Abstract
The ability to perceive biological motion (BM) is crucial for human survival and social interaction. Plentiful studies have found impaired BM perception in autism spectrum disorder characterized by deficits in social interaction. Children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) often exhibit similar deficits in social interaction, but few studies have investigated BM perception in ADHD. Here, we compared the differences in abilities to process local kinematic and global configurational cues, two fundamental abilities of BM perception, between typically developing (TD) and ADHD children. Then, we further investigated the relationship between BM perception and social interaction skills measured by the Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS) and examined the contributions of potential factors (e.g., gender, age, attention and intelligence) to BM perception. Results revealed that children with ADHD exhibited atypical BM perception with a clear dissociation between local and global BM information processing. Local BM processing ability was largely related to social interaction skills, whereas global BM processing ability would significantly improve with age. Critically, general BM perception (i.e., both local and global BM cues) was affected by sustained attention ability in ADHD. This relationship was mainly mediated by Reasoning Intelligence. These findings elucidate the atypical biological motion perception in ADHD and the potential factors related to BM perception. Moreover, this study provides new evidence for local BM perception as a hallmark of social cognition and advances the comprehensive understanding of the distinct roles of local and global processing in BM perception and social cognitive disorders.
Introduction
Attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a common developmental disorder. The prevalence of ADHD in children and adolescents is between 2% and 7%, with an average of around 5%1. Besides the well-established core symptoms of ADHD (including a deficit of sustained attention, hyperactivity, and impulsivity), some ASD characteristics, such as dysfunctions in social communication and social interaction, have also been frequently reported in ADHD children2–4. However, experimental studies of social cognition in ADHD are still scarce. Some studies described worse performance in tasks of social cognition. Among them, impaired theory of mind (ToM) and emotion recognition are most frequently reported5–7. It is difficult for children with ADHD to recognize others’ emotions and purposes. Other social cognition processes in ADHD are unclear. Further exploring diversified social cognition (e.g., biological motion perception) will provide a new understanding of impaired social function in ADHD. Moreover, recent studies revealed that the social cognition in ADHD can potentially vary with different factors at the cognitive, pathological, or developmental levels, such as general cognitive impairment5, the severity of symptoms8, or age5. Yet, how these factors relate to the dysfunctions of social cognition in ADHD is still in its infancy. Bridging the gap is important because this knowledge can help depict the developmental trajectory of social cognition and find effective interventions for impaired social interaction in ADHD.
Biological motion (BM), namely the movement of a living creature, carries much information beyond bodily movements9, such as intention10, emotion11, gender12, and identity13, 14. Since point-light displays (PLD) technology was used to depict human motions15, it is possible to separate biological motion from other characteristics like shape and color. Considering the seminal impact on cognitive science, developmental and clinical neuroscience, BM perception has drawn significant attention from scientists. Some researchers attempted to deconstruct BM processing into more specific components. Our study concentrated on two fundamental abilities to process BM cues (Figure 1): the abilities to process local BM cues derived from major joint motion tracks and global BM cues of human configuration. Separable neural signals revealed two independent processing stages16–18. The ability to process local BM cues is genetically highly preserved19; for example, newly hatched chicks without visual experience exhibit a spontaneous preference for BM20. A similar finding is also reported in 2-day-old babies21. Local BM cues not only carry locomotive direction22 but also aid in detecting life in the visual environment23 without observers’ explicit recognition or attention24–27. Moreover, the processing of local BM cues is unaffected by attention, relatively robust to masking noise and does not show a learning trend27, 28. In contrast, global BM processing involves top-down processing, and attention plays a critical role in global BM perception28, 29. Dispersed attention adversely affects its performance. Compared with local BM processing, global BM processing is susceptible to learning and is heavily hindered by increasing mask densities28. In recent years, BM perception has received wide attention in studies of mental disorders30, particularly in ASD, characterized by deficits in social communication and social interaction31, 32. An important reason is that BM perception is considered a hallmark of social cognition. Individuals with deficits in BM processing exhibit worse social perception in daily life10. Especially in local BM processing, it was correlated with autistic traits, especially in the subdimension of social communication19. Therefore, it is essential to focus on revealing differences in BM processing between typical and atypical development, which will enhance our comprehension of social dysfunction in atypical development.
