Nucleus reuniens of the thalamus contains head direction cells

Abstract

Discrete populations of brain cells signal heading direction, rather like a compass. These ‘head direction’ cells are largely confined to a closely-connected network of sites. We describe, for the first time, a population of head direction cells in nucleus reuniens of the thalamus in the freely-moving rat. This novel subcortical head direction signal potentially modulates the hippocampal CA fields directly and, thus, informs spatial processing and memory.

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.03075.001

eLife digest

Whether it is foraging for food or finding its way back to its nest, an animal often needs to know which direction it is heading in. Some neurons in a mammal’s brain have been shown to act like a compass, and send out nerve impulses whenever the animal points its head in a certain direction. For example, some of these neurons will fire when the animal faces northeast, but not when it faces northwest, and vice versa.

Importantly these neurons, called ‘head direction’ cells, do not actually measure the Earth's magnetic field. Rather, they respond to information about landmarks in the environment and the animal's movements of its head or body to work out which way the animal is facing.

Head direction cells are largely found in a closely-connected network of a few sites in the brain. However, Jankowski et al. have now discovered more of these cells in another region found deep within the centre of the brain. Measuring the nerve impulses from these neurons in rats that were moving freely around a test arena revealed that the neurons fired in exactly the same way as some other head direction cells in other regions of the brain. For example, they fired whenever the rat faced one direction, but stopped firing when it turned its head to face another.

Jankowski et al. showed that the head direction cells in this region of the brain continued to work when the lights were turned off in the test arena, or when the shape of the arena was changed from a circle to a square. These neurons began sending information about head direction as soon as the rat entered the test arena, and many continued to fire when the rat faced the same direction even when they were retested on several different days.

The head direction cells discovered by Jankowski et al. are connected to another region of the brain that is involved in remembering different locations in the environment and navigating between them. This suggests that these neurons might provide some of the information required to carry out these tasks. It also means that areas of the brain close to those that receive input from the outside world may perform more complex cognitive functions than previously thought.

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.03075.002

Introduction

Nucleus reuniens (NRe), one of the largest midline thalamic nuclei, receives extensive limbic inputs and provides a bridge linking the hippocampus (especially area CA1) with medial prefrontal cortex (McKenna and Vertes, 2004; Vertes, 2006; Prasad and Chudasama, 2013). Its functions are not well-understood, but it has been suggested that, via these connections, NRe influences memory consolidation for spatial learning and generalisation of fear conditioning (Eleore et al., 2011; Loureiro et al., 2012; Xu and Sudhof, 2013). To elucidate its functions open-field single-unit recordings in NRe of freely-moving rats were made (Mink et al., 1983). We have found an unexpected population of NRe cells signalling head direction (HD) in the horizontal plane, independent of location within the test arena. These cells resemble HD cells in the anterodorsal and anteroventral thalamic nuclei (Shinder and Taube, 2011; Tsanov et al., 2011), the lateral mammillary nucleus (Taube, 2007), and certain parahippocampal regions (Cassel et al., 2013). NRe cells maintain head directionality during light–dark transitions, and in environments of different shape. These cells establish directionality rapidly upon first entering an environment. NRe has not, to date, been part of the traditional HD circuit, which largely originates in the dorsal tegmental nucleus of Gudden and the lateral mammillary nucleus (Taube, 2007; Cassel et al., 2013). Subsequent processing is via the anterodorsal thalamic nucleus, dorsal presubiculum, entorhinal cortex and to the hippocampus (Su and Bentivoglio, 1990; Vertes et al., 2007). Our findings, therefore, reveal a novel head direction signal potentially modulating the hippocampal CA fields and, therefore, hippocampal spatial processing (Brandon et al., 2013).

Results

Changing visual conditions from light to dark to light does not affect HD cells

Lighting conditions were systematically varied across foraging sessions for 10 cells. The animal foraged during light-dark-light sessions (each 20 min). Light removal did not affect NRe head directional activity (Figure 1).

Head direction cells recorded in the nucleus reuniens.

