Learning-related population dynamics in the auditory thalamus
Abstract
Learning to associate sensory stimuli with a chosen action involves a dynamic interplay between cortical and thalamic circuits. While the cortex has been widely studied in this respect, how the thalamus encodes learning-related information is still largely unknown. We studied learning-related activity in the medial geniculate body (MGB; Auditory thalamus), targeting mainly the dorsal and medial regions. Using fiber photometry, we continuously imaged population calcium dynamics as mice learned a go/no-go auditory discrimination task. The MGB was tuned to frequency and responded to cognitive features like the choice of the mouse within several hundred milliseconds. Encoding of choice in the MGB increased with learning, and was highly correlated with the learning curves of the mice. MGB also encoded motor parameters of the mouse during the task. These results provide evidence that the MGB encodes task- motor- and learning-related information.
Introduction
Learning, the process of acquiring new knowledge through experience, is known to involve disparate brain areas, and particularly the cortex. For example, learning to discriminate between different sensory stimuli leads to changes in the respective primary sensory areas (Blake et al., 2002; Chen et al., 2015; Driscoll et al., 2017; Gilad and Helmchen, 2020; Jurjut et al., 2017; Komiyama et al., 2010; Li et al., 2008; Makino and Komiyama, 2015; Poort et al., 2015; Yan et al., 2014). Cortical neural responses have often been shown to strengthen after learning. For example, neural signals in animals that gain expertise in a specific task, show increased activity and result in higher discriminatory power between the learned stimuli (Gilad et al., 2018; Gilad and Helmchen, 2020; Poort et al., 2015; Wiest et al., 2010; Yan et al., 2014). In other cases, decreased responsiveness to the learned stimuli have been measured. These changes, too, may result in improvement in the discrimination of the stimuli (Christensen et al., 2019; Maor et al., 2020).
The cortex dynamically interacts with the thalamus via recurrent loops that are thought to involve more than mere sensory processing. Rather, thalamocortical circuitry also encodes high-order processing of cognitive functions such as attention, working memory and learning (Acsády, 2017; Audette et al., 2019; Bennett et al., 2019; Bolkan et al., 2017; Guo et al., 2017; McAlonan et al., 2008; Rose and Bonhoeffer, 2018; Roth et al., 2016; Saalmann and Kastner, 2015; Schmitt et al., 2017; Ward, 2013; Williams and Holtmaat, 2019; Zhang and Bruno, 2019). Where do cognitive responses arise and how do they impact basic sensory processing remains an area of active research. In this study, we focused on the medial geniculate body (MGB; auditory thalamus), asking whether and how it represents sensory and cognitive information along learning.
The MGB is, at least in part, a thalamic relay center of the auditory pathway, predominantly receiving direct input from the inferior colliculus (Calford and Aitkin, 1983; Peruzzi et al., 1997), but also from cortex (Winer et al., 2001) and other sources (Crabtree, 1998; Lee, 2015; Winer, 1992). Its projections target the cerebral cortex and numerous other brain areas such as the amygdala (LeDoux et al., 1991; Lee, 2015; Tasaka et al., 2020). Anatomical studies divide the MGB into three main sub-divisions: ventral, dorsal and medial (Calford and Aitkin, 1983; Clerici and Coleman, 1990; Hashikawa et al., 1991; Imig and Morel, 1985; Mo and Sherman, 2019; Rouiller et al., 1989; Smith et al., 2012). The ventral MGB relays topographically organized information from the inferior colliculus to the primary auditory cortex (Rouiller et al., 1989; Smith et al., 2012). This pathway is called the lemniscal pathway, and is considered to be the main auditory processing pathway. While tracing experiment suggest a simple topographical representations of sounds relayed from MGB to cortex (Hackett et al., 2011), physiological responses of MGB axon terminals suggest a more complex interaction (Vasquez-Lopez et al., 2017). Particularly, the dorsal and medial parts of the MGB are thought to process more complex information than the ventral MGB and not obey strict tonotopy. Dorsal and medial MGB project to higher-order cortical areas (Huang and Winer, 2000; Lee, 2015; Tasaka et al., 2020) and receive cortical feedback, among others, from layer 5 of the auditory cortex (Bartlett et al., 2000; Lee, 2015; Llano and Sherman, 2008). Those parts of the MGB, considered to be part of the non-lemniscal pathway, are well positioned to encode higher-order information of sensory, motor and associative nature, but only few studies have measured these in the auditory thalamus directly (Jaramillo et al., 2014).
Learning-related plasticity has been measured in the cortex after training animals to discriminate between two simple sounds, neurons in auditory cortex display several learning related modulations: encoding of higher-order choice information (Guo et al., 2019; Jaramillo et al., 2014), tonotopic map reorganization (Maor et al., 2020; Polley et al., 2006), increased responses to a target frequency and decreased responses to a distractor stimulus (Blake et al., 2002; David et al., 2012; Ghose, 2004; Ohl and Scheich, 2005). The inferior colliculus, too, has been shown to processes auditory information based on behavioral context and bodily movements, which are also considered high order processing (Calford and Aitkin, 1983; Casseday et al., 2002; Gruters and Groh, 2012; Yang et al., 2020). The MGB, being highly interconnected with both downstream and upstream brain regions like the inferior colliculus and cortex (Tasaka et al., 2020), is expected to encode learning-related activity. Indeed, the MGB has been shown to encode the choice of the mouse during an auditory discrimination task (Chen et al., 2019; Jaramillo et al., 2014). Furthermore, a large body of evidence indicates that the medial MGB is involved in auditory fear conditioning, by providing fast, less refined auditory information to the lateral amygdala (Han et al., 2008; Herry and Johansen, 2014; Maren and Quirk, 2004; Quirk et al., 1995; Romanski and LeDoux, 1992; Weinberger, 2011). To study learning related modulations in the MGB in the context of perceptual learning, we chronically imaged MGB population responses to sounds as mice learned a go/no-go auditory discrimination task. We find learning-related modulations in the MGB, and a particularly strong modulation to choice signals.
Results
Calcium imaging from the MGB along learning using fiber photometry
To study learning-related changes in the MGB, we injected AAV-GCaMP6f into the MGB of C57BL/6 mice and implanted a 400 µm optical fiber directly above the injection site. After a week of handling and habituation to head-fixation, we trained mice on a go/no-go auditory discrimination task. Each trial started with a visual start cue (orange LED; duration 0.1 s; 2 s before stimulus onset) followed by an auditory stimulus, either a go or a no-go pure tone sound (Figure 1A; duration 1 s; sounds separated by 0.5 octave; mostly 10 kHz for go and 7.1 kHz for no-go; see Materials and methods). After stimulus offset, we counted licks in a virtual response window of 3 s. Licking in response to the go sound were counted as ‘hit’ trials and rewarded with a drop of water. Withholding licking in response to the no-go sound were counted as correct rejection trials (CR), and were not punished or reinforced. Licking in response to the no-go sound were counted as false alarm trials (FA) that were followed by a mild punishment in the form of white noise (duration 3 s). Withholding licking for the go sound were counted as Miss trials, and were not punished. As mice learned to discriminate between the two sounds we continuously imaged population responses in the MGB in addition to monitoring their body movements during the task (Figure 1B). We first imaged six mice across learning. The fiber tips of the six mice were reconstructed to the higher-order regions of the MGB (Figure 1C, medial and dorsal parts grouped as MGB for simplicity). Mice learned the task within 1200–2000 trials as assessed by d’ (defined as d’=Z(Hit/(Hit+Miss)) – Z(FA/(FA+CR) where Z denotes the inverse of the cumulative distribution function; performance threshold was defined as d’=1; Figure 1D,E; fitted with a sigmoid function (Bathellier et al., 2013; Maor et al., 2020). The learning curves with respect to the go and no-go trials shows that individual mice varied widely in performance and strategy (Figure 1—figure supplement 1). Learning the task took different forms: some mice increased their CR rate, others had a steep increase in hit rate, whereas others gradually increased both hit and CR rates. Thus, mice learned the task using a range of behavioral strategies.

Behavioral paradigm, performance, and frequency tuning.
(A) Trial structure of a go/no-go auditory discrimination task and possible trial outcomes. (B) Behavioral setup for head fixed behaving mice along with simultaneous fiber photometry in the medial geniculate body (MGB). (C) Top: Fluorescent images of two coronal slices from two different mice, showing GCaMP6f (green) in the MGB along with the fiber track highlighted in white. DAPI staining in blue. Bottom: Localization of the fiber tip in the MGB for all six mice. (D) Behavioral learning curves for all mice (n = 6) depicting the performance (d’) as a function of trial number. Each learning curve was fitted with a sigmoid function. Dashed red line indicates the performance threshold (d’=1). (E) Learning threshold (the trial number where the learning curve crossed the performance threshold) for all mice. (F) A frequency response area plot (attenuation versus frequency) from one example recording of one example mouse. (G) Frequency tuning curve for the same example mouse. The frequencies used as go and no-go are marked by arrows. (H) Average responses to the go (blue) and no-go (gray) sounds. (I) Average evoked response (calculated from the 100 ms after stimulus onset; gray bar in ‘H’) to the go and no-go sounds from all mice. Error bars are s.e.m across mice. *p<0.05. Wilcoxon sign rank test.