Few studies focused on BM perception in children with ADHD, compared with numerous studies of impaired BM perception in ASD. An EEG study found neuro-electrophysiological changes in processing BM stimuli in ADHD33, whereas no behavioral difference existed, which could be improved by more carefully designed BM stimuli. Another study found that children with ADHD performed worse in BM detection with moderate ratios of noise34. Nevertheless, the accuracy of TD groups was also very low (i.e., poor performance), which indicated the inferior applicability of this paradigm. In short, factors such as inapplicable experimental designs may influence the precision and reliability of the results. Despite some preliminary hints, systematic evaluation of BM perception in ADHD is warranted.
Therefore, the current study aims to examine BM perception abilities in children with ADHD. The first question to be addressed by this study is whether the BM perception abilities differ between TD and ADHD children. As mentioned above, BM perception is related to social interaction and has two fundamental components. Local BM processing ability is genetically highly preserved, while global BM processing ability shows a learning trend and needs the involvement of attention. ADHD is a neurodevelopmental disorder characterized by attention deficit and mostly accompanied by impaired social interaction. Therefore, the second question is whether BM perception is associated with social interaction ability in ADHD, and if so, which factors (e.g., attention) could affect BM perception in ADHD. To answer these questions, we utilized a stable and ingenious behavioral paradigm used in a previous study and made minor modifications to fit it to children19. This paradigm combined an intact and a scrambled point-lighted walker with a noise mask to measure local and global BM information processing abilities. Using this paradigm, in Experiment 1, we compared the performance in processing two different BM cues (i.e., local and global BM cues) as well as their combination (i.e., both local and global cues) between TD and ADHD groups. Then, in Experiment 2, we collected the social interaction trait of participants to examine its correlation with the performance of BM tasks. Finally, we investigated the contributing factors of BM perception in ADHD and explored their potential relations.
Materials and methods
Participants
We recruited a total of one hundred and seventeen children with and without ADHD to enter this study. Eighty-one children met the ADHD diagnostic criteria of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5)35. The clinical diagnosis was first made by an experienced child and adolescent psychiatrist in the Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Outpatient Department of the Peking University Sixth Hospital, based on the ADHD Rating Scale. The Chinese version of the kiddie-schedule for affective disorders and schizophrenia-present and lifetime version DSM-5 (K-SADS-PL-C DSM-5)36, 37, a semi-structured interview instrument, was then implemented to confirm the diagnosis. Thirty-six typically developing (TD) children were from ordinary primary schools in Beijing and screened out the presence of ADHD, ASD, affective disorders and behavioral disorders by a trained psychiatrist. All subjects of ADHD groups have full-scale IQ above 75 (5th upper percentile) on Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Fourth Edition, and all TD children had a full-scale IQ that fell above the 5th percentile on the Raven’s Standard Progressive Matrices38 which is used in measuring reasoning ability and regarded as a non-verbal estimate of intelligence. Exclusion criteria for both groups included: (a) neurological diseases; (b) other neurodevelopmental disorders (e.g., ASD, Mental retardation, and tic disorders), affective disorders and schizophrenia; (c) that would impact experiments completion; (d) taking psychotropic drugs or stimulants in 30 days. (e) previous head trauma or neurosurgery.
36 TD children (age = 9.09 ± 2.18, 14 male) and 39 children with ADHD (age = 9.88 ± 2.23, 28 male) participated in Experiment 1. Groups did not differ by age (t = –1.550, p = .126) but differed in sex (χ2 = 8.964, p = 0.004). 42 ADHD children (age = 9.34 ± 1.89, 27 male) participated in Experiment 2. They were naïve to the task and did not participate in Experiment 1. Participant demographic characteristics are shown in Table 1. All individuals of each group had a normal or corrected-to-normal vision and were naïve to the objectives of experiments. Written informed consent was obtained from the parents of all children before testing. The Institutional Review Board of the Peking University Sixth Hospital has approved this study.
