Sound localization: These are not the neurons you are looking for

Studies that looked into how the auditory brainstem processes the difference in the intensity of a sound as it reaches each ear may have wrongly assumed which neurons were being recorded.
  1. Victor Benichoux  Is a corresponding author
  2. Daniel J Tollin  Is a corresponding author
  1. Institut Pasteur, France
  2. University of Colorado School of Medicine, Anschutz Medical Campus, United States

We may not realize it, but a noise to our left will arrive at our left ear a few hundred microseconds before it will reach our right ear. It will also be a few decibels louder on that side. Though we only hear a single coherent sound, our brain exploits these subtle differences in timing and volume (or level) to pinpoint where the noise came from.

These two cues are respectively termed the interaural time difference and the interaural level difference (Figure 1A and B). According to the duplex theory of sound localization (Rayleigh, 1907), each type of cue is used to find the origin of a different kind of sound: low-frequency sounds are located based on interaural time differences, and high-frequency sounds based on level differences. For over a century, this has served as the main framework to examine how we detect where a noise comes from.

The anatomical implementation of the duplex theory.

(A) Interaural time differences represent the difference between when a sound reaches the left ear (L) and the right ear (R). They can occur between the fine structure of the sound (blue), or the overall ‘envelope’ of the sound (green). (B) Differences in level result from a disparity in amplitudes (height of peaks and troughs) between the two ears (labeled ‘L’ and ‘R’). (C) The superior olivary complex receives input from each ear (black lines). The medial superior olive (MSO) receives excitation from both ears (blue lines). The lateral superior olive (LSO) receives excitation from the ear on the same side (blue lines) and inhibition from the ear on the opposite side (red lines) through the medial nucleus of the trapezoid body (MNTB), which is known for its exquisite temporal precision.

In addition, decades of anatomical and physiological studies have explored how this duplex theory could be wired in the brain. These experiments focused on the part of the auditory brainstem where the inputs from each ear first converge: the superior olivary complex (Figure 1C). This area is divided into several structures, which include a medial superior olive (MSO) and a lateral superior olive (LSO) on each side.

Neurons in the medial superior olive (MSO neurons) favor low-frequency sounds. They are also exquisitely sensitive to timing and can react to microsecond differences in the inputs from each ear. MSO neurons accomplish this feat by acting as coincidence detectors. That is, they respond only when excitatory signals from the two ears arrive within a few microseconds of one another. This means that they can detect interaural time differences (Yin and Chan, 1990).

In contrast, the cells in the lateral superior olive (LSO neurons) prefer high-frequency sounds. Extracellular recordings – which measure the electrical signals around the cells – indicated that these neurons respond more slowly, meaning that they could integrate the inhibitory and excitatory signals from the two ears , over a long time window – more than a millisecond (Figure 1C). This would make them able to detect interaural level differences, which could be relayed via changes in the firing rate of the neurons. While there are several types of neuron in the LSO, these characteristics were presumed to belong to the ‘principal neurons’, as these cells form over 80% of the structure.

Now, in eLife, Tom Franken of the Katholieke Universiteit Leuven, Philip Joris (Leuven) and Philip Smith (University of Wisconsin, Madison), report flaws in this assumption (Franken et al., 2018). In an extremely challenging set of experiments, the researchers latched microscopic glass electrodes directly onto individual LSO neurons in the brain of Mongolian gerbils to obtain recordings from within the cells. This was done without knowing whether the cells were principal neurons or neurons of other types. They then characterized how the neurons responded to sound and to interaural level differences. Finally, they identified which of these cells were principal neurons by injecting the neurons with a dye and examining their morphology under an electron microscope.

Together, these data revealed that, contrary to what was observed in the extracellular studies, the principal neurons of the LSO do not exhibit the type of slow response that integrates multiple inputs. Rather, their responses are similar to those shown by MSO neurons (that is, a fast response just to the onset of sound). However, these fast principal cells did still appear to convey information about interaural level differences.

Even before the work by Franken et al., auditory neuroscientists had struggled to directly match the duplex theory onto the superior olivary complex. LSO neurons are inhibited by another area in the brainstem, which encodes the timing of sound with extraordinarily high precision (Figure 1C). In fact, decades ago LSO cells were observed to be sensitive to interaural level differences as well as interaural time differences, which is consistent with this well-timed inhibitory input (Finlayson and Caspary, 1991; Joris and Yin, 1995; Tollin et al., 2005). Nonetheless the view that LSO principal cells are slower and relatively better at integrating signals compared to the MSO has persisted (Remme et al., 2014).

Franken et al. now show that this is not the case, and provide a clue as to why some previous studies may have missed this. Like MSO cells, LSO principal neurons fire off small action potentials, which are difficult to detect through extracellular measures. Thus, prior studies that used extracellular recordings were likely biased towards the other classes of LSO neurons that fire large action potentials; ironically, these cells can integrate signals over time, and also respond to interaural level differences.

To this day, behavioral observations largely support the duplex theory of sound localization (Macpherson and Middlebrooks, 2002). If this framework is not directly reflected in the organization of the superior olivary complex, how is the processing of time and level differences distributed across the LSO and MSO?