Prior to training we imaged fluorescent fiber response to a range of frequencies while the mouse was passively listening in the awake state (4–40 kHz; 10, 20, 30, 40 dB attenuations relative to 62 dBSPL; duration 0.1 s; see Materials and methods). Population responses in the MGB displayed frequency responses, biased to the low frequency range (Figure 1F). In the six mice shown below, the response to the go frequency was significantly higher as compared to the no-go frequency (Figure 1G–H, one example mouse; Figure 1I, average of all six mice; p<0.05; Signed rank test; Figure 1—figure supplement 2A for the tuning curves of all mice).
The tuning curves of individual neurons in the dorsal and medial parts of the MGB are generally broader than ventral MGB (Bordi and LeDoux, 1994; Weinberger, 2011), and they represent multiple frequencies (Hackett et al., 2011). It is therefore surprising that the responses we measured were rather sharply tuned. Further surprising was that our signals peaked at lower frequencies, while the bulk signal of photometry is summing activity from multiple neurons. Whereas we cannot determine the source of the signal explicitly, the fiber tip locations showed that most recordings were from the dorsal and medial MGB. The full frequency response areas showed that our signal spanned 2.5 octaves, at least in high intensities. However, we cannot exclude that feedback connections from higher cortex (Vasquez-Lopez et al., 2017) or signal leaking from the ventral MGB contributed to the signal we recorded. Thus, the main source of the fluorescent signal is primarily, though not exclusively, of non-leminsical origin.
MGB encodes the choice of the mouse
To evaluate how sounds and other task attributes were represented in the MGB across learning, we plotted responses to the go and no-go sounds as mice learned the task. Figure 2A shows responses to the go and no-go sounds of one representative mouse (Figure 2A). The most evident change in MGB responses across learning was during the late part of the trial (0.6–1 s after stimulus onset), and particularly so for go trials (Figure 2A).

MGB encodes choice in expert mice.
(A) 2-dimensional plots of the calcium responses in MGB during the trial (x-axis) and across learning (y-axis; 50 trial bins) for one example mouse divided into go (left) and no-go (right) sounds. Dashed black line indicates stimulus onset (1 s duration) and dashed red line indicates the learning threshold. (B) Calcium responses when the mouse was novice (left) and expert (right). Traces are shown separately for different trial types (hit, miss, CR and FA trials; same data as in ‘A’). Shaded error bars are s.e.m across trials. (C) Mean calcium response during stimulus presentation per mouse in hit and miss trials when mice were novice (left) and expert (right). Error bars are s.e.m across trials. (D) Choice responses (defined as the response difference between hit and miss trials) during the trial, averaged across all mice when they were novice (dashed line) and expert (solid line). *p<0.05. ***p<0.001. n.s. – not significant. Wilcoxon rank sum test.
A particularly informative comparison is between hit and miss trials, where the stimulus is identical but the choice of the mouse is different (either lick or no-lick). Thus, differences between hits and misses represent encoding of choice. In expert mice (defined as the last 500 trials), MGB responses were higher for hit as compared to miss trials, and more so than in the novice mice (defined as the first 500 trials; Figure 2B; compare blue to light blue traces; For comparison, CR and FA trials are plotted in gray; MGB responses for all six mice individually are presented in Figure 2—figure supplement 1). Higher responses in hit versus miss trials were evident in all (6/6) expert mice and in 50% (3/6) of novice mice (Figure 2C; p<0.05; Wilcoxon rank sum test for each mouse separately; minimum 40 trials for hit or miss). Choice responses (i.e. hit minus miss) increased gradually after stimulus onset, and were stronger in expert mice (Figure 2D). These data suggests that the MGB encodes more than only sounds. MGB encodes the choice of the mouse, which implies that the auditory thalamus is involved in higher-level sensory-motor processing or some cognitive attributes of the task.
MGB encodes sounds early and choices late
To further explore the role of MGB in high level processing versus low-level sound processing, we tested the discrimination between sensory stimuli and choices at the single trial level. To do so, we calculated the receiver operating curve (ROC) and derived the area under the curve (AUC) between pairs of different trial-type distributions. We define two different AUC measures by comparing hit trials to either FA (stim AUC; magenta; that is different stimuli but similar choice) or to Miss (choice AUC; green; that is similar stimuli but different choice) trials (Figure 3A). The AUC value ranges from 0 to 1 and quantifies the accuracy of an ideal observer. AUC values close to 0.5 indicate low discrimination whereas values away from 0.5 indicate high discrimination. Together, these comparisons encompass the full breadth of options in the task (see below for additional trial types). In addition, we calculated AUC values for each time frame along the trial for the novice and expert conditions separately (i.e. first and last 500 trials). As a control, we calculated the sample distribution of trial-shuffled data and calculated the AUC identically. We define significance as the time point when the observed AUC values exceed ±3 std above the mean of the shuffled distribution (dashed lines in Figure 3B). For example, AUC data from one mouse is shown in Figure 3B. The ‘stim-AUC’ value is significantly discriminative early in the trial in both the novice and expert cases (Figure 3B,<100 ms from stimulus onset; arrows). This example shows that MGB responses carry highly discriminative information between the stimuli early in the trial and that this does not change following learning. In contrast, ‘choice-AUC’ reached significance only in the expert condition and only later in the trial (Figure 3B, green trace,>200 ms from stimulus onset; arrow).

MGB discriminates early between stimuli and late between choices.
(A) Schematic of the two single trial discrimination measures: hit vs. FA (magenta; Stim AUC) or hit vs. miss (green; Choice AUC). (B) Stim and Choice AUCs during the trial for one example mouse during novice (black lines) and expert (colored lines). Dashed black lines display the mean ±3 s.t.d of AUC from trial shuffled data. Arrows indicate the latency of the AUC measure (i.e. the first time point the signal exceeded the shuffled data). (C) Latency of discrimination for the Stim and Choice AUCs for all mice (averaged and marked individually). Error bars are s.e.m across mice (n=6). (D) Stim and Choice AUCs averaged across all expert mice. Early and late times are marked in gray. (E) The two AUC measures averaged during early (left) and late (right) times when mice were novice (black lines) and expert (colored). Error bars are s.e.m across mice. *p<0.05. Wilcoxon signed-rank test.
To quantify this effect across mice, we defined the exact latency for discrimination as the time it takes to reach significance for each AUC measure in the expert case (arrows in Figure 3B). The latency of discrimination for ‘stim-AUC’ was significantly earlier than that of the ‘choice-AUC’ (Figure 3C; p<0.05; Signed rank test; n = 6 mice). In addition, the onset of the choice-AUC was significantly earlier than the onset of licking in expert mice, implying that choice signal in the MGB precedes the execution of the motor command (p<0.05; Signed Rank test; n = 6 mice; Onset defined in both traces as the peak of the 2nd derivative; Choice onset = 207 ± 109 ms; Lick onset = 385 ± 130 ms; mean ±std). Averaged across all mice, stim-AUCs increased early and abruptly whereas choice-AUC increased gradually (Figure 3D; choice-AUCs for individual mice are shown separately in Figure 3—figure supplement 1). We then pooled together the AUC values during the early (0.1–0.3 s after stimulus onset) and late (0.6–1 s after stimulus onset) times of the trial (Figure 3E). We found that 1) Stim-AUC was significantly higher than choice-AUC early in the trial (p<0.05; Signed rank test; n = 6 mice), 2) Choice-AUC was higher, yet not significantly, during the late part of the trial compared to early times (p=0.09; Signed rank test; n = 6 mice), and 3) Choice-AUC was higher, yet not significantly, in the expert compared to the novice state, particularly during late times during the trial (Figure 3E; p=0.06; Signed rank test; n = 6 mice). In addition, we calculated choice and stim AUCs based on other pairs of trial types: CR vs. Miss for an alternative stim-AUC and FA vs. CR for an alternative choice-AUC (Figure 3—figure supplement 2). The alternative stim-AUC displayed similar early onset discrimination in both novice and expert cases. In contrast, the choice-AUC showed significant discrimination only in some mice, but not all. This was mainly due to the fact that the responses were quite weak to the no-go sound (see Figure 2—figure supplement 1 for ΔF/F responses in CR and FA trials), resulting in low discrimination values. In summary, these data indicate that stimulus information is present in the MGB relatively early, whereas choice information develops relatively late - several hundred milliseconds after stimulus onset.