Assessment
K-SADS-PL-C DSM-5
K-SADS-PL-C DSM-5 is a semi-structured interview instrument for evaluating mental disorders in children and adolescents aged 6–18 years36. It involved thirty-five diagnoses based on the diagnostic criteria of DSM-5. A trained psychiatrist concluded the diagnosis by interviewing the parents and child. Chinese version showed great psychometric criterion37.
ADHD Rating Scale
ADHD Rating Scale is adapted from the ADHD diagnostic criteria of DSM39, which requires parents or teachers to complete the scale independently. Its Chinese version has an excellent psychometric criterion and consists of 2 subscales40: inattention (IA, nine items) and hyperactivity-impulsivity (HI, nine items). Each item is rated on a four-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (the symptom is “never or rarely”) to 4 (the symptom is “very often”). The final results will create three scores: (1) IA dimension score, (2) HI dimension score, and (3) Total score. Higher scores indicate more severe ADHD symptoms.
Social Responsiveness Scale
Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS) is a widely used quantitative measure with 65 items to assess the severity of social impairment in many mental disorders41, and the psychometric properties of the Chinese version are desirable42. It includes five subdimensions: social awareness, social cognition, social communication, social motivation, and autistic mannerisms. Each item is rated on a scale from 0 (never true) to 4 (almost always true), with higher scores indicating worse social ability.
Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Fourth Edition
Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Fourth Edition (WISC-IV) is widely used to test comprehensive intelligence in individuals aged 6–18. It contains fifteen subtests which comprise four broad areas of intellectual functioning: Verbal Comprehension, Perceptual Reasoning, Working Memory, and Processing Speed. Scores of the four broad areas constitute the full-scale intellectual quotient (FIQ).
QB Test
QB Test is a 15-min Continuous Performance Test (CPT) for assessing inattention and impulsivity with a high-resolution infrared camera monitoring participant’s activity43. Previous psychometric studies have validated its good measurement properties44. After the test was completed, several Q scores were calculated to summarize participant performance. The Q scores were standardized based on normative data matching gender and age. The higher Q score implies more abnormal performance. In this study, we focused on the QbInattention, the Q score responsible for sustained attention, particularly when children focused on tasks.
Stimuli
Stimuli were presented on a 14-inch monitor using MATLAB together with the PsychToolbox extensions. All subjects completed experiments in a dim-lit room with heads on the chinrest to ensure their eyes were 50 cm away from the monitor. Point-light BM stimuli sequences adopted in this study have been used in previous studies45, which were derived from configurations of individual walking motion on a treadmill and did not contain an overall translation. Each frame of BM sequences consisted of 13 white dots representing the human head and major joints and was displayed on a grey background. Each walking cycle lasted 1s with 30 frames. For each trial, the initial frame of BM sequences was randomized. The whole point-lighted BM walker presented about 5.7 ° visual angle vertically.
Procedure
In Experiment 1, 36 TD children and 39 children with ADHD were required to complete three tasks that were similar to but slightly modified from the versions implemented in our previous study19 (Figure 2). Each trial began with a fixation cross (0.6° * 0.6°). After a random 1200-1500 ms interval, the monitor displayed a BM sequence specific to each task, lasting 2 s (60 frames). Task 1 (BM-Local) assessed the ability to process local BM cues. 13 dots were randomly relocated within the range of intact BM walker, which rendered global configuration destroyed and local kinematics retained instead. After the display, we required children to press a left or right button to indicate the motion direction of the unidentified creature (i.e., scrambled BM walker) as accurately as possible. Children received no feedback on whether a right or wrong answer was given. There were 30 trials in total and 15 trials for each condition (left and right). Task 2 (BM-Global) tested the ability to process the global configuration cues of the BM walker. Children needed to judge whether there was an intact walker in a mask. Each trial contained a mask consisting of two scrambled target walkers (26 dots) within the boundary about 1.44 times the intact walker. And the scrambled or intact version of the target walker was randomly embedded into the mask. This procedure extracted global configuration cues from the original BM sequence because two conditions (i.e., scrambled and intact walker) contained the same information of local kinematics as the mask, and the local motion cues were hence uninformative. The correct choice asked for using the global component. To avoid learning the shape of the walker24, we set target walkers possibly facing one of five equally spaced directions from left to right. There were 30 trials composed of two conditions (intact or scrambled target). In Task 3 (BM-General), the intact target walker facing left or right was embedded into the mask. The mask is similar to Task 2. Children were required to judge the facing direction of the target walker (left or right). Because the mask was set to covey the local BM cues consistent with the target walker, children could perform the task using general (i.e., local + global) BM information. Task 3 was composed of 30 trials (15 for each facing directions). Other parameters of Task 2 and 3 were similar to Task 1. Before each task, children practiced five trials to make sure good understanding. We performed three tasks in a fixed order so that participants were naïve to the scrambled walker and only made choices using local BM cues in Task 1.