(A) 18 representative head direction (HD) cells in nucleus reuniens (NRe); (B) NRe location on a coronal (left) and corresponding sagittal (right) rat brain section (adapted from Paxinos and Watson, 2005); (C) representative histological specimen showing electrode track (left); recording positions corresponding to cell locations presented in panel a (upper right inset) showing location of NRe and detailed atlas (lower right inset); (D) representative recordings showing multi-day stability of HD cells: a representative cell recorded on each day of 16 days (multiple transitions from light-dark-light, and environmental transformations from circle to square to circle). The solid line is the mean spike waveform and dashed lines are M ± SD of the spike waveform. The green outline shows predicted firing rates given the proportion of time the animal spent looking in each direction, calculated according to the distributive hypothesis.

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.03075.003

Arena shape does not affect HD cells

We transformed arena shape (circle-square-circle; Figure 1D; circle-square; Figure 2A,D). There was no effect of environmental shape changes on any HD cells (n = 10).

Nucleus reuniens head direction cells do not appear to be spatially modulated and are present from first exposure to the environment.

(A and D) Representative waveforms for two units showing, (top to bottom), light-dark-light and circle–square transitions; from left to right, autocorrelation histogram, polar plots, tuning curves for clockwise (CW) vs counter-clockwise (CCW) head movements; (B) spatial analysis for cardinal orientations showing no effect of spatial position on unit activity; (C) temporal evolution of HD firing for cumulative samples (time ranges: 0–1, 0–2, 0–4, 0–8, 0–16 and 0–20 min) and independent time-binned samples (0–1, 1–2, 2–4, 4–8, 8–16 and 16–20 min) demonstrating that HD activity is present in the first minute of exposure to the arena; (D, E, F) as (A, B, C). Red lines are formed by continuous firing activity when the rat walks with its head directed in the preferred HD. The firing map represents −22.5° to +22.5°; as the ring was formed by eight plots each representing a 45° extent of head direction.

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.03075.004

Transitions between light and dark, arena shape, mean head direction, mean head direction for clockwise and counter-clockwise movement, peak head direction and peak firing rate were compared using t-test for paired two samples for means with Bonferroni correction. No significant differences were observed between conditions.

HD cells do not remap across days

22 cells were recorded across at least 2 consecutive days (15 were recorded for three or more days, and three were stably recorded for 14 or more days. Initial head direction remained stable across days, even for extended recordings), indicating no effect of time or sleep/wake cycle on their preferred directionality.

Temporal development of HD cells

Figure 2C,F depict the temporal evolution of head directional firing for cumulative samples and independent time-binned samples, demonstrating head directional activity is present from the first minute of exposure to the environment (irrespective of environment shape).

Separation angle (clockwise vs counter-clockwise) is present

Separation angle (the offset in mean peak firing rate for clockwise vs counter-clockwise head movements) is present in about 50% of recorded HD cells. There were no significant differences between mean head direction measured in degrees for clockwise and counter-clockwise movement in the t test for paired two samples for means in the whole population of HD cells recorded in NRe.

Theta-cycle skipping cells do not carry a head-directional signal

Brandon et al. (Paxinos and Watson, 2005) recorded in medial entorhinal cortex (mEC) and found units firing in a fixed synchronous or anti-synchronous relationship with alternate theta cycles. We find a similar population of cells in NRe (see online material (OM) for detailed calculations and Figure 3A,B). These cells, which do not carry a HD signal, are found only in NRe, providing a physiological marker for electrode depth. The theta skipping index is always positive ([mean ± SD] 0.334 ± 0.127; jump factor: 0.367 ± 0.075; frequency ratio: of 2.00 ± 0.018). Frequencies (mean ± SD) of the higher and lower oscillations were 8.670 ± 0.259 Hz and 4.334 ± 0.112 Hz, respectively.

Theta skipping cells are present in nucleus reuniens.

(A and B) 12 representative theta cycle skipping cells which do not carry HD signal recorded in NRe; (A) from the left waveform, autocorrelation per 10 ms, interspike interval histogram (ISIH), autocorrelations per 1000 ms with and without fitting to the model, firing intensity map and polar plot are presented for two representative cells. Red line: envelope of the autocorrelation histogram obtained by fitting histogram data to the equation (see OM); (B) waveforms, ISIHs, and autocorrelation histograms with redline envelope of data fitting to the equation (OM) for 10 theta cycle skipping cells recorded in NRe.