Plasticity in MGB correlates with learning
Continuous imaging throughout the whole learning process enabled us to investigate the activity of the MGB on a trial-by-trial manner, and compare it to behavioral performance of each mouse. For this analysis, which was focused on go trials only, we averaged the calcium signals at late times during the trial (0.6–1 s after stimulus onset) to obtain MGB responses as a function of learning (termed ‘MGB response curve’). The MGB response curve was sigmoid-like, resembling the learning curve of the mouse (Figure 4A shows data from two mice; line is a sigmoid function fit). Next, we calculated the MGB response curve for each time point along the trial separately, correlated it with the learning curve, and plotted the correlation coefficient along time (see Materials and methods). Positive correlations depict high similarity between the MGB response curve to the learning curve at the given time point. The correlation increased after stimulus onset, indicating a strong relationship between MGB modulations and the learning process (Figure 4B - two representative mice; Figure 4C- all mice). To find the maximum point of change for each MGB response curve, we normalized sigmoid fits of the MGB response curves for each mouse separately, and derived the trial threshold for each curve (Figure 4D; defined as the trial number in which the normalized curve crosses 0.5). The trial threshold of the MGB curves matched nicely the learning threshold of each mouse (Figure 4E; r = 0.89; p<0.05). Calculating the learning curves based on earlier times (0–0.3 s after stimulus onset) resulted in MGB curves which are much less sigmoid-like and their threshold did not match the learning curves of each mouse (r = 0.3; p>0.05), indicating that learning dynamics develop relatively late. To test whether MGB responses had undergone changes not directly related to learning, we compared between the responses to tones in passively listening mice before and after mice learned the task. We found no differences between the MGB frequency tuning curves or the response to the go frequency during passive listening, before and after learning (Figure 4—figure supplement 1), indicating that MGB plasticity occurs specifically for task related signals, at least at the population level. In summary, we found that changes in MGB responses are positively correlated with learning as evident in similar modulation of the choice signal and the performance of individual mice.

MGB changes correspond with gaining expertise.
(A) MGB responses (averaged during late times, 0.6–1 s after stimulus onset; gray bars in B) as a function of learning in two example mice. Each MGB response curve was fitted with a sigmoid function (black lines). The learning thresholds are marked with a gray line. (B) Correlation coefficient as a function of time between the MGB response curve and the learning curve of the two example mice shown in ‘A’. Positive values indicate that MGB responses change in a similar way to the behavioral performance of the mouse. (C) Same as B, but averaged across all six mice. Error bars are s.e.m across mice. (D) Normalized fits of the MGB response curve for all mice. Dashed red line indicates threshold. (E) Maximal change in MBG responses (i.e. the trial number in which the MGB fit crossed 0.5, dots) superimposed on the learning thresholds (gray crosses).
Body movements affect MGB responses
The strong relationship between MGB plasticity and learning prompted us to further investigate other parameters that may affect learning-related modulations, specifically related to encoding of choice. Factors that could affect MGB responses are parameters related to movement (e.g. forelimb movement, nose twitching, whisking, licking etc.), which were recently found to have substantial impact on neuronal activity in different brain areas (Gilad et al., 2018; Gilad and Helmchen, 2020; Musall et al., 2019; Stringer et al., 2019). As mentioned above, we continuously monitored the body movements of the mouse throughout the experiment (Figures 1A and 5A). As expected, changes in body movements were correlated with learning. Around the time mice crossed the learning threshold, they exhibited more body movements as evident by plotting their movement probability (Figure 5A, right). Movement probability was high both within the stimulus period (0–1 s after stimulus onset) and, as expected, during the response period (Figure 5A,B). During pre-stimulus periods (−1 to 0 relative to stimulus onset) expert mice moved significantly less compared to the novice case, indicating a higher level of alertness (p<0.01; Signed rank test). Movement probability during stimulus presentation across learning was sigmoid-like, and significantly higher in experts compared to novice mice (Figure 5C,D; p<0.05; Signed rank test; n = 6 mice). Thus, as part of the development of choice responses in MGB, the body movements of mice during the task were also strongly related to the learning process.

Body movements affect MGB responses.
(A) Left: schematic illustration of mouse body movements during the task. Right: A 2-dimensional plot of movement probability during go trials within the trial (x-axis) and across learning (y-axis; 50 trial bins) for an example mouse. Dashed green line indicates stimulus onset (1 s duration) and dashed blue line indicates learning threshold. Color scale bar indicates min/max of movement probability. (B) Movement probability during the trial for the example mouse in A when it was novice (black) and expert (orange). (C) Movement probability (averaged during late times; 0.6–1 s after stimulus onset; go trials) across learning fitted with a sigmoid function. Same data as in ‘B’. (D) Movement probability during late times for novice and expert averaged across all six mice. Error bars are s.e.m across mice. *p<0.05. Signed-rank test. (E) Single trial traces of MGB calcium response (top), body movement (middle) and a binary movement vector (bottom; thresholding the body movement, dashed red line; Materials and methods). To exclude effects of body movements, MGB responses were truncated just before the movement onset on a trial-by-trial basis (gray traces in each trial). (F) MGB calcium responses for different trial types when including (left) and excluding (right) body movements. Data is from one example mouse (same as Figure 2B). Error bars are s.e.m across trials. (G) Mean MGB responses (averaged during stimulus presentation) for movement-free hit and miss trials per mouse. Data from expert mice. Error bars are s.e.m across trials. *p<0.05. **p<0.01. ***p<0.001. n.s. – not significant. n.a. – not available. Wilcoxon rank sum test.
The strong effects of movement suggest that the abovementioned MGB ‘choice’ signal may simply represent body movements rather than choice, per se. Indeed, some MGB responses and body movement covaried strongly whereas others did not (Figure 5E). Thus, to rule out the possibility that movement is the dominant feature in our calcium signal, we calculated 'movement free' MGB responses by detecting the movement onset in each trial and truncating the MGB signal when mice moved (gray traces in Figure 5E; see Materials and methods). This allowed us to evaluate separately MGB responses without the body movements. Movement-free MGB responses still encoded the choice of the mouse. Specifically, we found higher responses in hit compared to miss trials (Figure 5F). Notably, the effect size was smaller and levels of statistical significance were weaker when compared to the signal with movement (Figure 5G; compare to Figure 2C ‘Expert’). Importantly, when mice were still novice, movement-free MGB responses did not encode choice, indicating that choice encoding develops with learning, independent of movement (Figure 5—figure supplement 1A). Indeed, movement-free MGB responses were also enhanced as a function of learning, indicating that enhanced responses in MGB are related to a learning processes in ways that are not only directly linked to the movements of the mouse (Figure 5—figure supplement 1B,C). To rule out movement-related artifacts, we also imaged mice during the task while exciting the MGB with a wavelength that does not excite the calcium indicator (565 nm). This non-calcium signal was analyzed in a similar manner to the calcium signal and resulted in a relatively flat trace throughout the trial, showing that there are no movement related artifacts (data not shown). In summary, body movements during the task are evident in the calcium signal and are learning-related. Nevertheless, MGB responses still maintains choice information that is separate from the motor parameters and, critically, develops with learning. These data strengthen the claim that MGB encodes high-level information.
Learning-related changes in MGB differ in different frequency bands
As noted above, MGB responses were tuned to frequency (Figure 1F–I). We next tested whether the changes we observed in MGB are general or specific to the response properties at the range of frequencies we used. To this end, we trained three additional mice on the task. In these mice, the go frequency did not correspond to the best frequency. That is the underlying MGB population response was not highest for the target frequency (tuned away from the go frequency; Figure 6A shows one mouse with the go signal in the outskirts of the tuning curve; Figure 1—figure supplement 2B for the tuning curves of all three mice). Interestingly, MGB responses in all three mice still showed choice encoding but the absolute direction of change was in the opposite direction as compared to the first cohort of mice (compare Figure 6B to Figure 2C). Specifically, the MGB responses to the hit trials were now significantly lower than to the miss trials and this was evident in expert mice only (Figure 6B; p<0.001; Signed rank test for each mouse separately; minimum 24 trials for hit or miss). In expert mice, choice encoding gradually developed with time along the trial and was evident only in expert mice (Figure 6C). Here again, MGB response curves were sigmoid-like (Figure 6D) and strongly correlated with learning (Figure 6E). A more detailed description of these effects is shown in Figure 6—figure supplement 1. Taken together, we infer that learning enhances responses to the target frequency while simultaneously suppressing activity away from the target frequency. Notably, both phenomena still result in the same effect (i.e. encoding of choice), but likely exploit different mechanisms.

MGB responses tuned away from the go frequency are suppressed during learning.
