fruitless tunes functional flexibility of courtship circuitry during development

  1. Jie Chen
  2. Sihui Jin
  3. Dandan Chen
  4. Jie Cao
  5. Xiaoxiao Ji
  6. Qionglin Peng  Is a corresponding author
  7. Yufeng Pan  Is a corresponding author
  1. The Key Laboratory of Developmental Genes and Human Disease, School of Life Science and Technology, Southeast University, China
  2. Co-innovation Center of Neuroregeneration, Nantong University, China

Peer review process

This article was accepted for publication as part of eLife's original publishing model.

History

  1. Version of Record published
  2. Accepted Manuscript published
  3. Accepted
  4. Received

Decision letter

  1. Kristin Scott
    Reviewing Editor; University of California, Berkeley, United States
  2. K VijayRaghavan
    Senior Editor; National Centre for Biological Sciences, Tata Institute of Fundamental Research, India

In the interests of transparency, eLife publishes the most substantive revision requests and the accompanying author responses.

Acceptance summary:

This study provides insight into how the sexually dimorphic transcription factor (Fruitless M) acts at different developmental stages to establish sex-specific courtship behavior. The novelty of the paper is that it identifies two distinct phases of the FruM action, one during the pupal stage and the other after adult emergence; the former is pivotal for normal development of female-directed courtship whereas the latter for suppressing male-to-male courtship.

Decision letter after peer review:

Thank you for submitting your article "fruitless tunes functional flexibility of courtship circuitry during development" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by three peer reviewers, and the evaluation has been overseen by Kristin Scott as the Reviewing Editor and K VijayRaghavan as the Senior Editor. The reviewers have opted to remain anonymous.

The reviewers have discussed the reviews with one another and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this decision to help you prepare a revised submission.

As the editors have judged that your manuscript is of interest, but as described below that additional experiments are required before it is published, we would like to draw your attention to changes in our revision policy that we have made in response to COVID-19 (https://elifesciences.org/articles/57162). First, because many researchers have temporarily lost access to the labs, we will give authors as much time as they need to submit revised manuscripts. We are also offering, if you choose, to post the manuscript to bioRxiv (if it is not already there) along with this decision letter and a formal designation that the manuscript is "in revision at eLife". Please let us know if you would like to pursue this option. (If your work is more suitable for medRxiv, you will need to post the preprint yourself, as the mechanisms for us to do so are still in development.)

Summary:

This short paper by Chen et al. is a crisp and clear study that provides evidence for spatial and temporal differences in the developmental and behavioral function of male isoform of fruitless (FruM) in courtship. The authors found two distinct phases of the FruM action, one during the pupal stage and the other after adult emergence; the former is pivotal for normal development of female-directed courtship whereas the latter for suppressing male-to-male courtship. The authors refer to these two phases as innate and experience-dependent components of male courtship, respectively. Knockdown and mutations of fruM were found to affect differently the innate and experience-dependent components, reflecting the different levels of residual fruM functions. Although the role of FruM function is heavily studied in the context of male behaviors, the novelty of the paper is that it unravels spatial, temporal and quantitative segregation of FruM function in development of circuits driving courtship behaviors and its function in regulating innate reproductive behaviors. However, validation of the efficacy of fruM manipulations is required in order to interpret the studies.

Essential revisions:

1) All of the conclusions critically depend on the assumption that RNAi-based knockdown of fruM is effective. It is important to verify that RNAi does knock down fruM expression in a developmental stage- or tissue-specific manner. Moreover, It is important to show that fruMi selectively inactivates fruM without affecting non-sex specific transcripts by performing qPCR with primers specific for fruM and Fru common forms. Immunohistochemistry (IHC), RNA in situ hybridization, or showing how knocking down fru at stage 5 changes the circuitry (i.e. numbers of fru positive cells or their wiring patterns) are approaches that may supplement as validation. Please provide details of qPCR or IHC.

2) In the experiment of stage-specific knockdown, no behavioral effect was observed when GAL80 was inactivated before stage-4, letting the authors conclude that "the critical period" commences after stage-5. However, it is not certain whether fru-GAL4 was transcriptionally active at stage-4 or earlier stages. Images of tissues stained for fru-GAL4 activity along the developmental stage should be added in a supplementary figure.