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.03075.005

Spatial analyses

All described HD cells were analysed to test for spatial/place cell modulation (OM). In no case was any spatial modulation of the HD cell signal detected (see, e.g., Figure 2B,E).

Discussion

Nucleus Reuniens has been investigated anatomically, but remains largely unexplored electrophysiologically (Cassel et al., 2013). The HD cells in NRe share many characteristics of other HD cells, such as those in the anterodorsal and anteroventral thalamic nuclei, the dorsal presubiculum and retrosplenial cortex (Taube, 2007). Current theoretical models of HD information suggest that the HD signal derives, in part, from the dorsal tegmental nucleus of Gudden and the lateral mammillary nuclei (Taube, 2007; Cassel et al., 2013). In addition to projecting to the anterodorsal thalamic nucleus, the lateral mammillary nucleus innervates NRe (McKenna and Vertes, 2004). NRe also receives inputs from presubiculum (postsubiculum) and retrosplenial cortex, other sites containing HD cells. This pattern of connectivity suggests various potential sources for the heading information found in NRe. The presence of spatial cells in NRe is highly significant, as it is the major source of direct thalamic projections to the CA fields of the rat hippocampus (Su and Bentivoglio, 1990). NRe also receives widespread cortical and limbic inputs, involving both interoceptive and exteroceptive information. This nucleus has a notable role linking medial prefrontal cortex with the hippocampus (Prasad and Chudasama, 2013). These NRe HD cells are, therefore, pivotally positioned to influence hippocampal spatial processing directly because of its dense, direct connections with both the prefrontal cortex and hippocampus (Vertes, 2006; Prasad and Chudasama, 2013). Finally, the theta-skipping cells may provide a pace-maker like function for synchronising some early components of the HD system.

Materials and methods

Online data archive

Request a detailed protocol

The data from which this paper was generated are archived at doi: 10.5061/dryad.j68v0 and may be accessed via http://dx.doi.org/10.5061/dryad.j68v0.

Rats

Request a detailed protocol

Eleven (4–6 months) male Lister-Hooded rats (B&K, UK) weighing 420–530 g were used. Upon arrival, animals were housed individually and handled by the experimenter daily for a week before being trained in the pellet-chasing task (see below). Rats were food-restricted to 85% of their ad libitum body weight and kept in a temperature-controlled laminar airflow unit and maintained on a 12-hr light/dark cycle (lights on from 08:00 to 20:00 hr). Experiments were carried out in strict accordance with regulations laid out by LAST Ireland and were compliant with the European Union directives on animal experimentation (86/609/EEC).

Behavioural testing

Request a detailed protocol

Experiments were conducted in a circular arena (diameter 96 cms) and square arena (60 × 60 cm). The insides of the arenas were a uniform matt black, and low-level lighting was used during light testing; all lights were extinguished during dark testing. All experiments were conducted during the day between 0900 and 2000 hr. Session lengths were typically 20 min duration. Rats performed a pellet-chasing task during the course of the experiments. During testing, 20 mg food pellets (TestDiet, 5TUL formula) were thrown in the arena at random locations ca. every 20 s. During the weeks of recordings, animals were allowed 20 g of food daily. The environment is partially curtained with a visual cue card in a constant location. We leave the rat in the environment during the LDL transitions.

In vivo electrophysiology and surgery

Request a detailed protocol

Detailed descriptions of the surgical protocol and recording techniques can be found elsewhere (Brotons-Mas et al., 2010; Tsanov et al., 2011). Briefly, rats were implanted with tetrodes of either four or eight bundles of ø 25 µm platinum–iridium wires (California Fine Wire Ltd., USA) mounted onto small driveable microdrives (Axona Ltd., UK) at the following coordinates targeted at the nucleus reuniens (see Figure 1 for histological verification): −1.60 mm posterior to bregma, −1.20 mm lateral to the midline and at angle of about 5.5°. Depth varied depending on the target structure and ranged from 4.8 to 5.6 mm below the brain surface (Figure 1; Paxinos and Watson, 2005). Rats were allowed at least 1 week of recovery post-surgery. Tetrodes were lowered slowly through the brain (maximal rates 25–50 µm/day), typically over a period of weeks to prevent tissue damage and to ensure successful NRe electrode targeting and penetration. Based on the daily record of the electrode position and post-mortem histological verification, each recording could be located along the tetrode trace. The recording sessions took place in arenas located in the centre of the test room, which contained multiple, large visual cues made salient to allow the animals to orient themselves in the environment. An example of a NRe HD cell is provided in Video 1.