In Experiment 2, 42 children with ADHD completed the same procedures as in Experiment 1. Beyond that, their parents should fill in the Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS) to assess social interaction.
Statistics
We carried out ANOVA to examine the difference in BM perception abilities between TD and ADHD children and the Pearson correlation analysis to assess the relationship between the accuracy of each task and the SRS score. General linear models and path analysis were used to explore the roles of potential factors on BM perception. A p value < 0.05 was considered statistically significant.
Results
Children with ADHD exhibit atypical BM perception
Figure 3 shows the mean accuracies (ACC) for both groups in the three tasks in Experiment 1. We tested the difference in ACC between the TD and ADHD groups in each task using one-way ANOVA (Gender as a covariate). In Task 1, the group comparison revealed a significant difference (F1,72 = 5.630, p = 0.020, η2 = 0.073). Children are asked to judge the facing direction of the target walker using global BM cues (Task 2) or general BM cues combining global with local BM cues (Task 3). TD group have higher accuracies than the ADHD group (Task 2: F1,72 = 12.600, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.149; Task 3: F1,72 = 19.926, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.217). In all statistical analyses, the main effects of gender were not significant (all p > 0.05).
Atypical perception of local BM information predicted impaired social interaction in ADHD
Experiment 1 demonstrates that the abilities of BM perception were atypical in ADHD. Our previous study revealed that local BM processing ability is heritable and negatively correlated with autistic traits, especially in the subdimension of communication19. In addition, substantial evidence indicates that children with ADHD often have problems in social interaction. We expected that compromised social interaction in ADHD might be associated with atypical local BM processing. So, we recruited 42 naïve children with ADHD to participate in Experiment 2 to explore this relationship. Their parents or caregivers familiar with children completed SRS (total scores = 54.64 ± 18.42). Thirty-three TD children had SRS scores (total score = 38.64 ± 12.47). The total SRS scores in the TD groups are significantly lower than those in the ADHD group (t = –4.277, p < 0.001). We found that children with higher total score of SRS performed worse in Task 1, i.e., the ability of local BM processing was negatively correlated to SRS total scores (r = –0.264, p = 0.022). Notably, significant correlations were also identified between SRS total score and global BM processing and between SRS total score and general BM processing (global BM perception: r = –0.238, p = 0.039; general BM perception: r = –0.359, p = 0.002).
In the further subgroup analysis, as seen in Figure 4, the correlation between the SRS total score and the ability to process local cues was only found in the ADHD group (ADHD: r = –0.461, p = 0.002; TD: r = 0.109, p = 0.547), particularly on subscales of social awareness, social cognition, social communication, social motivation (see Table 2 for complete information). However, no correlation was found between SRS scores and global or general BM processing in either ADHD or TD groups (all p > 0.05).
