reliably produces rewards. As s becomes larger, there is more and more daily ‘noise’ regarding which behaviour is rewarded.
We consider a population of fixed size with individuals. Each generation, individual agents are born naïve and make binary foraging decisions resulting in a food reward (or not). Agents decide and learn about the world through reinforcement learning governed by their individual learning parameters, φ and λ (see Equations 1 and 2). Both learning parameters can vary continuously, corresponding to the infinite-alleles model from population genetics (Otto and Day, 2011). Over the course of their lifetime, agents collect food rewards, and the sum of rewards collected over the last 800 foraging decisions (or ‘days’) determines their individual fitness. We ignore the first 200 choices because selection should respond to the steady state of the environment, independently of initial conditions (Otto and Day, 2011).
To generate the next generation, we assume asexual, haploid reproduction, and use fitness-proportionate (or ‘roulette wheel’) selection to choose individuals for reproduction (Yu and Gen, 2010; Otto and Day, 2011). Here, juveniles inherit both learning parameters, φ and λ, from their parent but with a small deviation (in random direction) due to mutation. Specifically, during each mutation event, a value drawn from zero-centered normal distributions or is added to the parent value on the logit/log-scale to ensure parameters remain within allowed limits (between 0 and 1 for φ; positive for λ). The mutation parameters and thus describe how much offspring values might deviate from parental values, which we set to 0.05. We restrict the risk-sensitivity rate λ to the interval 0–15, because greater values result in identical choice behaviour. All results reported in the main text are averaged over the last 5000 generations of 10 independent 7000-generation simulations per parameter combination. This duration is sufficient to reach steady state in all cases.
Data availability
All data (original and cleaned files) and code (for data munging, analyses, and figures) to reproduce our manuscript are available at our GitHub repository, (copy archived at Breen, 2024).
-
Knowledge Network for BiocomplexityGreat-tailed grackle behavioral flexibility and problem solving experiments, Santa Barbara, CA USA 2014-2015.https://doi.org/10.5063/F13B5XBC
-
Knowledge Network for BiocomplexityData: Implementing a rapid geographic range expansion - the role of behavior changes.https://doi.org/10.5063/F1QZ28FH
-
Knowledge Network for BiocomplexityData: Is behavioral flexibility manipulatable and, if so, does it improve flexibility and problem solving in a new context?.https://doi.org/10.5063/F1862DWC
References
-
What’s flexible in behavioral flexibility?Behavioral Ecology 28:943–947.https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arx007
-
Pay-off-biased social learning underlies the diffusion of novel extractive foraging traditions in a wild primateProceedings. Biological Sciences 284:20170358.https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2017.0358
-
The cognition of ‘nuisance’ speciesAnimal Behaviour 147:167–177.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2018.05.005
-
Inferential power in identifying frequency-dependent social learning strengthened by increasing behavioural optionsThe Journal of Animal Ecology 92:1532–1544.https://doi.org/10.1111/1365-2656.13826
-
Lizards from suburban areas learn faster to stay safeBiology Letters 15:20190009.https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2019.0009
-
Recent advances in our understanding of risk-sensitive foraging preferencesThe Proceedings of the Nutrition Society 61:509–516.https://doi.org/10.1079/pns2002181
-
Animal culture research should include avian nest constructionBiology Letters 17:20210327.https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2021.0327
-
Manipulative and technological skills do not require a slow life historyFrontiers in Ecology and Evolution 9:46.https://doi.org/10.3389/fevo.2021.635802
-
SoftwareSex-differences-in-Grackles-learning, version swh:1:rev:de7e74b22d3d9a1f96f7532437d11287d7a3f93aSoftware Heritage.