Video 1
An example of a well-discriminated head direction unit recorded in NRe while the rat is engaged in pellet chasing in a circular arena.

The unit corresponds to unit 4 of Figure 1A.

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.03075.006

Recording and statistical analysis

Request a detailed protocol

Standard statistical testing used Matlab scripts and Axona software. Unit identification involved several criteria. First, neurons had to be active in all conditions and had to present the same waveform characteristics (amplitude, height, and duration) in those conditions. Furthermore, units had to demonstrate a clean refractory period (>2 ms) in the inter-spike interval (ISI) histogram. Units were sorted using conventional cluster-cutting techniques and classified by the environmental manipulation to which they were exposed. Once well-defined neuronal signals were isolated and the rats explored the arena sufficiently (rats had to explore at least 90% of the open field in either session to be included in analysis to allow reliable calculation of spatial characteristics), recording commenced.

In total, 758 well-isolated units were recorded in 3 rats in NRe and the adjacent anteromedial thalamic nucleus (AM). After post-mortem histological verification, 483 cells were assigned to NRe. To select animals for analysis we set following criteria: histologically-verified electrodes should be placed in the lateral part of nucleus reuniens, therefore bypassing laterally the Rhomboid nucleus, avoiding the possibility that by mistake some cells would be wrongly assigned to NRe or the Rhomboid nucleus. Further, an observed electrophysiological criterion required co-localised HD cells and theta skipping cells in NRe. We did recordings in many animals in surrounding nuclei but as described in the present paper theta skipping cells are characteristic only for NRe, are electrophysiologically colocalised with HD cells therefore can serve as an electrophysiological marker of lateral part of NRe. Electrode tracks were localised predominantly in the lateral portion of NRe. The numbers and percentages of cells recorded in NRe were: 42 HD cells (8.7%); 19 theta cycle skipping cells (3.9%; these cells characteristically appeared in NRe, electrophysiologically co-localised with HD cells); 55 other theta modulated cells (11.3%); 23 fast firing cells (4.7%); 21 weakly-theta modulated cells (4.3%); 13 other spatially-tuned cells (2.2%). 309 cells (63.9%) were classified as unidentified low firing units–cells that did not exhibit any particular temporal or spatial properties or formed groups smaller than four cells with similar phenotype. Among unidentified low firing units 53 cells fired with maximum frequency lower than 1 Hz.

Head direction analyses

Request a detailed protocol

Directional analyses were performed for all recorded cells in nucleus reuniens, 42 units in total). The rat's HD was calculated for each tracker sample from the projection of the relative position of the LEDs onto the horizontal plane. The directional tuning function for each cell was obtained by plotting the firing rate as a function of the rat's directional heading, divided into bins of 5°. The firing rate was computed based on the total number of spikes divided by the total time in that bin (Taube et al., 1990). To restrict the influence of inhomogeneous sampling on directional tuning, we accepted data only if all directional bins were sampled by the rat. The directionality of the HD units in the horizontal plane (measured in degrees) was normalized for comparison of the HD firing rate properties. The peak firing rate of cells that respond to different direction of heading was aligned to a HD of 180° (Bassett et al., 2005). The firing rate was normalized (with values between 0 and 1) with respect to the peak firing rate for each unit (Bassett et al., 2005). The firing rate is calculated by dividing the number of spikes by the number of visits at a particular head direction bin. The CW/CCW separation was calculated by considering a particular angular head velocity threshold (120°/s). When the rat moves at +120°/s, it was placed into the CW head direction category (similarly, for CCW, −120°/s). If it coincidentally happens that spikes occur at a lower rate during slower head movements, the visit count will increase in the non-separating case, the spike count will be relatively decreased (causing a decrease in the firing rate compared to the CW/CCW separation).