(A) Frequency tuning curve for an example mouse with peak tuning frequency away from the go sound. (B) Mean MGB calcium response during stimulus presentation per mouse in hit and miss trials. Left- novice mice, Right- expert mice. Error bars are s.e.m across trials. ***p<0.001. n.s. – not significant. Wilcoxon rank sum test. (C) Choice responses, averaged across the three mice that are tuned away from the go frequency, for the expert (solid line) and novice (dashed line) conditions. Gray traces are for the go-tuned mice, same as Figure 2D. (D) MGB response curve along learning in one mouse that is tuned away from the go frequency (compare to Figure 4A). (E) Correlation between MGB response curves and learning curves as a function of time, averaged across the three mice. Error bars are s.e.m across mice. Gray traces are for the go-tuned mice, same as Figure 4C.
Discussion
We describe learning-related changes in evoked responses to sounds in the auditory thalamus. We find that auditory thalamus encodes the choice of the mouse several hundred milliseconds (~200 ms; Figure 3C) after sound onset, by specifically enhancing activity to the go frequency as learning proceeds. Thus, late thalamic responses to the same sound are different depending on its learned state. MGB responses alone allowed us to predict with high precision when each mouse learned the task. Taken together, we provide strong evidence relating MGB plasticity to learning a simple auditory discrimination task.
Higher-order thalamic nuclei are part of a thalamocortical loop that are suggested to encode high-order cognitive information (Bolkan et al., 2017; Guo et al., 2017; Schmitt et al., 2017). Choice encoding in the thalamus (Chen et al., 2019; Gimenez et al., 2015; Jaramillo et al., 2014) bears similarity to the observations made in the cortex (Gilad et al., 2018; Guo et al., 2019; Harvey et al., 2012; Pho et al., 2018; Siegel et al., 2015; Yang et al., 2016), indicating the intimate involvement of thalamocortical loops during learning. The appearance of learning related choice encoding was consistent across mice and evident at the population level (Figure 3). Since we are unable to dissect the fiber photometry signal to its single cell components, we can only speculate about the nature of cells encoding the choice. The increase in choice signal could arise from small contributions of a large fraction of MGB neurons or from large contributions of a smaller population. A previous study found that neurons in the MGB (and similarly in cortex) mediate choice but to a limited extent, whereas only a small fraction (16%) of recorded neurons encoded choice information, supporting the latter possibility (Jaramillo et al., 2014). One noted difference is that Jaramillo et al., 2014 recorded responses from all MGB sub-nuclei, including ventral MGB, while our measurements were more localized to the medial and dorsal MGB. Ventral MGB might not encode choice, and thus previous work may have underestimated the contribution of single neurons in the non-lemniscal pathway in signaling choice. In fact, further functional dissection is warranted. For example, dorsal MGB will likely be affected more prominently by motor parameters compared to the medial MGB (Bartlett et al., 2000; Huang and Winer, 2000; Lee, 2015). Further, specific populations of MGB neurons that project to different targets will likely transfer specific learning-related information in a selective manner Chen et al., 2019; as has been shown for the enhancement of choice information in the cortico-striatal pathway (Guo et al., 2019). Simultaneous recordings, preferably with electrophysiology, with tight regulation of the information transfer between thalamus and cortex will be instrumental in deciphering the origin of choice information.
The observed changes in MGB points to pronounced plasticity during the training phase. The mechanisms of this plasticity are unknown and may involve several factors such as inhibitory effects, changes in excitation-inhibition balance, and other forms of synaptic plasticity in specific pathways to the MBG (Audette et al., 2019; McAlonan et al., 2008; Rose and Bonhoeffer, 2018). Learning-related dynamics have been shown to involve disparate circuit elements such as deep cortical layers (Audette et al., 2019; Olsen et al., 2012), specific inhibitory neuron subtypes (Hashikawa et al., 1991; Khan et al., 2018; Peruzzi et al., 1997) or other thalamic sub-nuclei (Audette et al., 2019; Bennett et al., 2019). Given that cortex encodes choice, higher-order thalamocortical connections are good candidates to drive synaptic plasticity in cortex during learning (Audette et al., 2019). It is unknown whether the synaptic changes from the thalamus to the cortex are the source of change in the MGB or whether corticothalamic feedback is the source. The prolonged latency of choice development in our study, implies an involvement of a late component that may suggest top-down feedback interaction. In addition, an interesting sub-thalamic nucleus that may be involved in gating higher-order information is the thalamic reticular nucleus (TRN) which sends GABAergic projections to MGB (Crabtree, 1998; Lee, 2015). TRN could gate incoming sensory information, where in the novice case it is expected to exert strong inhibition to MGB which is then decreased with learning; thus enabling enhanced activation to the go sound already at the level of the MGB (McAlonan et al., 2008; Nakajima et al., 2019). Such a mechanism may contribute to the enhanced discrimination between go tuned and no-go tuned recordings sites in the MGB of expert mice. Selective disinhibition or inhibition could enhance go-tuned neuronal populations or suppress no-go tuned populations, respectively.
Local and global functional connectivity are rich in cortical networks, including auditory cortex (Rothschild et al., 2010; Maor et al., 2016). Selective changes in local and global functional connectivity based on task demands is not unprecedented, and the mechanisms underlying it has been attributed to the thalamus (Nakajima and Halassa, 2017). We show that as mice learn the task, the target frequency enhances activity in tuned population whereas non-tuned populations are simultaneously suppressed. This phenomenon may make the target stimulus more salient based on context and bear similarities to other enhancement/suppression mechanisms found in visual thalamus (Fisher et al., 2017; Jones et al., 2012; Nakajima et al., 2019; Wilke et al., 2009). Comparing the different MGB subdivisions, we hypothesize that that medial and dorsal MGB neurons encoding non-target frequencies will be suppressed, whereas the ventral MGB may continue with a similar response profile for all frequencies. Since higher-order MGB projects to higher-order auditory cortex (part of the non-lemniscal), this information may bypass incoming information from the lemniscal pathway and may aid in increasing discrimination performance.
We find that motor parameters, such as body movements, are also learning-related. As mice learn to associate between the go sound and a reward, they increase their body movement in preparation for licking and the upcoming reward, well within the stimulation period. MGB responses were also affected by body movements to different extents, as might be expected by the motor efferent cortical feedback from layer 5 (Bartlett et al., 2000; Lee, 2015; Llano and Sherman, 2008). Another possibility is that movement related encoding may ascend from the inferior colliculus, an upstream area (Gruters and Groh, 2012; Yang et al., 2020). The fact that MGB still encodes choice after truncating movement signals, indicates that the MGB does not inherit all task information from the inferior colliculus, but rather integrates information coming from both top-down and bottom-up inputs. The effect of motor parameters on neuronal signals were recently observed in a brain-wide manner, emphasizing the need to strictly monitor and consider motor parameters during different tasks (Gilad et al., 2018; Gilad and Helmchen, 2020; Musall et al., 2019; Stringer et al., 2019); and particularly so when measuring higher-order information. In summary, we show that the auditory thalamus encodes high-order information during the time course of learning, implying that the learning process requires brain-wide activity spanning across both cortical and subcoritcal regions.
Materials and methods
Reagent type (species) or resource | Designation | Source or reference | Identifiers | Additional information |
---|---|---|---|---|
Transfected construct (Adeno-associated virus) | pAAV.Syn.GCaMP6f.WPRE.SV40 | Addgene | RRID:Addgene_100837 | |
Software, algorithm | Matlab, Labview | Mathworks, National Instruments |
Animals
A total of n = 9, 8–16 week-old female C57BL/6 mice were used in this work. All experiments were approved by Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (IACUC) at the Hebrew University of Jerusalem, Israel (Permit Number: NS-19-15706-4).
Surgery
Request a detailed protocolTo express a calcium indicator in MGB neurons and implant an optical fiber for imaging, mice were anesthetized with 2% isoflurane (in pure O2) and body temperature was maintained at 37°C. We applied local anesthesia to the area of surgery (lidocaine 1%), exposed and cleaned the skull. Next, we drilled a small hole in the skull and injected 240 nl of an AAV virus pAAV.Syn.GCaMP6f.WPRE.SV40 (AAV9; from Addgene) using a glass pipette, to the dorsal or medial part of the MGB (3.3 mm posterior to bregma; 1.9 mm lateral to bregma; 3 mm in depth). Using the same coordinates, we then inserted a 400 µm optical fiber with an attached cannula (CFMC14L05; Thorlabs), directly above the injection site (2.95 mm deep). We chronically fixed the fiber position using dental cement that was mixed with a few drops of superglue for extra strength and stability. Finally, a metal post for head fixation was glued onto the bone in the back side of the right hemisphere. This procedure enabled a chronic and reliable imaging of population calcium responses from the MGB.