-
Linking behaviour to dynamics of populations and communities: application of novel approaches in behavioural ecology to conservationPhilosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences 374:20190008.https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2019.0008
-
Ecology, cognition, and the hippocampus: A tale of two frogsBrain, Behavior and Evolution 97:211–224.https://doi.org/10.1159/000522108
-
The origins of cognitive flexibility in chimpanzeesDevelopmental Science 25:e13266.https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.13266
-
Behavioral plasticity can facilitate evolution in urban environmentsTrends in Ecology & Evolution 37:1092–1103.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tree.2022.08.002
-
Cultural diffusion dynamics depend on behavioural production rulesProceedings. Biological Sciences 289:20221001.https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2022.1001
-
Expanding population edges: theories, traits, and trade-offsGlobal Change Biology 22:494–512.https://doi.org/10.1111/gcb.13107
-
Coping styles and behavioural flexibility: towards underlying mechanismsPhilosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 365:4021–4028.https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2010.0217
-
A national scale inventory of resource provision for biodiversity within domestic gardensBiological Conservation 142:761–771.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biocon.2008.12.016
-
Dynamic social learning in temporally and spatially variable environmentsRoyal Society Open Science 7:200734.https://doi.org/10.1098/rsos.200734
-
A causal framework for cross-cultural generalizabilityAdvances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science 5:251524592211063.https://doi.org/10.1177/25152459221106366
-
Range expansion of the great-tailed grackle in the 1900sJournal of the Iowa Academy of Science 100:54–59.
-
BookCognitive Ecology: The Evolutionary Ecology of Information Processing and Decision MakingUniversity of Chicago Press.
-
Invertebrate biodiversity and conservationCurrent Biology 31:R1214–R1218.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2021.06.058
-
BookComputational Modeling of Cognition and BehaviorCambridge University Press.https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781316272503
-
A framework for understanding noise impacts on wildlife: an urgent conservation priorityFrontiers in Ecology and the Environment 11:305–313.https://doi.org/10.1890/120183
-
Human-caused disturbance stimuli as a form of predation riskConservation Ecology 6:60111.https://doi.org/10.5751/ES-00404-060111
-
Use of urban bird feeders by white-winged doves and great-tailed grackles in texasTexas Ornithological Society 44:34.
-
Garden bird feeding predicts the structure of urban avian assemblagesDiversity and Distributions 14:131–137.https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1472-4642.2007.00439.x
-
Visual discrimination and reversal learning in rough-necked monitor lizards (Varanus rudicollis)Journal of Comparative Psychology 125:246–249.https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023148
-
The ecological impacts of nighttime light pollution: a mechanistic appraisalBiological Reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society 88:912–927.https://doi.org/10.1111/brv.12036
-
Variable and consistent traffic noise negatively affect the sleep behavior of a free-living songbirdThe Science of the Total Environment 778:146338.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.scitotenv.2021.146338
-
Male mating strategies and the mating system of great-tailed gracklesBehavioral Ecology 11:132–141.https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/11.2.132
-
Risky theories—the effects of variance on foraging decisionsAmerican Zoologist 36:402–434.https://doi.org/10.1093/icb/36.4.402
-
Behavioral flexibility: A review, a model, and some exploratory testsLearning & Behavior 48:173–187.https://doi.org/10.3758/s13420-020-00421-w
-
Animal cognition in an urbanised worldFrontiers in Ecology and Evolution 9:633947.https://doi.org/10.3389/fevo.2021.633947
-
Global impacts of future urban expansion on terrestrial vertebrate diversityNature Communications 13:1–12.https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-022-29324-2
-
Should dispersers be fast learners? Modeling the role of cognition in dispersal syndromesEcology and Evolution 11:14293–14302.https://doi.org/10.1002/ece3.8145
-
Learning to learn: advanced behavioural flexibility in a poison frogAnimal Behaviour 111:167–172.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2015.10.018
-
DataGreat-tailed Grackle behavioral flexibility and problem solving experiments, Santa Barbara, CA USA 2014-2015Knowledge Network for Biocomplexity.https://doi.org/10.5063/F13B5XBC
-
DataIs behavioral flexibility manipulatable and, if so, does it improve flexibility and problem solving in a new context?Knowledge Network for Biocomplexity.https://doi.org/10.5063/F1862DWC
-
Are the more flexible great-tailed grackles also better at behavioral inhibitionAnimal Behavior Cognition 9:14–36.https://doi.org/10.26451/abc.09.01.03.2022
-
DataData: Implementing a rapid geographic range expansion - the role of behavior changesKnowledge Network for Biocomplexity.https://doi.org/10.5063/F1QZ28FH
-
Effects of urbanization on great-tailed grackle habitat use and nest success in sherman texasUrban Naturalist 15:1–14.