Franken et al. suggest that the LSO principal neurons in fact process information in a way that is similar to the MSO: they both act as coincidence detectors. Yet, in contrast to the MSO, the LSO detects the coincidence of excitation from the ear on the same side and inhibition from the opposite side. Rather than conveying information about level differences through changes in their firing rates, the fast LSO cells do so via shifts in the delay of the interaction between excitation and inhibition, which is dependent on the intensity of the sound. Indeed, computational modeling of LSO neurons that detect the coincidence of excitation and inhibition from the two ears was recently shown to effectively create sensitivity to time differences for high-frequency sounds, but also to preserve sensitivity to level differences (Ashida et al., 2017). It is also possible that level differences are processed at the next stage of the brain auditory system; in particular, the neurons in the auditory midbrain integrate inputs over longer time windows, long enough to compute the level-related signals (Brown and Tollin, 2016; Li and Pollak, 2013).

The study by Franken et al. is a good example of how prior expectations can involuntarily mislead scientific endeavor. Indeed, in previous studies, investigators would often classify the neurons that they were ‘blindly’ recording according to predefined sets of expected responses. Franken et al. also cautiously note that, in some prior publications, neurons that exhibited a non-canonical response similar to the one they observed in their own study were typically considered 'pathological' and discarded. Only systematic and rigorous experimental approaches can help detect such anomalies, and rightly cast doubt on well-established scientific dogma.

The new findings now need to be replicated across the species where LSO neurons have been studied to determine whether they will stand the test of time. In the meantime, these results encourage us to pause and rethink how the duplex theory can be implemented in the superior olivary complex.

References

Article and author information

Author details

  1. Victor Benichoux

    Victor Benichoux is in the Unit of Genetics and Physiology of Hearing, Department of Neuroscience, Institut Pasteur, Paris, France

    For correspondence
    victor.benichoux@pasteur.fr
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0001-7586-4397
  2. Daniel J Tollin

    Daniel J Tollin is in the Department of Physiology and Biophysics, University of Colorado School of Medicine, Anschutz Medical Campus, Aurora, United States

    For correspondence
    daniel.tollin@ucdenver.edu
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0001-6790-1290

Publication history

  1. Version of Record published: July 27, 2018 (version 1)

Copyright

© 2018, Benichoux et al.

This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.

Metrics

  • 5,872
    views
  • 217
    downloads
  • 3
    citations

Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.

Download links

A two-part list of links to download the article, or parts of the article, in various formats.

Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)

Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)

Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)

  1. Victor Benichoux
  2. Daniel J Tollin
(2018)
Sound localization: These are not the neurons you are looking for
eLife 7:e39244.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.39244

Further reading

    1. Neuroscience
    Aedan Yue Li, Natalia Ladyka-Wojcik ... Morgan Barense
    Research Article

    Combining information from multiple senses is essential to object recognition, core to the ability to learn concepts, make new inferences, and generalize across distinct entities. Yet how the mind combines sensory input into coherent crossmodal representations - the crossmodal binding problem - remains poorly understood. Here, we applied multi-echo fMRI across a four-day paradigm, in which participants learned 3-dimensional crossmodal representations created from well-characterized unimodal visual shape and sound features. Our novel paradigm decoupled the learned crossmodal object representations from their baseline unimodal shapes and sounds, thus allowing us to track the emergence of crossmodal object representations as they were learned by healthy adults. Critically, we found that two anterior temporal lobe structures - temporal pole and perirhinal cortex - differentiated learned from non-learned crossmodal objects, even when controlling for the unimodal features that composed those objects. These results provide evidence for integrated crossmodal object representations in the anterior temporal lobes that were different from the representations for the unimodal features. Furthermore, we found that perirhinal cortex representations were by default biased towards visual shape, but this initial visual bias was attenuated by crossmodal learning. Thus, crossmodal learning transformed perirhinal representations such that they were no longer predominantly grounded in the visual modality, which may be a mechanism by which object concepts gain their abstraction.

    1. Genetics and Genomics
    2. Neuroscience
    Bohan Zhu, Richard I Ainsworth ... Javier González-Maeso
    Research Article

    Genome-wide association studies have revealed >270 loci associated with schizophrenia risk, yet these genetic factors do not seem to be sufficient to fully explain the molecular determinants behind this psychiatric condition. Epigenetic marks such as post-translational histone modifications remain largely plastic during development and adulthood, allowing a dynamic impact of environmental factors, including antipsychotic medications, on access to genes and regulatory elements. However, few studies so far have profiled cell-specific genome-wide histone modifications in postmortem brain samples from schizophrenia subjects, or the effect of antipsychotic treatment on such epigenetic marks. Here, we conducted ChIP-seq analyses focusing on histone marks indicative of active enhancers (H3K27ac) and active promoters (H3K4me3), alongside RNA-seq, using frontal cortex samples from antipsychotic-free (AF) and antipsychotic-treated (AT) individuals with schizophrenia, as well as individually matched controls (n=58). Schizophrenia subjects exhibited thousands of neuronal and non-neuronal epigenetic differences at regions that included several susceptibility genetic loci, such as NRG1, DISC1, and DRD3. By analyzing the AF and AT cohorts separately, we identified schizophrenia-associated alterations in specific transcription factors, their regulatees, and epigenomic and transcriptomic features that were reversed by antipsychotic treatment; as well as those that represented a consequence of antipsychotic medication rather than a hallmark of schizophrenia in postmortem human brain samples. Notably, we also found that the effect of age on epigenomic landscapes was more pronounced in frontal cortex of AT-schizophrenics, as compared to AF-schizophrenics and controls. Together, these data provide important evidence of epigenetic alterations in the frontal cortex of individuals with schizophrenia, and remark for the first time on the impact of age and antipsychotic treatment on chromatin organization.