Auditory discrimination task
Request a detailed protocolMice were trained on a go/no-go auditory discrimination task (Figure 1A). We used the exact same hardware and software of an automated behavior system we recently described called the “Educage” (see details in Maor et al., 2020). Unlike the Educage where mice are free to train from their home, here, we trained mice on this system while head fixed in an isolated acoustic chamber. In short, each trial started with a visual start cue (orange LED placed in front of the mouse; duration 0.1 s; 2 s before stimulus onset) followed by an auditory stimulus, either a go or a no-go pure tone (Figure 1A; duration 1 s; 62 dBSPL; sounds separated by 0.5 octave). All mice were trained on 10 kHz for go and 7.1 kHz for no-go except for mouse #3 which was trained on 8.2 kHz for go and 5.8 kHz for the no-go. After stimulus offset, we counted licks in a virtual response window of 3 s. Licking in response to the go sound were counted as ‘hit’ trials and rewarded with a drop of water. Withholding licking in response to the no-go sound were counted as correct rejection trials (CR), and were not punished or reinforced. Licking in response to the no-go sound were false alarm trials (FA) that were followed by a mild punishment of white noise (duration of 3 s). Withholding licking for the go sound were counted as misses, and were not punished. The licking detector remained in a fixed and reachable position throughout the entire trial and mice were free to lick at any time. Licking before the response cue was allowed and did not lead to punishment or early reward. Note that the visual cue merely signals the start of the trial, but had no predictive power with respect to go or no-go condition.
Training and performance
Request a detailed protocolNine mice were trained on the task. Mice were first handled and accustomed to head fixation before starting the schedule of water restriction. Before imaging began mice were conditioned to lick for reward after the go sound (presented within a similar trial structure as the task itself). Imaging began only after mice reliably licked for the presented sound (typically after the 1 st day; 200–400 trials). On the first day of imaging, mice were presented with the go sound for 50 consecutive trials, after which the no-go sound was gradually introduced (starting from 10% and increasing by 10% approximately every 50 trials). By the end of the 1 st day, the no-go sound reached 50% probability (Guo et al., 2014). During the 2nd day, most mice continuously licked for both sounds. Thus, after roughly 100 trials, we increased no-go probability to 80% and waited for mice to perform three consecutive CR trials before returning to 50% probability. This was done for several times until mice increased their performance, specifically learning to withhold licking for the no-go sound. In mice that still continued to lick for both sounds we also repeated the no-go sound several times until the mouse performed correctly. In all mice, a 50–50% protocol was reached typically on the 1st or 2nd day. Most mice learned the task within 3–7 days corresponding to roughly 1200–2000 trials (Figure 1D). An effort was made to maintain a constant position of the mouse, speaker and cameras across imaging days in order to maintain similar stimulation and imaging conditions.
Optical fiber setup
Request a detailed protocolAs mice trained on and performed the task we continuously imaged MGB population responses. A ferrule patch cable (M79L01; Thorlabs) was connected to the implanted cannula using a mating sleeve (ADAF1; Thorlabs) and the cable end was connected (SMA connector) to an optical imaging setup (FOM-2; MCI), enabling us to excite the MGB at 470 nm (blue; 0.3 mW power output kept constant throughout the experiments) and collect emission light at 520 nm (green). As a control, we also imaged mice during the task while exciting the MGB with a wavelength that does not excite the calcium indicator (565 nm).
Histology
Request a detailed protocolMice were given an overdose of Pental and were perfused transcardially with phosphate-buffered saline (PBS) followed by 4% paraformaldehyde (PFA) in PBS. Brains were post-fixed for 12–24 hr in 4% PFA in PBS and then cryoprotected for >24 hr in 30% sucrose in PBS. Then, 100 μm coronal slices of the entire brain were made using a freezing microtome (Leica SM 2000R), incubated for 15 min in 2.5 μg/ml of DAPI (4',6-diamidino-2-phenylindole), mounted onto glass slides, and imaged using an Olympus IX-81 epi-fluorescent microscope with a 4 × and 10 × objective lens (0.16 and 0.3 NA; Olympus). Fiber tracks and GCaMP6f expression were detected in all mice and were mainly localized to the MGB (Figure 1C).
Body tracking
Request a detailed protocolIn addition to fiber photometry, we tracked body movements of the mouse during the task (Figures 1B and 5A). The mouse was illuminated with a 940 nm infra-red LED. A body camera monitored the movements of the mouse at 30 Hz (The Imaging Source; DMK 23UV024 camera). We used movements of both forelimbs, neck and jaw regions to assess body movements (Figure 1A; see Data Analysis below). Mice performed the task in the dark.
Data analysis
Request a detailed protocolData analyses and statistics were performed using custom-written code in MATLAB. For MGB signals, each trial was normalized to baseline several frames before the visual cue (frame 0 division). We did not find a substantial difference in baseline fluorescence across imaging days and also within an imaging day, implying there was no over bleaching of the signal. This enabled us to quantitatively compare ΔF/F signals across long time periods. To better control for possible changes in MGB signal across days, we tested for frequency tuning (playing a range of frequencies 4–40 kHz; 10, 20, 30, 40 dB attenuations relative to 62 dB; duration 0.1 s) in each mouse, both before and after training. We did not see a significant difference in ΔF/F response between the cases (Figure 4—figure supplement 1; p>0.05; n=6 mice; Signed rank test).
Next, we divide trials based either on stimuli (i.e. go or no-go) or on choice (i.e. lick or no-lick per stimulus type). MGB ΔF/F signals were plotted in two dimensional temporal spaces where the x-axis is the trial temporal structure, and the y-axis is the learning profile across trials and days (Figure 2A). From this 2D temporal space we averaged across trials during the novice and expert phases (defined as the first and last 500 trials respectively; Figure 2B). Alternatively, we averaged across time frames within the trial structure, to obtain an MGB response curve across learning (Figure 4A; see below).
Discrimination power between different trial types
Request a detailed protocolTo measure how well could neuronal populations discriminate between trial types (i.e. hit, miss, CR and FA), we calculated a receiver operating characteristics (ROC) curve between the distribution of a pair of trial types and calculated its area under the curve (AUC). We define two different AUC measures by comparing hit trials to either FA (stim AUC; magenta; i.e. different stimuli but similar choice) or to Miss (choice AUC; green; i.e. similar stimuli but different choice) trials (Figure 3A). The AUC value ranges from 0 to 1 and quantifies the accuracy of an ideal observer. AUC values close to 0.5 indicate low discrimination whereas values near away from 0.5 indicate high discrimination. In addition, we calculated AUC values for each time frame along the trial for the novice and expert conditions separately (i.e. first and last 500 trials). To assess significance, we calculated the sample distribution by trial shuffling between go and no-go sounds (n = 100 iterations). When the signal exceeded the mean ±3 std of the sample distribution it was defined as significant (Figure 3B, arrows).
Calculation of learning curve and MGB response curves
Request a detailed protocolTo calculate the learning curve for each mouse, trials were binned (n = 50 trials with no overlap) across learning and the performance (defined as d’=Z(hit/(hit+miss)) – Z(FA/(FA+CR)) where Z denotes the inverse of the cumulative distribution function) was calculated for each bin. Next, each behavioral learning curve was fitted with a sigmoid function
Where a denotes the amplitude, b the time point (in trial numbers) of the inflection point, and c the steepness of the sigmoid. A d’=1 was defined as the performance threshold and mice were ordered based on the trial number in which they crossed threshold (i.e. learning threshold; Figure 1E). Different pthresholds did not change the order with which mice learned (see Figure 1E).
To compare the behavioral learning curve with responses in the MGB we calculated the mean MGB response across learning (in the same 50 trial bins as the learning curve), averaged during late times (0.6–1 s after stimulus onset; 0.2–0.5 s for m2). Our main focus in this study was on the go sound (hit and miss trials grouped together). Therefore, stimulus identity was kept similar across learning. MGB response curves were also fitted with a sigmoid function (black curves in Figure 4A; Curves were smoothed with a Gaussian kernel (2σ = 9) for visualization only). The sigmoid fits of the MGB response curves were normalized between 0 and 1 in order to compare between curves from different mice. In addition, we calculated the MGB response curve for each time frame separately. This was correlated with the fixed learning curve of the mouse, to obtain a time course (within the trial) correlation coefficient value between the MGB response curve and the learning curve (Figure 4B; no smoothing applied).
Calculating body movements
Request a detailed protocolWe used a body camera to detect general movements of the mouse (30 Hz frame rate; Figures 1A and 5A). For each imaging day, we first outlined the forelimbs, neck and mouth areas (one area of interest for each), which were reliable areas to detect general movements. Next, we calculated the body movement (one minus frame-to-frame correlation) within these areas as a function of time for each trial. Thresholding at 3 s.d. (across time frames before stimulus cue) above baseline resulted in a binary movement vector (either ‘moving’ or ‘quiet’) for each trial (Gilad et al., 2018; Gilad and Helmchen, 2020). This movement analysis was very strict, where any movement of the forelimb, neck or jaw would be flagged as a period of movement. A detailed movement analysis of each body part is presented in Figure 5—figure supplement 2. In addition, the licking onset was always later compared to the movement onset, since one of the detection areas was the jaw area. This was done for each trial to achieve a 2D space of movement probability within the trial temporal structure (x-axis) versus the learning process (i.e. trial number; y-axis; Figure 5A). To obtain MGB signals that are ‘movement-free’, that is do not contain direct effect of body movements, we detected the first movement onset for each trial, defined as 0.2 s before crossing the movement threshold (Figure 5E). Next, MGB signals were truncated from movement onset and onwards in a single trial manner. This analysis resulted in MGB trials, each with a different length, that did not contain body movements (Figure 5F,G). Similar results were obtained using other body parts such as the nose and mouth (including licking).