-
Applying evolutionary models to the laboratory study of social learningEvolution and Human Behavior 26:483–508.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2005.04.003
-
Integrating function and mechanismTrends in Ecology & Evolution 24:670–675.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tree.2009.05.011
-
Sex-biased dispersal and the speed of two-sex invasionsThe American Naturalist 177:549–561.https://doi.org/10.1086/659628
-
Sex and stochasticity affect range expansion of experimental invasionsEcology Letters 16:354–361.https://doi.org/10.1111/ele.12049
-
BookA Biologist’s Guide to Mathematical Modeling in Ecology and EvolutionPrinceton University Press.https://doi.org/10.2307/j.ctvcm4hnd
-
SoftwareR: A language and environment for statistical computingR Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria.
-
How does it feel to be like a rolling stone? Ten questions about dispersal evolutionAnnual Review of Ecology, Evolution, and Systematics 38:231–253.https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.ecolsys.38.091206.095611
-
From patterns to emerging processes in mechanistic urban ecologyTrends in Ecology & Evolution 21:186–191.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tree.2005.11.019
-
Reproductive skew affects social information useRoyal Society Open Science 6:182084.https://doi.org/10.1098/rsos.182084
-
Behavioural adjustments for a life in the cityAnimal Behaviour 85:1101–1112.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.01.023
-
Movement and conformity interact to establish local behavioural traditions in animal populationsPLOS Computational Biology 14:e1006647.https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1006647
-
Serial reversal learning in bumblebees (Bombus impatiens)Animal Cognition 17:723–734.https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-013-0704-1
-
Influence of learning on range expansion and adaptation to novel habitatsJournal of Evolutionary Biology 22:2201–2214.https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1420-9101.2009.01836.x
-
Can cognitive ability give invasive species the means to succeed? A review of the evidenceFrontiers in Ecology and Evolution 8:187.https://doi.org/10.3389/fevo.2020.00187
-
Evolution of sex-biased dispersalThe Quarterly Review of Biology 91:297–330.https://doi.org/10.1086/688097
-
A cognitive computational approach to social and collective decision-makingPerspectives on Psychological Science 19:538–551.https://doi.org/10.1177/17456916231186964
-
Urbanization’s effects on problem solving abilities: A meta-analysisFrontiers in Ecology and Evolution 10:834436.https://doi.org/10.3389/fevo.2022.834436
-
Plasticity’s role in adaptive evolution depends on environmental change componentsTrends in Ecology & Evolution 37:1067–1078.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tree.2022.08.008
-
Enhanced problem-solving ability as an adaptation to urban environments in house miceProceedings. Biological Sciences 288:20202504.https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2020.2504
-
ThesisThe great-tailed grackle (Quiscalus mexicanus Gmelin) in the Western United States of America: Range expansion and secondary contact between subspeciesUniversity of California, Riverside, Riverside, United States.
-
Welcome to the TidyverseJournal of Open Source Software 4:1686.https://doi.org/10.21105/joss.01686
-
Behavioral flexibility and species invasions: the adaptive flexibility hypothesisEthology Ecology & Evolution 22:393–404.https://doi.org/10.1080/03949370.2010.505580
-
BookIntroduction to Evolutionary AlgorithmsSpringer Science & Business Media.https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-84996-129-5
Article and author information
Author details
Funding
Open access funding provided by Max Planck Society. No other funding was received for this work.
