Behavior: Flying squirrels, hidden treasures

In the rain forests of Hainan, China, two species of squirrel create grooves on the surface of smooth nuts so that they can wedge them in the forks between branches.
  1. Pizza Ka Yee Chow  Is a corresponding author
  1. School of Psychology at the University of Chester, United Kingdom

Just how we may stash away precious chocolate bars for future cravings, many animals hoard food to prepare for times of low supply (Vander Wall, 1990). Tree squirrels, for instance, are well known for employing this strategy. Some, like the American red and Douglas squirrels, are predominantly ‘larder hoarders’ who tend to store nuts and seeds in one dedicated spot, called a midden (Steele, 1998; Steele, 1999). Others, like the Eurasian red squirrels and Eastern grey squirrels, tend to be ‘scatter hoarders’ who cache their items one by one in tree cavities, between logs, or inside holes in the ground (Smith, 1968; Wauters and Casale, 1996). Now, in eLife, Suqin Fang and colleagues from institutes in China and Canada — including Han Xu and Lian Xia as joint first authors — report a surprising hoarding strategy employed by certain Chinese populations of flying squirrels (Xu et al., 2023).

Xu et al. accidentally discovered this caching behaviour when conducting fieldwork in Hainan Island, China. Amongst the shrubs and saplings of the Hainan Tropical Rainforest National Park, they began to notice that many nuts from Cyclobalanopsis trees were ‘stuck’ within the fork present between diverging twigs. The smooth, oval-shaped nuts showed a spiral groove around their middle section, as well as chew marks of more varied depths towards their bottom end.

Intrigued, the team conducted three systematic field searches and set trail cameras to record animal activity around the clock. After months of work, a few images and videos revealed that two species of flying squirrels, Hylopetes phayreielectilis and Hylopetes alboniger (commonly known as the Indochinese and the particolored flying squirrels), were responsible for making these grooves and affixing the nuts (Figure 1).

Food hoarding behaviour of two flying squirrel species, Hylopetes phayreielectilis and Hylopetes alboniger in Hainan Island, China.

Upon finding a nut from the Cyclobalanopsis tree, two out of the nine species of flying squirrels present in Hainan rainforests make a spiral groove around the middle of the nut (top), as well as grooves at its bottom (bottom), before fixing it in the fork of twigs. This hoarding strategy may help to preserve the nuts for longer in this demanding, humid environment.

Importantly, this work suggests that the behaviour could represent an adaptive response to the demands of tropical forests. Tree squirrels face many challenges when it comes to hoarding their food, such as competitors that pilfer their stocks or environmental conditions that may degrade stored items. In response, they have developed a range of pre- and post-hoarding behaviours to minimise food loss (Robin and Jacobs, 2022). For example, to stop seeds and nuts from sprouting, American red squirrels store cones away from the soil in their middens while Eastern grey squirrels remove the structures responsible for germination from their acorns prior to hiding them (Steele et al., 2001; Vander Wall, 1990). Xu et al. noted that Cyclobalanopsis nuts would remain intact for several months once lodged within forks, whereas they germinate in two to three months when on the ground of tropical rainforests (Zhou, 2001).

Arguably, suspended nuts are there for all to see; this can result in thieving, a major social challenge that can lead hoarders to lose 30% of their food per day (Vander Wall, 1990; Vander Wall and Jenkins, 2003). The team recorded several types of behaviour that could help to minimize pilferage, including the animals often storing their nuts away from the tree where they had found them. Caches were also regularly visited by squirrels – a habit that could allow them to relocate items away from compromised locations, for example. Without knowing for certain that the same animal was both storing and checking on a particular nut, however, it is difficult to confirm that this indeed a post-hoarding strategy against thieves. Specifically identifying individuals in future field studies could help to better understand the factors that influence decision-making and behaviour before and after hoarding.

Many species of squirrel play a crucial role for forests, yet flying squirrels have often received less attention (Koprowski and Nandini, 2008). The findings by Xu et al. should renew interest in these rodents, as well as open new avenues of research in behavioural ecology and animal cognition. For example, tree squirrels are usually active during the day and recover their hoarded food using spatial memory alongside olfactory or visual cues; it is unclear if flying squirrels, which are active during the night, use these strategies as well (Gould, 2017; Li et al., 2018). It would also be interesting to investigate whether regularly visiting hoards serves as memory reinforcement to facilitate future recovery, as it is the case for other scatter-hoarding rodents such as agoutis (Hirsch et al., 2013). And finally, an exciting question remains unanswered: does grooving and affixing the nuts rely on trial-and-error and predisposed innate behaviours, or does it require planning and other advanced cognitive processes? More longitudinal observations and systemic experiments in the field will be needed to reveal the intricacies of this behaviour, and how it interacts with the demands of the environment.