Statistical analysis
Request a detailed protocolIn general, non-parametric two-tailed statistical tests were used. The Whitney rank sum test was used to compare between two medians from two populations and the Wilcoxon signed rank test was used to compare a population's median to zero (or between two paired populations). Multiple group correction was used when comparing between more than two groups. Significance was set at p=0.05.
Data availability
The data and custom code that support the findings of this study are publicly available at: https://osf.io/mt3bc.
-
Open Science FrameworkID 10.17605/osf.io/mt3bc. Auditory thalamus - fiber photometry.
References
-
Thalamic projections sustain prefrontal activity during working memory maintenanceNature Neuroscience 20:987–996.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4568
-
BookIntegrative Functions in the Mammalian Auditory PathwayIn: Oertel D, Popper AN, Fay RR, editors. The Inferior Colliculus: A Hub for the Central Auditory System. Springer. pp. 238–318.https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4757-3654-0_7
-
Pathway-specific reorganization of projection neurons in somatosensory cortex during learningNature Neuroscience 18:1101–1108.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4046
-
Anatomy of the rat medial geniculate body: I. cytoarchitecture, Myeloarchitecture, and neocortical connectivityThe Journal of Comparative Neurology 297:14–31.https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.902970103
-
Organization in the auditory sector of the cat's thalamic reticular nucleusThe Journal of Comparative Neurology 390:167–182.https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1096-9861(19980112)390:2<167::AID-CNE1>3.0.CO;2-#
-
Learning in mammalian sensory cortexCurrent Opinion in Neurobiology 14:513–518.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2004.07.003
-
Adaptive categorization of sound frequency does not require the auditory cortex in ratsJournal of Neurophysiology 114:1137–1145.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00124.2015
-
Sounds and beyond: multisensory and other non-auditory signals in the inferior colliculusFrontiers in Neural Circuits 6:96.https://doi.org/10.3389/fncir.2012.00096
-
Choice-Selective neurons in the auditory cortex and in its striatal target encode reward expectationThe Journal of Neuroscience 39:3687–3697.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2585-18.2019
-
Linking topography to tonotopy in the mouse auditory thalamocortical circuitJournal of Neuroscience 31:2983–2995.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5333-10.2011
-
Encoding of fear learning and memory in distributed neuronal circuitsNature Neuroscience 17:1644–1654.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3869
-
Auditory thalamocortical projections in the cat: laminar and areal patterns of inputThe Journal of Comparative Neurology 427:302–331.https://doi.org/10.1002/1096-9861(20001113)427:2<302::AID-CNE10>3.0.CO;2-J
-
Tonotopic organization in ventral nucleus of medial geniculate body in the catJournal of Neurophysiology 53:309–340.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.1985.53.1.309
-
Learning enhances sensory processing in mouse V1 before improving behaviorThe Journal of Neuroscience 37:6460–6474.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3485-16.2017
-
Exploring functions for the non-lemniscal auditory thalamusFrontiers in Neural Circuits 9:1–8.https://doi.org/10.3389/fncir.2015.00069
-
Evidence for nonreciprocal organization of the mouse auditory thalamocortical-corticothalamic projection systemsThe Journal of Comparative Neurology 507:1209–1227.https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.21602
-
Learning enhances the relative impact of top-down processing in the visual cortexNature Neuroscience 18:1116–1122.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4061
-
Neural correlates of learning pure tones or natural sounds in the auditory cortexFrontiers in Neural Circuits 13:82.https://doi.org/10.3389/fncir.2019.00082
-
Neuronal signalling of fear memoryNature Reviews Neuroscience 5:844–852.https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1535
-
A sensorimotor pathway via Higher-Order thalamusThe Journal of Neuroscience 39:692–704.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1467-18.2018
-
Single-trial neural dynamics are dominated by richly varied movementsNature Neuroscience 22:1677–1686.https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-019-0502-4
-
Thalamic control of functional cortical connectivityCurrent Opinion in Neurobiology 44:127–131.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2017.04.001
-
Learning-induced plasticity in animal and human auditory cortexCurrent Opinion in Neurobiology 15:470–477.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2005.07.002
-
A monosynaptic GABAergic input from the inferior colliculus to the medial geniculate body in ratThe Journal of Neuroscience 17:3766–3777.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-10-03766.1997
-
Perceptual learning directs auditory cortical map reorganization through top-down influencesJournal of Neuroscience 26:4970–4982.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3771-05.2006
-
Equipotentiality of thalamo-amygdala and thalamo-cortico-amygdala circuits in auditory fear conditioningThe Journal of Neuroscience 12:4501–4509.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.12-11-04501.1992
-
Experience-dependent plasticity in the lateral geniculate nucleusCurrent Opinion in Neurobiology 53:22–28.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2018.04.016
-
Thalamic nuclei convey diverse contextual information to layer 1 of visual cortexNature Neuroscience 19:299–307.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4197
-
Functional organization and population dynamics in the mouse primary auditory cortexNature Neuroscience 13:353–360.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.2484
-
The cognitive thalamusFrontiers in Systems Neuroscience 9:39.https://doi.org/10.3389/fnsys.2015.00039
-
Thalamocortical projections to rat auditory cortex from the ventral and dorsal divisions of the medial geniculate nucleusThe Journal of Comparative Neurology 520:34–51.https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.22682
-
The thalamus: gateway to the mindWiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science 4:609–622.https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.1256
-
Changes in S1 neural responses during tactile discrimination learningJournal of Neurophysiology 104:300–312.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00194.2010
-
BookThe Mammalian Auditory Pathway: NeuroanatomyIn: Webster DB, Popper AN, Fay RR, editors. The Functional Architecture of the Medial Geniculate Body and the Primary Auditory Cortex. Springer. pp. 222–409.https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4612-4416-5_6
-
Projections of auditory cortex to the medial geniculate body of the catThe Journal of Comparative Neurology 430:27–55.https://doi.org/10.1002/1096-9861(20010129)430:1<27::AID-CNE1013>3.0.CO;2-8
-
Perceptual training continuously refines neuronal population codes in primary visual cortexNature Neuroscience 17:1380–1387.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3805
-
Origins of choice-related activity in mouse somatosensory cortexNature Neuroscience 19:127–134.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4183
Decision letter
-
Brice BathellierReviewing Editor; CNRS, France
-
Andrew J KingSenior Editor; University of Oxford, United Kingdom
-
Brice BathellierReviewer; CNRS, France
-
Tania Rinaldi BarkatReviewer; Basel University, Switzerland
-
Michael PeckaReviewer; Ludwig-Maximilians Universitaet Muenchen, Germany
In the interests of transparency, eLife publishes the most substantive revision requests and the accompanying author responses.
Acceptance summary:
This longitudinal photometric study elegantly shows the existence of choice-related signals in the mouse auditory thalamus which develop together with learning. This indicates that choice signals not only reflect the decision of the animal but also the learnt association between sounds and actions. This is a highly interesting conclusion corroborated by the striking observation that the direction of choice signals depends on sound tuning.
Decision letter after peer review:
Thank you for submitting your article "Learning-related population dynamics in the auditory thalamus" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by three peer reviewers, including Brice Bathellier as the Reviewing Editor and Reviewer #1, and the evaluation has been overseen by Andrew King as the Senior Editor. The following individuals involved in review of your submission have agreed to reveal their identity: Tania Rinaldi Barkat (Reviewer #2); Michael Pecka (Reviewer #3).
The reviewers have discussed the reviews with one another and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this decision to help you prepare a revised submission.
We would like to draw your attention to changes in our revision policy that we have made in response to COVID-19 (https://elifesciences.org/articles/57162). Specifically, when editors judge that a submitted work as a whole belongs in eLife but that some conclusions require a modest amount of additional new data, as they do with your paper, we are asking that the manuscript be revised to either limit claims to those supported by data in hand, or to explicitly state that the relevant conclusions require additional supporting data.
Our expectation is that the authors will eventually carry out the additional experiments and report on how they affect the relevant conclusions either in a preprint on bioRxiv or medRxiv, or if appropriate, as a Research Advance in eLife, either of which would be linked to the original paper.
Summary:
Using fiber photometry in the medial geniculate body, the authors follow the activity of the non-lemniscal auditory thalamus throughout learning of a Go/No-Go discrimination task using measurements of GCaM6f fluorescence. They find that (i) thalamic responses are affected by choice and that this cannot only be explained by increased movements, (ii) that choice effects are greater after learning and follow the learning time course, (iii) that choice effects are of opposite signs in regions tuned or non-tuned to the Go stimulus.