Reasoning intelligence and attention regulated BM processing
Experiment 2 revealed that local BM processing instead of global BM processing was related to social interaction. Then, which factors would regulate the difference in processing global BM cues? Previous meta-analysis studies of BM perception in ASD found mixed results about the effect of age and IQ on processing BM31, 32. In addition, neural responses related to BM processing are strongly regulated by attention29. Did these factors play critical roles in BM perception in ADHD, a neurodevelopmental disorder characterized by attention deficit? To answer this question, we integrated data from the ADHD group in Experiment 1 and Experiment 2 (n = 80, one child did not complete QB test). Two linear models were built to investigate contributing factors: (a) BM-global = age + gender + FIQ + QbInattention; (b) BM-general = age + gender + FIQ + QbInattention + BM-local + BM-global. We screened factors with the largest contribution to the models by step-wise regression. For model (a), the ability to process global BM cues was enhanced with age (standardized β = 0.251, p = 0.025). For model (b), higher FIQ, particularly on subdimension Perceptual Reasoning (standardized β = 0.271, p = 0.005), and more excellent global BM processing (standardized β = 0.290, p = 0.004) predicted better performance in general BM processing. Furthermore, as children grew up, the ability to probe general BM information increased (standardized β = 0.365, p < 0.001). It is worth noting that QbInattention had a highly negative correlation with Perceptual Reasoning (r = –0.355, p = 0.001) and general BM perception (r = –0.246, p = 0.028). Given the risk of collinearity, we further used path analysis to visualize these relationships post hoc (Figure 5). The result showed sustained attention (i.e., QbInattention) did not predict performance in BM-General (i.e., Task 3) directly. However, a significant indirect path via the ability of Perceptual Reasoning was found. In addition, as children with ADHD grew up, their performance in BM-General (i.e., Task 3) not only improved directly but enhanced via better processing of global BM cues. Moreover, we built model (c) to corroborate that the local BM processing was stable with age and unaffected by attention and IQ: (c) BM-local = age + gender + FIQ + QbInattention. No regressor has remained after step-wise regression.
In addition, we further built three models to explore the effects of Reasoning IQ and age on BM perception in TD children: (a) BM-local = age + gender + FIQ. (b) BM-global = age + gender + FIQ; (c) BM-general = age + gender + FIQ + BM-local +BM-global. No regressor has remained after step-wise regression in model (a). For models (b) and (c), there are positive relations between age and performance (BM-global: standardized β = 0.396, p = 0.017; BM-general: standardized β = 0.330, p = 0.049).
Discussion
Our study contributes several promising findings on atypical biological motion perception in ADHD. Specifically, we found that children with ADHD performed worse in processing local, global and general BM information than TD children. Notably, there was a clear dissociation between local and global BM information processing. The ability to process local BM cues, which is highly heritable, could predict deficits of social interaction in ADHD. By contrast, global BM processing, which is largely shaped by experience, would significantly improve with age. Moreover, the results also unraveled the potential factors related to BM perception: Reasoning Intelligence played a mediating effect on the relationship between attention and general BM perception.
BM perception is a hot topic in the field of visual cognition. One critical reason is that BM naturally contains biological and social properties. BM processing has significant value in successful daily life, particularly in nonverbal communication11, 46 and adaptive behavior29, 47. As an early emerging ability, BM can be discriminated in the newborn baby21. In typical development children, there is a linkage between BM perception and social cognitive abilities48. For example, 12-month-old infants showed social behaviors (i.e., following gaze) elicited by BM displays49. It is, therefore, of the supreme importance of BM to children’s development of social cognition. This “social interpretation” of BM proposed that the ability to process BM may be taken as a measure of difficulties in social interaction19. An example is that poor performance of BM perception is a hallmark of impaired social function in children with ASD10, 50. That implies that atypical BM perception may contribute to the deficits of social interaction in ADHD. Albeit we found a correlation between the SRS total score and the accuracy of each of the tasks in all participants, only a significantly negative correlation between the SRS total score and the accuracy of processing local cues was found in ADHD group in the further subgroup analysis, which provides preliminary evidence for the hypothesis and demonstrates that the local BM processing can predict impaired social interaction in children with ADHD. This finding is consistent with previous studies that revealed a common genetic basis for local BM processing ability and social communication19. In the subgroup analysis, however, we did not find a correlation between social interaction and global BM processing, nor between social interaction and general BM processing. That is because the ability to process global BM cues is greatly affected by environment and general cognition functions19.