Spatial analyses

Additional analyses examined for spatial modulation of these cells. The spatial specificity (or spatial information content) is expressed in bits per spike (Skaggs et al., 1996). Place field size is computed as the region of the arena in which the firing rate of the place cell is above 20% of the maximum firing frequency (Hollup et al., 2001). A place field was identified if nine neighbouring pixels (sharing a side) were above 20% of the peak firing rate. Place field size was represented in number of pixels. The spatial selectivity of a firing field (ratio of maximal signal to noise) was calculated by dividing the firing rate of the cell in the bin with the maximum average rate by its mean firing rate over the entire apparatus (Skaggs et al., 1996). Average frequency is the total number of spikes divided by the total recording time and is expressed in Hz. Where recordings were conducted on successive days, units were matched based on their spike amplitude, height, and spike duration with the respective units from the control recording. Exploration was assessed by comparing the occupancy of bins and the number of visits per bin between the two recording conditions.

Autocorrelation histograms

Request a detailed protocol

Autocorrelation histograms of the ISI distribution were obtained for ISI lags of −1000 ms to +1000 ms between spikes with 1 ms-wide bins. The non-normalized autocorrelation signal was fitted to Equation 1 (an extended form of the one used by Brandon et al. (2013)).

y(x)=[a1cos(ω1x)+a2cos(ω2x)]×exp(|x|τ1)+b+c1exp(|x|τ2)c2exp(|x|τ3)

Here, y is the autocorrelation signal, x is the lag, a1 and a2 are the amplitude of the oscillating terms with frequency ω1 and ω2 modulated by exponential with decay constant τ1.The terms τ2 and τ3 are the decay constants for the exponentials with amplitude c1 and c2. The parameters a1, a2, b, c1 and c2 were allowed to vary in the range [0, N], where N is the peak of the autocorrelation signal. The range of decay constants are, τ1 = [0, 5000], τ2 = [0, 100], τ3 = [0, 10] in millisecond unit. ω1 and ω2 were varied in the range [12π, 24π] and [6π, 12π], respectively.

Assuming spikes were generated following a Poisson process, the ISI was considered to follow an exponential distribution, which also gives an exponential distribution in its autocorrelation, represented in Equation 1 by the positive exponential component comprised of (c1, τ2). The initial dip in the autocorrelation for the refractory period in the ISI was given by the fast-decaying negative exponential (c2, τ3). The alternating low and high peaks are modelled as the superposition of two oscillations, given by two slow decaying cosine functions, as if the high peaks are generated when the oscillations are in the same-phase and the low peaks are generated when they are in anti-phase. The baseline shifts for the cosine terms and all the constant errors are interpreted in constant b. Equation 1 does not hold the prior assumption that one of the periodic functions oscillates exactly at half-frequency of the other, as others have assumed (Brandon et al., 2013). The curve fitting followed the measurement of ‘jump factor’, defined as the relative contribution of the low frequency components in the higher peaks given by a2/(a1 + a2). Frequency ratios of the cosine functions were also measured to verify the superposition model of the Equation 1. Theta cycle skipping index, TS was measured by Equation 4; Brandon et al., 2013.

As described (Brandon et al., 2013), TS range should be [−1 to +1]. A positive TS is present if the second peak after the centre peak is larger than the first one; a negative TS is not expected according to the model in Equation 1 if it is fitted for the theta-skipping cycle cell with an alternative low and high peaks and low peaks appearing first after the centre peak. We did not find any negative TSs (as can be seen in the raw autocorrelograms; see supplementary figures). The greater the TS, the greater the larger peak is jumped from the theta-modulated signal, caused by the interference of the second oscillation. We measured this effect with an alternative index ‘jump factor’ defined as a2/(a1+ a2), which provides a direct measurement of the relative contribution of the two oscillations. A jump factor >0.5 means the contribution of the slow varying interfering oscillation in the jump is greater than the theta-range oscillation. If the frequency ratio follows a 2:1 ratio (or closer), then the mathematical basis of using two cosine functions of different frequency ranges to obtain consecutive in-phase or anti-phase superposition to yield lower and higher peaks respectively, is verified.