References

  1. Book
    1. Gould KL
    (2017) Spatial memory in food-hoarding animals
    In: John HB, editors. Learning and Memory: A Comprehensive Reference. Academic Press. pp. 285–307.
    https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-809324-5.21016-X
    1. Koprowski JL
    2. Nandini R
    (2008)
    Global hotspots and knowledge gaps for tree and flying squirrels
    Current Sciences 95:851–856.
  2. Book
    1. Vander Wall SB
    (1990)
    Food Hoarding in Animals
    University of Chicago Press.
  3. Book
    1. Zhou T
    (2001)
    Cultivation Techniques of Main Tropical Economic Trees in China
    Chinese Forestry Publishing House.

Article and author information

Author details

  1. Pizza Ka Yee Chow

    Pizza Ka Yee Chow is in the School of Psychology at the University of Chester, Chester, United Kingdom

    For correspondence
    p.chow@chester.ac.uk
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0002-8208-592X

Publication history

  1. Version of Record published:

Copyright

© 2023, Chow

This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.

Metrics

  • 551
    views
  • 33
    downloads
  • 0
    citations

Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.

Download links

A two-part list of links to download the article, or parts of the article, in various formats.

Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)

Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)

Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)

  1. Pizza Ka Yee Chow
(2023)
Behavior: Flying squirrels, hidden treasures
eLife 12:e89823.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.89823

Further reading

    1. Ecology
    2. Microbiology and Infectious Disease
    Benedikt M Mortzfeld, Shakti K Bhattarai, Vanni Bucci
    Short Report

    Interspecies interactions involving direct competition via bacteriocin production play a vital role in shaping ecological dynamics within microbial ecosystems. For instance, the ribosomally produced siderophore bacteriocins, known as class IIb microcins, affect the colonization of host-associated pathogenic Enterobacteriaceae species. Notably, to date, only five of these antimicrobials have been identified, all derived from specific Escherichia coli and Klebsiella pneumoniae strains. We hypothesized that class IIb microcin production extends beyond these specific compounds and organisms. With a customized informatics-driven approach, screening bacterial genomes in public databases with BLAST and manual curation, we have discovered 12 previously unknown class IIb microcins in seven additional Enterobacteriaceae species, encompassing phytopathogens and environmental isolates. We introduce three novel clades of microcins (MccW, MccX, and MccZ), while also identifying eight new variants of the five known class IIb microcins. To validate their antimicrobial potential, we heterologously expressed these microcins in E. coli and demonstrated efficacy against a variety of bacterial isolates, including plant pathogens from the genera Brenneria, Gibbsiella, and Rahnella. Two newly discovered microcins exhibit activity against Gram-negative ESKAPE pathogens, i.e., Acinetobacter baumannii or Pseudomonas aeruginosa, providing the first evidence that class IIb microcins can target bacteria outside of the Enterobacteriaceae family. This study underscores that class IIb microcin genes are more prevalent in the microbial world than previously recognized and that synthetic hybrid microcins can be a viable tool to target clinically relevant drug-resistant pathogens. Our findings hold significant promise for the development of innovative engineered live biotherapeutic products tailored to combat these resilient bacteria.

    1. Ecology
    Cody A Freas, Ajay Narenda ... Ken Cheng
    Research Article

    For the first time in any animal, we show that nocturnal bull ants use the exceedingly dim polarisation pattern produced by the moon for overnight navigation. The sun or moon can provide directional information via their position; however, they can often be obstructed by clouds, canopy, or the horizon. Despite being hidden, these bodies can still provide compass information through the polarised light pattern they produce/reflect. Sunlight produces polarised light patterns across the overhead sky as it enters the atmosphere, and solar polarised light is a well-known compass cue for navigating animals. Moonlight produces an analogous pattern, albeit a million times dimmer than sunlight. Here, we show evidence that polarised moonlight forms part of the celestial compass of navigating nocturnal ants. Nocturnal bull ants leave their nest at twilight and rely heavily on the overhead solar polarisation pattern to navigate. Yet many foragers return home overnight when the sun cannot guide them. We demonstrate that these bull ants use polarised moonlight to navigate home during the night, by rotating the overhead polarisation pattern above homing ants, who alter their headings in response. Furthermore, these ants can detect this cue throughout the lunar month, even under crescent moons, when polarised light levels are at their lowest. Finally, we show the long-term incorporation of this moonlight pattern into the ants’ path integration system throughout the night for homing, as polarised sunlight is incorporated throughout the day.