BM perception is considered a multi-level phenomenon51–53. At least in part, processing information of local BM and global BM are different16, 19. Our results also demonstrate such a dissociation. Local motion processing is stable with age and not susceptible to attention and IQ, which shows a phylogenetically highly preserved mechanism20, 21, 54, 55 and benefits of life detection22. On the contrary, a development tendency seems to exist in global BM processing that tends to be improved with age. A previous study indicated BM perception was enhanced with age in TD children56. This variation is also observed in children with ASD, and some developmental overlaps were found in the ability to process visual motion information57. Performance of BM processing within ASD becomes more aligned with TD children, and the discrepancy between the two groups alleviates with the increase in age32. With regard to ADHD, deficits of social cognition are likely to improve with age in most individuals 5. Our study further verified this point in BM perception. It is worth noting that the ability of global BM processing positively correlated with the performance of processing general BM cues, i.e., children with ADHD are apt to catch global BM cues to judge the facing direction of the walker when displayed with local and global BM cues, which correspond to a hierarchical model51. Once a living creature is detected, an agent (i.e., is it a human?) can be recognized by a coherent, articulated body structure perceptually organized from its motions (i.e., local BM cues)58, which is a top-down processing and probably requires attention28, 59, especially in the presence of competing information29. In addition, cortical processing of BM is strongly modulated by attention60. Therefore, as a disorder characterized by deficits of sustained attention, we found its severity of inattention was negatively related to the performance of BM processing.
Interestingly, a compensation strategy was found in children with impaired BM perception. No difficulty of BM recognition was found in ASD individuals with high IQ61, but children with ASD showed weaker adaptation effects for biological motion than TD children62. One possibility is that individuals with high IQ and impaired BM perception can better create or reason reliable strategies used for BM recognition63, 64. The current study supported this assumption. Children with higher IQ, particularly Perceptual Reasoning, have better performance. Because of the effect of attention deficits on Perceptual Reasoning, the performance within ADHD will not become aligned with that in TD children.
Overall, our study first revealed two atypical fundamental abilities underlying BM perception in children with ADHD, which showed a clear dissociation. Significantly, anomalous local BM processing predicts impaired social interaction in ADHD. Moreover, these results unraveled the potential contributions of age, IQ and attention to processing BM information. These findings also shed new light on the direction of future studies, which should explore the performance of more advanced BM processing in children with ADHD, such as emotion and identity recognition in BM tasks. In addition, it is necessary to delineate the profiles for neural processing of biological motion in ADHD. A comparative study is warranted to investigate differences between ADHD and ASD, which is essential for finding the common neuropsychological traits and biomarkers of social cognition impairments.
Acknowledgements
This research was supported by grants from the Beijing Municipal Science and Technology Commission (Z181100001518005), the Ministry of Science and Technology of China (2021ZD0203800), the National Natural Science Foundation of China (31830037), the Interdisciplinary Innovation Team (JCTD-2021-06), and Fundamental Research Funds for the Central Universities.
Declaration of interests
The authors declare no competing interest.
Ethics
All procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008. The Institutional Review Board of the Peking University Sixth Hospital has approved this study (reference number for approval: (2020) Ethics Review No.9).