3) As a fruM null genotype, only fru[LexA]/fru[4-40] was used. More allelic combinations, those without fru[LexA] in particular, are to be tested, because fru[LexA] behaves differently from other alleles when male chaining is measured.

[Editors' note: further revisions were suggested prior to acceptance, as described below.]

Thank you for re-submitting your work entitled "fruitless tunes functional flexibility of courtship circuitry during development" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by one peer reviewer who reviewed the original manuscript, and the evaluation has been overseen by a Reviewing Editor and a Senior Editor. The reviewer has opted to remain anonymous.

We regret to inform you that your work will not be considered further for publication in eLife. Specifically, the lack of validation of the efficacy of RNAi at different developmental stages was a major weakness noted in the original reviews and has not been addressed in the revision.

Major Comments:

The authors did not incorporate our request to verify the effect of RNAi "in a developmental stage- or tissue-specific manner" (that is, under conditions they employed in Figure 1). To reply to their arguments:

1) The new Figure 1—figure supplement 1B uses the constitutive expression of miRNA against fruM, just like data in Figure 2F. These are insufficient to address our concern that the authors' conclusion critically depends on unproven efficacy of stage-specific RNAi using GAL80ts. My request was simple: perform qRNA or immunohistochemistry against fruM using exactly the same animals authors used in Figure 1.

2) One image showing neural development defect (Figure 1—figure supplement 1M) is inappropriate to prove the efficacy of RNAi treatment, for three reasons. First is an obvious lack of biological replicates. Second is that we still do not know whether the effect is generalizable across the entire nervous system. Lastly, if they wish to insist that the developmental defects caused by RNAi was the reason why their courtship performance plummeted at stage 5, they must perform similar comparison across stages. That is what we initially meant by verifications in "developmental stage-specific manner". What happens when knockdown is induced at stage 4 or stage 6? Does the anatomical phenotype correlate with behavior? This information must be critical for supporting the model authors shown in Figure 3.

3) GFP expression patterns (Figure 1—figure supplement 1D-K) does not replace the need to directly address the effects of RNAi. They should have performed direct evaluation of fru expression when RNAi was induced at stage 4, since their conclusion depends on the assumption that RNAi knocked fruM down then.

Therefore, I am afraid authors have not presented sufficient data to support their conclusion regarding the developmental stage-specific effects of fru. I am particularly disappointed because 1) the experiment was straightforward and within the expertise of the authors, and 2) stage-specific function of fruM takes up such a significant portion of Discussion.

I acknowledge that authors improved their manuscript by adding discussion and method details. Knowing the difficult situation that all scientists around the world are currently experiencing, I am very reluctant to be unnecessarily critical. However, the caveats on the verification of stage-specific RNAi are left largely unaddressed and are a major confound with the study.

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.59224.sa1

Author response

Essential revisions:

1) All of the conclusions critically depend on the assumption that RNAi-based knockdown of fruM is effective. It is important to verify that RNAi does knock down fruM expression in a developmental stage- or tissue-specific manner. Moreover, It is important to show that fruMi selectively inactivates fruM without affecting non-sex specific transcripts by performing qPCR with primers specific for fruM and Fru common forms. Immunohistochemistry (IHC), RNA in situ hybridization, or showing how knocking down fru at stage 5 changes the circuitry (i.e. numbers of fru positive cells or their wiring patterns) are approaches that may supplement as validation. Please provide details of qPCR or IHC.

We used UAS-fruMi (attp40) and UAS-fruMi (attp2) targeting the male-specific fruM mRNA, and a scrambled version UAS-fruMiScr (attp2) as a control. These stocks were generated previously in the Baker lab in Janelia and proved to be effective (Meissner et al., 2016; Chen et al., 2017). We now provided additional experiments validating the efficiency of these stocks. First, the fruM mRNA level was significantly lower using either UAS-fruMi (attp40) (Figure 2F) or UAS-fruMi (attp2) (Figure 1—figure supplement 1B) driven by fruGAL4. Furthermore, mRNA level of FruCom from the P4 promotor is not affected using UAS-fruMi driven by fruGAL4 (Figure 1—figure supplement 1C). Knocking down fruM in all fruGAL4 neurons may affect neuronal development of many types of fruM neurons and we focused on the gustatory receptor neurons innervating the VNC for their easily identified phenotype: loss of midline crossing (Figure 1—figure supplement 1M, white arrow). We found that knocking down fruM for 2 days at stage 5 is sufficient to result in loss of midline crossing of the gustatory receptor neurons in adult (Figure 1—figure supplement 1N, white arrow), further supporting the efficiency of fruM knock-down by UAS-fruMi.