Histological analyses

Request a detailed protocol

On completion of the recording studies, the rats received an overdose of anaesthetic (1.5 g of urethane (Sigma-Aldrich, Dublin, Ireland) dissolved in 4.5 ml water) and were then perfused intracardially with 250 ml of 0.1 M phosphate-buffered saline (PBS) at room temperature followed by 350 ml of 4% paraformaldehyde in 0.1 M PBS at ∼4°C, after which the brains were removed and placed in 4% paraformaldehyde (for at least 72 hr). Brains were blocked, placed on a freezing platform, and 40 µm coronal sections were cut with a sledge microtome (Leica 1400). Two, alternate series that used all sections were taken through the rostral thalamus. One series was mounted directly onto gelatine-subbed slides, and then allowed to dry overnight before staining with cresyl violet, a Nissl stain. The second series was immunologically reacted with the neuronal marker α-NeuN (MAB 377; Chemicon, Watford, UK), then with a secondary horse anti-mouse rat adsorbed antibody (AI-2001; Vector Laboratories Ltd, Peterborough, UK) and subsequently visualised with Vector Elite ABC (PK-6100; Vector Laboratories Ltd) and diaminobenzidine. A Leica DM5000B microscope with Leica DFC310FX digital camera and Leica Application Suite image acquisition software was used for brightfield microscopy. NeuN provides a stain assumed to be selective for neurons, that is it does not label glial cells. As a consequence it can sometimes make it easier to see underlying cytoarchtectonic features.

Recording positions were determined by calculating distance above the deepest electrode position, and calculating distance below the first penetration into the tissue. The electrodes often caused tissue distortion and this was carefully allowed for in the position calculations. Positions of recorded cells were estimated as follows: theoretical positions of electrodes tips and NRe borderlines were estimated by reference to atlas (Paxinos and Watson, 2005) and reconstructed histological specimens. The electrodes sometimes caused tissue distortion even with very slow penetrations, and this was allowed for in the position calculations. The position of electrodes below brain surface was known for each recording session and expressed in µm, thereby allowing estimates of each cell position to be derived.

Data availability

The following data sets were generated

References

    1. Eleore L
    2. López-Ramos JC
    3. Guerra-Narbona R
    4. Delgado-García JM
    5. Izquierdo I
    (2011)
    Role of reuniens nucleus projections to the medial prefrontal cortex and to the hippocampal pyramidal CA1 area in associative learning
    e25538, PLOS ONE, 6, 1–11, 10.1371/journal.pone.0023538.
  1. Book
    1. Paxinos G
    2. Watson C
    (2005)
    The Rat Brain in stereotaxic coordinates
    Elsevier.
    1. Taube JS
    2. Muller RU
    3. Ranck JB Jnr
    (1990)
    Head-direction cells recorded from the postsubiculum in freely moving rats. I. Description and quantitative analysis
    The Journal of Neuroscience 10:420–435.

Article and author information

Author details

  1. Maciej M Jankowski

    Institute of Neuroscience, Trinity College Dublin, Dublin, Ireland
    Contribution
    MMJ, Acquisition of data, Analysis and interpretation of data, Drafting or revising the article
    Contributed equally with
    Md Nurul Islam
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  2. Md Nurul Islam

    Institute of Neuroscience, Trinity College Dublin, Dublin, Ireland
    Contribution
    MNI, Analysis and interpretation of data, Drafting or revising the article
    Contributed equally with
    Maciej M Jankowski
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  3. Nicholas F Wright

    School of Psychology, Cardiff University, Cardiff, United Kingdom
    Contribution
    NFW, Analysis and interpretation of data
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  4. Seralynne D Vann

    School of Psychology, Cardiff University, Cardiff, United Kingdom
    Contribution
    SDV, Analysis and interpretation of data, Drafting or revising the article
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  5. Jonathan T Erichsen

    Department of Optometry and Visual Science, Cardiff University, Cardiff, United Kingdom
    Contribution
    JTE, Analysis and interpretation of data, Drafting or revising the article
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  6. John P Aggleton

    School of Psychology, Cardiff University, Cardiff, United Kingdom
    Contribution
    JPA, Conception and design, Analysis and interpretation of data, Drafting and revising the article
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  7. Shane M O'Mara

    Institute of Neuroscience, Trinity College Dublin, Dublin, Ireland
    Contribution
    SMO'M, Conception and design, Analysis and interpretation of data, Drafting and revising the article
    For correspondence
    smomara@tcd.ie
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.