Date availability
The data analyzed during the study is available after publication at https://osf.io/37p5s/
References
- 1ADHD in children and young people: prevalence, care pathways, and service provisionLancet Psychiatry 5:175–186https://doi.org/10.1016/s2215-0366(17)30167-0
- 2Examining autistic traits in children with ADHD: does the autism spectrum extend to ADHD?J Autism Dev Disord 41:1178–1191https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-010-1135-3
- 3Autism symptoms in Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: a familial trait which correlates with conduct, oppositional defiant, language and motor disordersJ Autism Dev Disord 39:197–209https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-008-0621-3
- 4Autistic traits in a population-based ADHD twin sampleJ Child Psychol Psychiatry 48:464–472https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01720.x
- 5Meta-analysis of social cognition in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): comparison with healthy controls and autistic spectrum disorderPsychol Med 46:699–716https://doi.org/10.1017/s0033291715002573
- 6Reading mind from the eyes in individuals with attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): A meta-analysisExpert Rev Neurother 22:889–896https://doi.org/10.1080/14737175.2022.2151899
- 7Social cognition in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)Neurosci Biobehav Rev 34:734–743https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.10.009
- 8Social difficulties in adolescent attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Social motivation, social anxiety and symptom severity as contributing factorsJ Clin Psychol 79:1113–1129https://doi.org/10.1002/jclp.23462
- 9Action categories and the perception of biological motionPerception 22:15–22https://doi.org/10.1068/p220015
- 10Biological Motion Processing as a Hallmark of Social CognitionCerebral Cortex 22:981–995https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhr156
- 11The perception of emotion from body movement in point-light displays of interpersonal dialoguePerception 34:1171–1180https://doi.org/10.1068/p5203
- 12Recognizing the sex of a walker from a dynamic point-light displayPerception & Psychophysics 21:575–580https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03198740
- 13Self recognition versus recognition of others by biological motion: viewpoint-dependent effectsPerception 35:911–920https://doi.org/10.1068/p5540
- 14Recognizing people from their movementJ Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 31:210–220https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.31.1.210
- 15Visual perception of biological motion and a model for its analysisPerception & Psychophysics 14:201–211https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03212378
- 16A two-stage framework for neural processing of biological motionNeuroImage 259https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2022.119403
- 17Human functional magnetic resonance imaging reveals separation and integration of shape and motion cues in biological motion processingJ Neurosci 29:7315–7329https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.4870-08.2009
- 18Distinct neural mechanisms for body form and body motion discriminationsJ Neurosci 34:574–585https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.4032-13.2014
- 19Heritable aspects of biological motion perception and its covariation with autistic traitsProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 115:1937–1942https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1714655115
- 20Visually inexperienced chicks exhibit spontaneous preference for biological motion patternsPLoS Biol 3https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.0030208
- 21A predisposition for biological motion in the newborn babyProc Natl Acad Sci U S A 105:809–813https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0707021105
- 22The inversion effect in biological motion perception: evidence for a “life detector”?Curr Biol 16:821–824https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2006.03.022
- 23Perception of animacy and direction from local biological motion signalsJ Vis 8:1–10https://doi.org/10.1167/8.5.3
- 24Characterizing global and local mechanisms in biological motion perceptionJ Vis 9:1–10https://doi.org/10.1167/9.5.8
- 25Global Processing of Biological MotionsPsychological Science 5:221–225https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1994.tb00504.x
- 26Searching for life motion signals. Visual search asymmetry in local but not global biological-motion processingPsychol Sci 21:1083–1089https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797610376072
- 27Acceleration carries the local inversion effect in biological motion perceptionJournal of Vision 9:19–19https://doi.org/10.1167/9.1.19
- 28Active versus passive processing of biological motionPerception 31:837–853https://doi.org/10.1068/p3072
- 29Attention, biological motion, and action recognitionNeuroimage 59:4–13https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.05.044
- 30Deficient biological motion perception in schizophrenia: results from a motion noise paradigmFrontiers in Psychology 4https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00391
- 31Anomalous Perception of Biological Motion in Autism: A Conceptual Review and Meta-AnalysisScientific Reports 10https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-61252-3
- 32Biological motion perception in autism spectrum disorder: a meta-analysisMolecular Autism 10https://doi.org/10.1186/s13229-019-0299-8