Result (i) is not novel as it was known that neurons in the thalamus show choice-related activity. Thus, the main novelty lies in results (ii) and (iii), which are very interesting. However, some concerns about these results need to be addressed by new analyses, to secure and strengthen the conclusions.
Essential revisions:
1) The authors provide an interesting analysis of the movements of the animal, showing that movements increase when the animal learns the task. The authors show that compensating for movements maintains the modulation of activity in the thalamus by choice. However, the effect is weakened in the absence of movements. Given that naive mice show less movements and also weaker choice-related activity, it is critical to show that increased modulation by choice in expert mice is not just due to movement. Hence, the learning analysis should be re-done with the same movement correction.
2) It is disappointing that there is much less choice modulation of thalamic activity on No-Go trials (Figure 3—figure supplement 2). Maybe this is because there is less movement of the animals on No-Go trials. Related to the last point, it is would be good to show that even if the choice-related effects are smaller, they are also learnt. Also, the data in Figure 3—figure supplement 2 is presented in AUC. Please also show it in a format closer to the raw data (i.e. DF/F, as in Figure 2 for hit and miss trials).
3) The latency of choice-related signals should be compared to discrimination latencies in behavior (i.e. time at which the Go and No-Go stimuli generate different licking distributions). This would help to show whether this signal could be causal to choice or just a posterior signal. Also some supplementary examples show choice signals that drop below significance again before the end of the sound presentation, so one wonders whether this signal change was represented in the behavior of the mice (longer response latency etc)?
4) It is not really clear in which regions of the auditory thalamus the recordings were made. Based on histology (Figure 1C), it seems that most recordings are from the medial or the dorsal part of MGB. However, the authors also claim that, based on the tuning profiles, the recordings might be from the ventral part (subsection “Calcium imaging from the MGB along learning”). As the ventral MGB is part of the lemniscal pathway, whereas the dorsal and medial MGB are part of the non-lemniscal pathway, knowing where the activity comes from changes the way the data have to be interpreted. Could it also be that the signal is contaminated by inputs coming from other areas, such as the cortex? It is clear how difficult it can be to distinguish the different subdivisions of the MGB, and that additional experiments (for example by silencing cortex to remove cortical feedback in the recorded responses) to clearly answer this would be beyond the scope of this paper. However, the text should be revised to address this more clearly and to avoid confusion.
5) Related to the previous comment – it is surprising that many tuning curves show best frequencies around 10kHz. Could that be a hint of where the recordings were obtained, or where the activity comes from? If the recordings were made at such diverse locations as indicated in Figure 1C, wouldn't you have expected more diversity in the tuning curves (Figure 1—figure supplement 2, Figure 4—figure supplement 1 and Figure 6)?
6) The question of movement-related activity is very interesting (Figure 5). However, the definition of movement onset is not convincing. To our understanding, it is based on the detection of forelimb and neck muscle movements. However, many other movements could be happening earlier. How does this movement onset align, for example, to lick onset (which has been shown in previous studies to happen 200-300ms after tone onset, much earlier than the movement onset shown in Figure 5E)? What about the contribution of movement planning? How does the movement before sound presentation compare in naïve vs. expert mice?
7) The results from Figure 6 are intriguing. How can you explain that the results are frequency dependent? And if it is only neurons with the BF at the GO tone that increase their activity upon learning, whereas the neurons with a BF different from the GO frequency decrease their activity, how would this look at the whole MGB population level?
8) The general introduction is a bit confusing regarding the overall scope/relevance of the study. The functional dichotomy that the authors describe in the Abstract and Introduction to exist between cortex and thalamus seems to be oversimplified / overstated. The field today largely regards the recurrent networks between thalamus and cortex as a functional unit, and that the idea of a simple relay station for the thalamus is outdated. Likewise, the authors correctly note that findings of choice coding like the one presented here were already made in cortex and even in MGB. Hence, as discussed also by the authors, the strong inter-connectivity of MGB with both cortex and midbrain (where task-specific modulation of responses can already be found) make it not very surprising that MGB is involved as well. Yet the Introduction largely lacks an explanation of this high degree of recurrent connectivity and questions "whether MGB encodes learning related modulation beyond the fear system". The authors should provide a more accurate and nuanced description of what is known and what to expect.
Revisions expected in follow-up work:
Related to point 8 above, the novelty in the presented data is not that one can observe learning-related modulation in MGB per se, but the rather direct demonstration of how it might contribute to learning associated plasticity. To further reinforce this point, two questions should be addressed:
1) Can the same phenomenon be observed in A1 or associated cortices of the non-lemniscal pathway, and if so, where do they arise first? The authors do discuss this issue; however one remains wondering if (or to what extent) the reported findings are reflections of cortical processing, so if available, some data on this question would be very useful.
2) How do performance and MGB responses develop a few weeks after mice have learned the task but were not further trained, i.e. to what extent have performance and neural coding of choice crystallized and thus reflect a form of long-term plasticity associated with learning?
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.56307.sa1Author response
Essential revisions:
1) The authors provide an interesting analysis of the movements of the animal, showing that movements increase when the animal learns the task. The authors show that compensating for movements maintains the modulation of activity in the thalamus by choice. However, the effect is weakened in the absence of movements. Given that naive mice show less movements and also weaker choice-related activity, it is critical to show that increased modulation by choice in expert mice is not just due to movement. Hence, the learning analysis should be re-done with the same movement correction.
In the original draft we have already shown that movement-free MGB signal encodes the choice of the mouse (former Supplementary Figure 6, currently Figure 5—figure supplement 1). Following this comment, we have now re-done the learning analysis using the movement-free signals, showing that MGB enhancement is maintained. The results have been added as new Figure 5—figure supplement 1B, C; subsection “Body movements affect MGB responses”.
2) It is disappointing that there is much less choice modulation of thalamic activity on No-Go trials (Figure 3—figure supplement 2). Maybe this is because there is less movement of the animals on No-Go trials. Related to the last point, it is would be good to show that even if the choice-related effects are smaller, they are also learnt. Also, the data in Figure 3—figure supplement 2 is presented in AUC. Please also show it in a format closer to the raw data (i.e. DF/F, as in Figure 2 for hit and miss trials).
We have now added the MGB responses (ΔF/F) for all six mice for all trial types (see new Figure 2—figure supplement 1). Figure 3—figure supplements 1 and 2 show the AUC values for all response types. In fact, we don’t find this result surprising mainly due to the fact that our recording site is, by design, strong to the ‘go’ sound. Responses to the no-go sound are either weak or non-existent. In mice with a response to the no-go sound (mice #4 and #5), choice encoding did indeed develop with learning. In terms of movement, mice move more in FA compared to CR trials, similar to movement differences in Hit vs. Miss, indicating that movement per se cannot fully explain the weak choice encoding in the No-go trials. We refer to this in the main text (see subsection “MGB encodes sounds early and choices late”).
3) The latency of choice-related signals should be compared to discrimination latencies in behavior (i.e. time at which the Go and No-Go stimuli generate different licking distributions). This would help to show whether this signal could be causal to choice or just a posterior signal. Also some supplementary examples show choice signals that drop below significance again before the end of the sound presentation, so one wonders whether this signal change was represented in the behavior of the mice (longer response latency etc)?
Done. We find that the onset of licking was significantly later than the onset of choice encoding, implying a possible causal relationship. We added it only as text (see subsection “MGB encodes sounds early and choices late”). An example from one mouse (mouse #1) is shown in Author response image 1. In case the reviewers think it is critical to add another supplementary figure with this kind of data we will gladly do it.
4) It is not really clear in which regions of the auditory thalamus the recordings were made. Based on histology (Figure 1C), it seems that most recordings are from the medial or the dorsal part of MGB. However, the authors also claim that, based on the tuning profiles, the recordings might be from the ventral part (subsection “Calcium imaging from the MGB along learning”). As the ventral MGB is part of the lemniscal pathway, whereas the dorsal and medial MGB are part of the non-lemniscal pathway, knowing where the activity comes from changes the way the data have to be interpreted. Could it also be that the signal is contaminated by inputs coming from other areas, such as the cortex? It is clear how difficult it can be to distinguish the different subdivisions of the MGB, and that additional experiments (for example by silencing cortex to remove cortical feedback in the recorded responses) to clearly answer this would be beyond the scope of this paper. However, the text should be revised to address this more clearly and to avoid confusion.
5) Related to the previous comment – it is surprising that many tuning curves show best frequencies around 10kHz. Could that be a hint of where the recordings were obtained, or where the activity comes from? If the recordings were made at such diverse locations as indicated in Figure 1C, wouldn't you have expected more diversity in the tuning curves (Figure 1—figure supplement 2, Figure 4—figure supplement 1 and Figure 6)?