Acknowledgements
We thank Jean-François Gerard and Rachel Harrison for useful feedback on our study before data analyses; and James St Clair, Sue Healy, and Richard McElreath for similarly useful presubmission feedback. We also thank two anonymous reviewers for their thoughtful and useful feedback on our manuscript. We are further and most grateful to Richard McElreath for study and overall full support. And we thank all members, past and present, of the Grackle Project for collecting and making available, either via public archive or permissed advanced access, the data utilised herein (see Data provenance). We note this material uses illustrations from Vecteezy.com. Finally, we further note this material uses data from the eBird Status and Trends Project at the Cornell Lab of Ornithology, eBird.org. Any opinions, findings, and conclusions or recommendations expressed in this material are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the views of the Cornell Lab of Ornithology.
Version history
- Preprint posted:
- Sent for peer review:
- Reviewed Preprint version 1:
- Reviewed Preprint version 2:
- Version of Record published:
Cite all versions
You can cite all versions using the DOI https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.89315. This DOI represents all versions, and will always resolve to the latest one.
Copyright
© 2023, Breen and Deffner
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
-
- 1,174
- views
-
- 178
- downloads
-
- 1
- citations
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.
Download links
Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)
Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)
Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)
Further reading
-
- Ecology
Advances in tracking technologies have revealed the diverse migration patterns of birds, which are critical for range mapping and population estimation. Population trends are usually estimated in breeding ranges where birds remain stationary, but for species that breed in remote areas like the Arctic, these trends are often assessed in over-wintering ranges. Assessing population trends during the wintering season is challenging due to the extensive movements of birds in these ranges, which requires a deep understanding of the movement dynamics. However, these movements remain understudied, particularly in the mid-latitudes, where many Arctic breeders overwinter, increasing uncertainty in their ranges and numbers. Here, we show that the Arctic breeding raptor Rough-legged buzzard, which overwinters in the mid-latitudes, has a specific wintering strategy. After migrating ca. 1500 km from the Arctic to mid-latitudes, the birds continue to move throughout the entire over-wintering period, traveling another 1000 km southwest and then back northeast as the snowline advances. This continuous movement makes their wintering range dynamic throughout the season. In essence, this movement represents an extension of the quick migration process, albeit at a slower pace, and we have termed this migration pattern ‘foxtrot migration’, drawing an analogy to the alternating fast and slow movements of the foxtrot dance. These results highlight the potential errors in range mapping from single mid-winter surveys and emphasize the importance of this migration pattern in assessing the conservation status of bird species. Understanding this migration pattern could help to correctly estimate bird populations in over-wintering ranges, which is especially important for species that nest in hard-to-reach regions such as the Arctic.
-
- Ecology
- Evolutionary Biology
Seasonal polyphenism enables organisms to adapt to environmental challenges by increasing phenotypic diversity. Cacopsylla chinensis exhibits remarkable seasonal polyphenism, specifically in the form of summer-form and winter-form, which have distinct morphological phenotypes. Previous research has shown that low temperature and the temperature receptor CcTRPM regulate the transition from summer-form to winter-form in C. chinensis by impacting cuticle content and thickness. However, the underling neuroendocrine regulatory mechanism remains largely unknown. Bursicon, also known as the tanning hormone, is responsible for the hardening and darkening of the insect cuticle. In this study, we report for the first time on the novel function of Bursicon and its receptor in the transition from summer-form to winter-form in C. chinensis. Firstly, we identified CcBurs-α and CcBurs-β as two typical subunits of Bursicon in C. chinensis, which were regulated by low temperature (10 °C) and CcTRPM. Subsequently, CcBurs-α and CcBurs-β formed a heterodimer that mediated the transition from summer-form to winter-form by influencing the cuticle chitin contents and cuticle thickness. Furthermore, we demonstrated that CcBurs-R acts as the Bursicon receptor and plays a critical role in the up-stream signaling of the chitin biosynthesis pathway, regulating the transition from summer-form to winter-form. Finally, we discovered that miR-6012 directly targets CcBurs-R, contributing to the regulation of Bursicon signaling in the seasonal polyphenism of C. chinensis. In summary, these findings reveal the novel function of the neuroendocrine regulatory mechanism underlying seasonal polyphenism and provide critical insights into the insect Bursicon and its receptor.