- 33Visual Processing of Biological Motion in Children and Adolescents with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: An Event Related Potential-StudyPLOS ONE 9https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0088585
- 34Deficits in Working Memory and Theory of Mind May Underlie Difficulties in Social Perception of Children with ADHDNeurology Research International 2021 3793750https://doi.org/10.1155/2021/3793750
- 35Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5American psychiatric association Washington, DC
- 36K-SADS-PL DSM-5 November 2016Advanced Center for Intervention and Services Research (ACISR) for Early Onset Mood and Anxiety Disorders-Western Psychiatric Institute and Clinic Child and Adolescent Research and Education (CARE) Program Yale University
- 37Reliability and validity of the Chinese version of the kiddie-schedule for affective disorders and schizophrenia-present and lifetime version DSM-5 (K-SADS-PL-C DSM-5)Journal of Affective Disorders 317:72–78https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2022.08.062
- 38Raven’s Standard Progressive Matrices: new school age norms and a study of the test’s validityPersonality and Individual Differences 34:375–386
- 39Assessing culturally different students for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder using behavior rating scalesJ Abnorm Child Psychol 26:187–198https://doi.org/10.1023/a:1022620217886
- 40Parent Ratings of ADHD Symptoms in Chinese Urban Schoolchildren: Assessment With the Chinese ADHD Rating Scale-IV: Home VersionJ Atten Disord 19:1022–1033https://doi.org/10.1177/1087054712461177
- 41Social responsive scale (SRS) manualLos Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services
- 42Investigating the validation of the Chinese Mandarin version of the Social Responsiveness Scale in a Mainland China child populationBMC Psychiatry 17https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-016-1185-y
- 43QbTest plus technical manualSweden: Gothenburg
- 44ADHD and the QbTest: Diagnostic Validity of QbTestJournal of Attention Disorders 22:1074–1080https://doi.org/10.1177/1087054715595697
- 45Perception of biological motion: a stimulus set of human point-light actionsBehav Res Methods Instrum Comput 36:625–629https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03206542
- 46Perception of emotion from dynamic point-light displays represented in dancePerception 25:727–738https://doi.org/10.1068/p250727
- 47Gender recognition from point-light walkersJ Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 31:1247–1265https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.31.6.1247
- 48Individual differences in changes in infants’ interest in social signals in relation to developmental indexInfant Behav Dev 32:381–391https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2009.06.004
- 49Biological motion displays elicit social behavior in 12-month-oldsChild Dev 80:1069–1075https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01317.x
- 50Disrupted action perception in autism: behavioral evidence, neuroendophenotypes, and diagnostic utilityDev Cogn Neurosci 2:25–35https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2011.05.005
- 51Biological motion perceptionThe senses: A comprehensive reference 2:231–238
- 52Shape-independent processing of biological motionPeople watching: Social, perceptual, and neurophysiological studies of body perception :82–100
- 53What is biological motion?Definition, stimuli and paradigms. Social perception: Detection and interpretation of animacy, agency, and intention :13–36
- 54Filial responses as predisposed and learned preferences: Early attachment in chicks and babiesBehav Brain Res 325:90–104https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2016.09.018
- 55Bottlenosed dolphin and human recognition of veridical and degraded video displays of an artificial gestural languageJ Exp Psychol Gen 119:215–230https://doi.org/10.1037//0096-3445.119.2.215
- 56Biological Motion Perception Is Affected by Age and Cognitive Style in Children Aged 8–15Neurology Research International 2015 594042https://doi.org/10.1155/2015/594042
- 57Development of motion processing in children with autismDev Sci 13:826–838https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00939.x
- 58Principles of categorizationMorgan Kaufmann
- 59Attention-based visual routines: spritesCognition 80:47–60https://doi.org/10.1016/s0010-0277(00)00153-0
- 60Object-based attentional modulation of biological motion processing: Spatiotemporal dynamics using functional magnetic resonance imaging and electroencephalographyJournal of Neuroscience 30:9064–9073https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1779-10.2010
- 61IQ Predicts Biological Motion Perception in Autism Spectrum DisordersJournal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 42:557–565https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-011-1267-0
- 62Intact recognition, but attenuated adaptation, for biological motion in youth with autism spectrum disorderAutism Res 9:1103–1113https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.1595
- 63The psychophysics of visual motion and global form processing in autismBrain 133:599–610https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awp272
- 64Impaired recognition of emotions from body movements is associated with elevated motion coherence thresholds in autism spectrum disordersNeuropsychologia 47:3023–3029https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.05.019
Article and author information
Author information
Version history
Copyright
© 2023, Tian et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
- views
- 832
- downloads
- 137
- citation
- 1
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.