2) In the experiment of stage-specific knockdown, no behavioral effect was observed when GAL80 was inactivated before stage-4, letting the authors conclude that "the critical period" commences after stage-5. However, it is not certain whether fru-GAL4 was transcriptionally active at stage-4 or earlier stages. Images of tissues stained for fru-GAL4 activity along the developmental stage should be added in a supplementary figure.

We provided fruGAL4 expression images in all developmental stages from embryo to adult and found specific expression at all stages (Figure 1—figure supplement 1D-K).

3) As a fruM null genotype, only fru[LexA]/fru[4-40] was used. More allelic combinations, those without fru[LexA] in particular, are to be tested, because fru[LexA] behaves differently from other alleles when male chaining is measured.

We did not provided additional fruM null mutant behaviors for the initial submission as there are already published data (Pan et al., 2014). We now re-tested two other fruM null combinational mutants (fru4-40/fruAJ96u3 and fru4-40/fruSat15) for their male-male and male-female courtship, as well as male chaining behavior (Figure 2—figure supplement 3A-F), which further supports our findings that fruM null males did not court either male or female flies initially, and developed intensive male chaining behavior after grouping for 1-3 days.

[Editors' note: further revisions were suggested prior to acceptance, as described below.]

Major Comments:

The authors did not incorporate our request to verify the effect of RNAi "in a developmental stage- or tissue-specific manner" (that is, under conditions they employed in Figure 1). To reply to their arguments:

1) The new Figure 1—figure supplement 1B uses the constitutive expression of miRNA against fruM, just like data in Figure 2F. These are insufficient to address our concern that the authors' conclusion critically depends on unproven efficacy of stage-specific RNAi using GAL80ts. My request was simple: perform qRNA or immunohistochemistry against fruM using exactly the same animals authors used in Figure 1.

We only recently generated a fruV5 knock-in line and anti-FruM antibody successfully, and these tools could allow us to visualize FruM expression to answer the above question. These tools were validated by immunostaining against V5 and FruM in wild-type males and females, as well as in fruM mutants (see Figure 1—figure supplement 2).

We then used the fruV5 line and FruM antibody to check if 2-day heat shock at 30°C during development is sufficient to knock down fruM expression. We dissected brains immediately after 2-day heat shock at stage 5 or 7 and found anti-V5 and anti-FruM signals were dramatically decreased, such that only a small fraction of neurons could be weakly labelled. In comparison, control males with the same age but raised at 18˚C have regular anti-V5 and anti-FruM signals (see Figure 1). We actually performed such experiment from stage 4 to 9, all of which showed similarly strong knock-down of fruM expression, and we put the result of stage 5 and 7 into Figure 1 to show the efficiency of fruM microRNAi. We also checked anti-V5 and anti-FruM signals in males that have been heat shocked for 4 days during adulthood and found similar knock-down effect (now part of Figure 2). These results clearly demonstrate an effective knock-down of fruM expression upon 2-day heat shock at 30˚C.

As induction of fruM microRNA for 2 days effectively knocked down fruM expression during that period, but fruM microRNA may not be degraded immediately and has longer effect, we further tested to how much extent such knock-down effect may last. Thus, we dissected brains of adult males that have been heat shocked for 2 days at different developmental stages (from stage 1 to 9), and found that males that have been heat shocked at earlier stages (such as stage 1-5) still have strong FruM expression (check the anti-V5 signals, the anti-FruM signals are relatively weaker), while those heat shocked at later stages (stages 6-9) still have reduced FruM expression in adult males (see Figure 1—figure supplement 3).

Thus, these results showed that induction of fruM microRNA for 2 days effectively knocked down fruM expression, and FruM expression did not fully restore to regular levels if heat shocked at later stages. However, such a caveat (acknowledged in the discussion part) does not compromise our conclusion at all, as even if induction of fruM microRNA at stage 5 may have minor effect on later stages, its role on behavior is still due to stage 5, as induction of fruM microRNA at stages 6-9 did not affect male courtship.