Indeed, the limited resolution of fiber photometry in combination with bulk viral injections precludes a decisive determination of the exact source of the signal. The histology shows that we were recording from the non-lemniscal pathway, and closer examination of the full FRAs, shows that we picked up signal ranging 1.4-3.8 octaves across mice. Thus, the tuning curves that we show are slightly biased towards highly tuned responses, while responses are in fact broader. That said, we cannot exclude contribution of feedback or even some leak from MGBv, though we think the latter is less likely. We now refer to this issue in the text explicitly (see subsection “Calcium imaging from the MGB along learning using fiber photometry”).
6) The question of movement-related activity is very interesting (Figure 5). However, the definition of movement onset is not convincing. To our understanding, it is based on the detection of forelimb and neck muscle movements. However, many other movements could be happening earlier. How does this movement onset align, for example, to lick onset (which has been shown in previous studies to happen 200-300ms after tone onset, much earlier than the movement onset shown in Figure 5E)? What about the contribution of movement planning? How does the movement before sound presentation compare in naïve vs. expert mice?
The reviewer is correct that licking started several hundred ms after tone onset. Our movement analysis is actually strict and more stringent than common analyses of licking onset. Any movement of the forelimb, neck or jaw is flagged as a period of movement. To explain this analysis further, we now added an additional supplementary figure with new analysis that we hope clarifies this further (see new Figure 5—figure supplement 2, and subsection “Calculating body movements”). New Figure 5—figure supplement 2 show a more systematic view of our analysis which is, again, more stringent as compared to a standard licking analysis (see also point 3 above). We prefer to leave the examples in Figure 5E as they show the diversity of movement and exemplify the sensitivity of our measurement.
Pre-stimulus movement was generally low. In a new analysis, we did not detect much movement in expert mice during the pre-stimulus period (-1 sec to stimulus onset), which was significantly weaker than in naives. This is now clarified in the text (see subsection “Body movements affect MGB responses”).
7) The results from Figure 6 are intriguing. How can you explain that the results are frequency dependent? And if it is only neurons with the BF at the GO tone that increase their activity upon learning, whereas the neurons with a BF different from the GO frequency decrease their activity, how would this look at the whole MGB population level?
We agree that this result is intriguing. Since the number of mice here is small (n=3), it primarily calls for attending to this issue in future experiments rather than being at the core of the paper. Note that since fiber photometry’s signal arises from many neurons it’s impossible to tease apart single-neuron BF responses and we can only speculate about the type of change the MGB undergoes as a whole (see also point 4 and 5 above). Our interpretation is intended to stay at face value. That is, the total response output of the MGB as a whole remains constant whereas go- and no-go tuned neurons are selectively increased or decreased, respectively. Mechanistic explanation to such phenomena is beyond the scope of this paper, but have been suggested elsewhere, and included in the third paragraph of the Discussion.
8) The general introduction is a bit confusing regarding the overall scope/relevance of the study. The functional dichotomy that the authors describe in the Abstract and Introduction to exist between cortex and thalamus seems to be oversimplified / overstated. The field today largely regards the recurrent networks between thalamus and cortex as a functional unit, and that the idea of a simple relay station for the thalamus is outdated. Likewise, the authors correctly note that findings of choice coding like the one presented here were already made in cortex and even in MGB. Hence, as discussed also by the authors, the strong inter-connectivity of MGB with both cortex and midbrain (where task-specific modulation of responses can already be found) make it not very surprising that MGB is involved as well. Yet the Introduction largely lacks an explanation of this high degree of recurrent connectivity and questions "whether MGB encodes learning related modulation beyond the fear system". The authors should provide a more accurate and nuanced description of what is known and what to expect.
Thanks for pointing this out. We agree and now restructured the Introduction such that learning-related changes are at the core of the text. We also made some changes in the Discussion.
Revisions expected in follow-up work:
Related to point 8 above, the novelty in the presented data is not that one can observe learning-related modulation in MGB per se, but the rather direct demonstration of how it might contribute to learning associated plasticity. To further reinforce this point, two questions should be addressed:
1) Can the same phenomenon be observed in A1 or associated cortices of the non-lemniscal pathway, and if so, where do they arise first? The authors do discuss this issue; however one remains wondering if (or to what extent) the reported findings are reflections of cortical processing, so if available, some data on this question would be very useful.
Given that the main effect is in a late time during the trial, we expect the changes are indeed of a non-lemniscal source. As for the origin of this phenomenon, we currently have no data on this issue. As we discuss, electrophysiology (preferably in multiple brain regions) of the lemniscal and non-lemniscal pathway simultaneously, could be a great follow-up work to answer this.
2) How do performance and MGB responses develop a few weeks after mice have learned the task but were not further trained, i.e. to what extent have performance and neural coding of choice crystallized and thus reflect a form of long-term plasticity associated with learning?
Unfortunately, we do not have data on mice that learned but not further trained and then imaged again. We have data on mice that continued to train on harder tasks, but these had unforeseen issues, like instability of behavioral performance at perceptual limits, and technical issues developing after months of imaging. We suspect that the strong choice signal in MGB is malleable and would change with different behavioral and cognitive demands, but we have no data to support this.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.56307.sa2Article and author information
Author details
Funding
ERC consolidator grant (#616063)
- Adi Mizrahi
Israel Science Foundation (#224/17)
- Adi Mizrahi
H2020 Marie Skłodowska-Curie Actions (659719 postdoctoral fellowship)
- Ariel Gilad
Israel Science Foundation (#2453/18)
- Adi Mizrahi
The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.
Acknowledgements
We thank Joseph Jubran for help with the programming of the behavioral system. We thank members of the Mizrahi laboratory for commenting on early versions of this manuscript. This work was supported by an ERC consolidators grant to AM (#616063), Israeli Science Foundation grants to AM (#224/17, #2453/18), and the European Union’s Horizon 2020 research and innovation programme under the Marie Skłodowska-Curie grant agreement No 659719 to AG.
Ethics
Animal experimentation: All experiments were approved by Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (IACUC) at the Hebrew University of Jerusalem, Israel (Permit Number: NS-19-15706-4).
Senior Editor
- Andrew J King, University of Oxford, United Kingdom
Reviewing Editor
- Brice Bathellier, CNRS, France
Reviewers
- Brice Bathellier, CNRS, France
- Tania Rinaldi Barkat, Basel University, Switzerland
- Michael Pecka, Ludwig-Maximilians Universitaet Muenchen, Germany
Version history
- Received: February 24, 2020
- Accepted: July 7, 2020
- Accepted Manuscript published: July 8, 2020 (version 1)
- Version of Record published: July 20, 2020 (version 2)
Copyright
© 2020, Gilad et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
-
- 3,359
- Page views
-
- 347
- Downloads
-
- 3
- Citations
Article citation count generated by polling the highest count across the following sources: Crossref, PubMed Central, Scopus.
Download links
Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)
Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)
Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)
Further reading
-
- Neuroscience
Ultrasonic vocalizations (USVs) fulfill an important role in communication and navigation in many species. Because of their social and affective significance, rodent USVs are increasingly used as a behavioral measure in neurodevelopmental and neurolinguistic research. Reliably attributing USVs to their emitter during close interactions has emerged as a difficult, key challenge. If addressed, all subsequent analyses gain substantial confidence. We present a hybrid ultrasonic tracking system, Hybrid Vocalization Localizer (HyVL), that synergistically integrates a high-resolution acoustic camera with high-quality ultrasonic microphones. HyVL is the first to achieve millimeter precision (~3.4–4.8 mm, 91% assigned) in localizing USVs, ~3× better than other systems, approaching the physical limits (mouse snout ~10 mm). We analyze mouse courtship interactions and demonstrate that males and females vocalize in starkly different relative spatial positions, and that the fraction of female vocalizations has likely been overestimated previously due to imprecise localization. Further, we find that when two male mice interact with one female, one of the males takes a dominant role in the interaction both in terms of the vocalization rate and the location relative to the female. HyVL substantially improves the precision with which social communication between rodents can be studied. It is also affordable, open-source, easy to set up, can be integrated with existing setups, and reduces the required number of experiments and animals.
-
- Neuroscience
How does the human brain combine information across the eyes? It has been known for many years that cortical normalization mechanisms implement ‘ocularity invariance’: equalizing neural responses to spatial patterns presented either monocularly or binocularly. Here, we used a novel combination of electrophysiology, psychophysics, pupillometry, and computational modeling to ask whether this invariance also holds for flickering luminance stimuli with no spatial contrast. We find dramatic violations of ocularity invariance for these stimuli, both in the cortex and also in the subcortical pathways that govern pupil diameter. Specifically, we find substantial binocular facilitation in both pathways with the effect being strongest in the cortex. Near-linear binocular additivity (instead of ocularity invariance) was also found using a perceptual luminance matching task. Ocularity invariance is, therefore, not a ubiquitous feature of visual processing, and the brain appears to repurpose a generic normalization algorithm for different visual functions by adjusting the amount of interocular suppression.