Locomotion: How vision shapes the paths we choose

A mathematical model can predict the path walkers take through a rugged landscape, including the tendency of people to avoid paths that are too steep, even if it means going farther.
  1. Arthur D Kuo  Is a corresponding author
  1. Faculty of Kinesiology and Department of Biomedical Engineering, University of Calgary, Canada

When walking, our brain needs to make decisions, such as where to step and which path to follow. Visual inputs are central to this process, as they allow us to continuously estimate our motion and orientation, as well as regulate our direction and speed (Warren et al., 2001).

This continuous regulation can be explained using mathematical models that rely on vector calculus (Matthis et al., 2022). However, walking also involves discrete choices – such as selecting which available foothold to use, or turning left or right – that are guided by visual features of the path ahead, such as the rockiness and steepness of the terrain (Matthis et al., 2018). While it may seem obvious that people avoid rugged paths when easier alternatives exist, there is no mathematical model to explain this behavior. Now, in eLife, Mary Hayhoe and colleagues – including Karl Muller as first author – report the results of a study that sheds new light on how these decisions are made (Muller et al., 2024).

The researchers analyzed data from previous studies in which they had tracked the eye and body movements of nine individuals as they walked across natural terrains (Matthis et al., 2022; Bonnen et al., 2021). Participants also wore eye-tracking cameras that captured visual information about where they were looking within the surrounding physical landscape. Muller et al. used this data to create a three-dimensional computational reconstruction of the terrains, which they aligned to the body and eye movements of the participants. This allowed them to develop egocentric visual depth images (which estimated how the retinas of individuals detect the depth of the terrain), and to determine where participants placed their feet.

The researchers – who are based at the University of Texas at Austin, Indiana University and Northeastern University – then used these visual depth images and foothold locations to train a neural network where to step in the reconstructed terrain. The trained model successfully predicted where the human participants placed their feet better than chance, suggesting that decisions about where to step are influenced by visual depth cues. This agrees with prior work showing reduced depth cues cause walkers to shift their gaze to closer footholds, perhaps so they can devote more attention to resolving depth ambiguities (Bonnen et al., 2021).

Next, Muller et al. used their model to investigate whether walkers chose flatter, more winding paths over direct, steeper alternatives that require more energy to cross due to the uneven terrain (Darici and Kuo, 2023; Kowalsky et al., 2021). The team compared two metrics: tortuosity, which is the ratio between the length of a path and the direct distance between its start and end; and a dimensionless measure of ruggedness, equal to the average angle of the up and down steps within a path. These metrics were recorded for the actual path the walkers took, and also for simulated, alternative routes they might have taken through the same terrain.

Muller et al. found that the tortuosity of the paths chosen by the human walkers correlated with the ruggedness of the most direct simulated path through the terrain: the steeper and more rugged the direct path, the more likely a walker is to detour and take a longer, flatter route. This suggests a trade-off whereby walkers assess paths with high tortuosity to be less energetically costly than ones which are straighter but more rugged. Muller et al. also discovered that this trade-off was stronger for shorter individuals, possibly due to greater difficulty with steep steps, though this effect warrants further study (as discussed in the public reviews of the article).

Studying locomotion in natural settings is challenging, as terrains and visual features are harder to manipulate or measure compared to controlled laboratory environments. The methods created by Muller et al. open up new avenues for understanding decision-making during locomotion. However, there is much about the trade-off proposed in the study that remains unknown.

For example, controlled experiments are needed to confirm whether the ruggedness of the simulated direct paths match those experienced in real life. In addition, the human eye likely perceives other aspects of the terrain beyond just depth, and it remains to be seen whether other visual cues help evaluate different structural properties of the landscape, such as friction and the hardness of footholds. Related issues in natural locomotion, such as energy expenditure, also merit further investigation. For instance, the energetic costs of tortuous versus rugged paths still need quantification, as do temporal costs, which are also suspected to govern ecological behavior (Carlisle and Kuo, 2023).

The approach taken by Muller et al. could also be used to study people in human-designed environments, such as pedestrians crossing busy roads (do people jaywalk rather than use pedestrian crossings to save energy, time, or other costs?). Whether navigating natural landscapes or cityscapes, humans constantly create their own paths according to a variety of physical and perceptual criteria.

References

Article and author information

Author details

  1. Arthur D Kuo

    Arthur D Kuo is in the Faculty of Kinesiology and Department of Biomedical Engineering, University of Calgary, Calgary, Canada

    For correspondence
    arthurdkuo@gmail.com
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0001-5233-9709

Publication history

  1. Version of Record published:

Copyright

© 2024, Kuo

This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.

Metrics

  • 347
    views
  • 30
    downloads
  • 0
    citations

Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.

Download links

A two-part list of links to download the article, or parts of the article, in various formats.

Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)

Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)

Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)

  1. Arthur D Kuo
(2024)
Locomotion: How vision shapes the paths we choose
eLife 13:e104965.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.104965
  1. Further reading

Further reading

    1. Neuroscience
    Jacob A Miller
    Insight

    When navigating environments with changing rules, human brain circuits flexibly adapt how and where we retain information to help us achieve our immediate goals.

    1. Neuroscience
    Franziska Auer, Katherine Nardone ... David Schoppik
    Research Article

    Cerebellar dysfunction leads to postural instability. Recent work in freely moving rodents has transformed investigations of cerebellar contributions to posture. However, the combined complexity of terrestrial locomotion and the rodent cerebellum motivate new approaches to perturb cerebellar function in simpler vertebrates. Here, we adapted a validated chemogenetic tool (TRPV1/capsaicin) to describe the role of Purkinje cells — the output neurons of the cerebellar cortex — as larval zebrafish swam freely in depth. We achieved both bidirectional control (activation and ablation) of Purkinje cells while performing quantitative high-throughput assessment of posture and locomotion. Activation modified postural control in the pitch (nose-up/nose-down) axis. Similarly, ablations disrupted pitch-axis posture and fin-body coordination responsible for climbs. Postural disruption was more widespread in older larvae, offering a window into emergent roles for the developing cerebellum in the control of posture. Finally, we found that activity in Purkinje cells could individually and collectively encode tilt direction, a key feature of postural control neurons. Our findings delineate an expected role for the cerebellum in postural control and vestibular sensation in larval zebrafish, establishing the validity of TRPV1/capsaicin-mediated perturbations in a simple, genetically tractable vertebrate. Moreover, by comparing the contributions of Purkinje cell ablations to posture in time, we uncover signatures of emerging cerebellar control of posture across early development. This work takes a major step towards understanding an ancestral role of the cerebellum in regulating postural maturation.