Funding

Wellcome Trust (WT092480)

  • Shane M O'Mara

The funder had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by The Wellcome Trust.

Ethics

Animal experimentation: Experiments were conducted in accordance with European Community directive, 86/609/EC, and the Cruelty to Animals Act, 1876, and followed Bioresources Ethics Committee, Trinity College, Dublin, Ireland, as well as LAST Ireland and international guidelines of good practice. Surgery was conducted under ketamine/xylazine anaesthesia, an appropriate post-surgery monitoring and analgesia regime was in place, and every effort was made to minimize suffering.

Copyright

© 2014, Jankowski et al.

This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.

Metrics

  • 3,936
    views
  • 419
    downloads
  • 93
    citations

Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.

Download links

A two-part list of links to download the article, or parts of the article, in various formats.

Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)

Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)

Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)

  1. Maciej M Jankowski
  2. Md Nurul Islam
  3. Nicholas F Wright
  4. Seralynne D Vann
  5. Jonathan T Erichsen
  6. John P Aggleton
  7. Shane M O'Mara
(2014)
Nucleus reuniens of the thalamus contains head direction cells
eLife 3:e03075.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.03075

Share this article

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.03075

Further reading

    1. Cell Biology
    2. Neuroscience
    Lu Yang, Zijin Lin ... Li Liu
    Research Article

    Blood–brain barrier (BBB) prevents neurotoxins from entering central nervous system. We aimed to establish and characterize an in vitro triple co-culture BBB model consisting of brain endothelial cells hCMEC/D3, astrocytoma U251 cells, and neuroblastoma SH-SY5Y cells. Co-culture of SH-SY5Y and U251 cells markedly enhanced claudin-5 and VE-cadherin expression in hCMEC/D3 cells, accompanied by increased transendothelial electrical resistance and decreased permeability. Conditioned medium (CM) from SH-SY5Y cells (S-CM), U251 cells (U-CM), and co-culture of SH-SY5Y and U251 cells (US-CM) also promoted claudin-5 and VE-cadherin expression. Glial cell line-derived neurotrophic factor (GDNF) levels in S-CM and US-CM were significantly higher than CMs from hCMEC/D3 and U-CM. Both GDNF and US-CM upregulated claudin-5 and VE-cadherin expression, which were attenuated by anti-GDNF antibody and GDNF signaling inhibitors. GDNF increased claudin-5 expression via the PI3K/AKT/FOXO1 and MAPK/ERK pathways. Meanwhile, GDNF promoted VE-cadherin expression by activating PI3K/AKT/ETS1 and MAPK/ERK/ETS1 signaling. The roles of GDNF in BBB integrity were validated using brain-specific Gdnf silencing mice. The developed triple co-culture BBB model was successfully applied to predict BBB permeability. In conclusion, neurons enhance BBB integrity by upregulating claudin-5 and VE-cadherin expression through GDNF secretion and established triple co-culture BBB model may be used to predict drugs’ BBB permeability.

    1. Neuroscience
    Typhaine Comyn, Thomas Preat ... Pierre-Yves Plaçais
    Research Article

    Relevance-based selectivity and high energy cost are two distinct features of long-term memory (LTM) formation that warrant its default inhibition. Spaced repetition of learning is a highly conserved cognitive mechanism that can lift this inhibition. Here, we questioned how the spacing effect integrates experience selection and energy efficiency at the cellular and molecular levels. We showed in Drosophila that spaced training triggers LTM formation by extending over several hours an increased mitochondrial metabolic activity in neurons of the associative memory center, the mushroom bodies (MBs). We found that this effect is mediated by PKCδ, a member of the so-called ‘novel PKC’ family of enzymes, which uncovers the critical function of PKCδ in neurons as a regulator of mitochondrial metabolism for LTM. Additionally, PKCδ activation and translocation to mitochondria result from LTM-specific dopamine signaling on MB neurons. By bridging experience-dependent neuronal circuit activity with metabolic modulation of memory-encoding neurons, PKCδ signaling binds the cognitive and metabolic constraints underlying LTM formation into a unified gating mechanism.