As we now have anti-V5 to visualize FruM, we also checked efficiency of spatial fruM knock-down in flies using Otd-flp. We found that consistent with fruGAL4 expression (labeled by GFP), FruM was efficiently knocked down mainly in brain or VNC (see Figure 3—figure supplement 3).

For stage-dependent developmental deficit of gustatory receptor neurons, see below.

2) One image showing neural development defect (Figure 1—figure supplement 1M) is inappropriate to prove the efficacy of RNAi treatment, for three reasons. First is an obvious lack of biological replicates. Second is that we still do not know whether the effect is generalizable across the entire nervous system. Lastly, if they wish to insist that the developmental defects caused by RNAi was the reason why their courtship performance plummeted at stage 5, they must perform similar comparison across stages. That is what we initially meant by verifications in "developmental stage-specific manner". What happens when knockdown is induced at stage 4 or stage 6? Does the anatomical phenotype correlate with behavior? This information must be critical for supporting the model authors shown in Figure 3.

In order to address this concern, we tried to analyze the midline crossing defects of the gustatory receptor neurons (GRNs) in males that have been heat shocked for 2 days in each developmental stage, although we still could not analyze other neuronal types due to technical limits (hard to identify developmental defect of other neuronal types using the genetic setup in this study) as well as the limited scope of this study.

We first checked basal expression of GFP in tub-GAL80ts/UAS-mCD8GFP; fruGAL4/UAS-fruMi males raised constantly at 18°C, and observed a small fraction of neurons expressing GFP (see Figure 1—figure supplement 4), which indicates a strong but not 100% suppression of GAL4 activity by GAL80ts at 18˚C. Note that the GRNs we tried to analyze were not GFP-positive, indicating a successful suppression of GAL4 activity in these neurons at 18˚C.

We then dissected VNC of tub-GAL80ts/UAS-mCD8GFP; fruGAL4/UAS-fruMi males that have been heat shocked for 2 days in each developmental stage, with 5 samples for each stage. We found that these GRNs were only labeled in males that have been heat shocked after stage 4 (stages 5-9, yellow arrows), probably because these GRNs were developed during pupation, which is consistent with a previous study (Mellert et al., 2012). Thus, we can only compare the effect of fruM knock down after stage 4 on the development of these GRNs.

Interestingly, we found that all males heat shocked at stage 5 showed defect of midline crossing in these GRNs, 60% of males heat shocked at stage 6 showed defect of midline crossing, while all males heat shocked after stage 6 showed regular midline crossing (white arrow). Males heat shocked for 4 days during adulthood also have regular midline crossing. These results clearly showed a critical developmental period (stages 5-6) where FruM function to ensure regular development of GRNs.

3) GFP expression patterns (Figure 1—figure supplement 1D-K) does not replace the need to directly address the effects of RNAi. They should have performed direct evaluation of fru expression when RNAi was induced at stage 4, since their conclusion depends on the assumption that RNAi knocked fruM down then.

We now performed RNAi in every stage and used anti-V5, anti-FruM and anti-GFP to address these concerns, as above described.

Therefore, I am afraid authors have not presented sufficient data to support their conclusion regarding the developmental stage-specific effects of fru. I am particularly disappointed because 1) the experiment was straightforward and within the expertise of the authors, and 2) stage-specific function of fruM takes up such a significant portion of Discussion.

I acknowledge that authors improved their manuscript by adding discussion and method details. Knowing the difficult situation that all scientists around the world are currently experiencing, I am very reluctant to be unnecessarily critical. However, the caveats on the verification of stage-specific RNAi are left largely unaddressed and are a major confound with the study.

We thank the reviewer for these comments, and we now performed extensive analysis of FruM expression using anti-V5 and anti-FruM antibodies, as well as developmental defect of GRNs, in males that have induced expression of fruM microRNAi in almost every developmental stage. We believe these results have addressed reviewer’s concerns and significantly enhanced this manuscript.

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.59224.sa2

Download links

A two-part list of links to download the article, or parts of the article, in various formats.

Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)

Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)

Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)

  1. Jie Chen
  2. Sihui Jin
  3. Dandan Chen
  4. Jie Cao
  5. Xiaoxiao Ji
  6. Qionglin Peng
  7. Yufeng Pan
(2021)
fruitless tunes functional flexibility of courtship circuitry during development
eLife 10:e59224.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.59224

Share this article

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.59224