Improving emotional-action control by targeting long-range phase-amplitude neuronal coupling
Abstract
Control over emotional action tendencies is essential for everyday interactions. This cognitive function fails occasionally during socially challenging situations, and systematically in social psychopathologies. We delivered dual-site phase-coupled brain stimulation to facilitate theta-gamma phase-amplitude coupling between frontal regions known to implement that form of control, while neuropsychologically healthy human male participants were challenged to control their automatic action tendencies in a social–emotional approach/avoidance-task. Participants had increased control over their emotional action tendencies, depending on the relative phase and dose of the intervention. Concurrently measured fMRI effects of task and stimulation indicated that the intervention improved control by increasing the efficacy of anterior prefrontal inhibition over the sensorimotor cortex. This enhancement of emotional action control provides causal evidence for phase-amplitude coupling mechanisms guiding action selection during emotional-action control. Generally, the finding illustrates the potential of physiologically-grounded interventions aimed at reducing neural noise in cerebral circuits where communication relies on phase-amplitude coupling.
Introduction
The ability to control emotional actions is paramount for successful engagement in human social interactions (Hare, 2017). Long-standing theorizing and accumulating empirical evidence indicate that affective cues automatically activate approach-avoidance action tendencies (Ridderinkhof, 2017; Phaf et al., 2014). Effective emotion control requires the cognitive capacity to suppress those automatic action tendencies and to select an alternative course of action (Etkin et al., 2006; Koch et al., 2018). The importance of emotional-action control becomes apparent when it is disrupted: In social psychopathologies such as social anxiety, the inability to override social avoidance tendencies constitutes the core maintaining factor of the disorder (Craske and Stein, 2016). There is great interest in potentiating this cognitive capacity to enhance treatment efficacy, as well as to help professionals exposed to socially challenging situations. However, improving human emotional-action control has proven difficult (den Uyl et al., 2015). In this study, we use a brain stimulation intervention designed to enhance synchrony within a cerebral circuit known to support emotional-action control (Bramson et al., 2018; Volman et al., 2011a). By modeling how effective and structural connectivity of that cerebral circuit mediates the behavioral effects of the intervention, we provide an account of its neural effects, paving the way for physiologically-grounded therapeutic interventions in social-emotional disorders (Voytek and Knight, 2015).
Previous non-invasive brain stimulation interventions have been successful in reducing emotional-action control. This was achieved by disrupting neural activity – putatively by injecting neural noise (Schwarzkopf et al., 2011) – in a region known to coordinate emotional-action control: the anterior prefrontal cortex (aPFC) (Volman et al., 2011a). Here we explore whether it is possible to enhance emotional-action control by using brain stimulation aimed at reducing neural noise, targeting a cortical circuit known to regulate those action-tendencies (den Uyl et al., 2015; Bramson et al., 2018; Volman et al., 2011a). This gain-of-function intervention is grounded on recent insights showing that emotional-action control requires neural synchronization between aPFC theta-band rhythm and sensorimotor broadband gamma activity (Bramson et al., 2018; Voytek et al., 2015; Canolty and Knight, 2010). We reasoned that endogenous neural synchronization might be enhanced by applying separate time-varying electric fields (transcranial alternating current stimulation; tACS [Fröhlich and McCormick, 2010]) to aPFC and sensorimotor cortex (SMC). tACS influences spike timing of individual neurons (Fröhlich and McCormick, 2010; Krause et al., 2019; Johnson et al., 2020), and entrains neural rhythms to its frequency and phase (Reinhart and Nguyen, 2019; Polanía et al., 2015). We applied dual-site phase-coupled tACS to enhance the endogenous synchronization of SMC gamma-band power (75 Hz) with the peaks of aPFC theta-band rhythm (6 Hz) evoked during emotional-action control (Bramson et al., 2018; Nowak et al., 2017; Akkad et al., 2020). Enhanced phase-amplitude coupling would reduce neural noise in aPFC-SMC communication (Voytek and Knight, 2015; Canolty and Knight, 2010), allowing for improved control over emotional-action tendencies. Crucially, we apply this aPFC-SMC stimulation while 41 human male participants with no history of mental illness perform an emotional-action control task (Figure 1A). We compare the online behavioral and neural effects of in-phase, anti-phase, and sham couplings between the power envelope of SMC gamma stimulation and the peaks of aPFC theta stimulation (Figure 2A,B). Importantly, concurrent whole-brain BOLD-fMRI quantified local and remote dose-dependent cerebral effects of the electrical stimulations (Evans et al., 2020). tACS effects on each participants’ cerebral connectivity were further qualified using dynamic causal modeling (Friston et al., 2003), informed by MR-tractography (Behrens et al., 2007).

Behavioral and cerebral effects of the approach-avoidance task used to manipulate control over emotional action tendencies.
(A) Conceptual visualization of the approach-avoidance task. Participants pushed- or pulled a joystick away- or toward themselves to approach- or avoid happy and angry faces. Approaching angry- and avoiding happy faces is incongruent with action tendencies to approach appetitive and avoid aversive situations. (B) Behavioral results in the sham condition of the task. Participants make more errors in the incongruent trials (circles) than in the congruent trials (triangles). Black lines visualize the mean and standard error of the mean. Gray bar depicts the group average congruency effect. (C) Approach-avoidance congruency-related BOLD changes (p<0.01 cluster-level inferences corrected for multiple comparisons). Trials involving responses incongruent with automatic action tendencies showed stronger BOLD signal in anterior prefrontal- and parietal areas, and reduced signal in the left amygdala/hippocampus and medial prefrontal cortex.

Behavioral effects of dual-site phase-coupled tACS on emotional-action control are dose- and phase-dependent.
(A)Two sets of ring electrodes were placed over the right aPFC and left SMC. Modeling of the current density showed that stimulation reached both regions of interest with intensities known to support phase entrainment when matched to the endogenous rhythms (Reato et al., 2010). (B) During the experiment, stimulation conditions were alternated between in-phase, anti-phase, and sham conditions in pseudo-random fashion. The 75 Hz stimulation over SMC was amplitude-modulated according to the 6 Hz stimulation over aPFC, either in-phase or anti-phase with the peaks of the 6 Hz aPFC stimulation. Sham consisted of an initial stimulation of 10 s, after which stimulation was terminated. (C) Concurrent tACS-fMRI quantified changes in BOLD signal evoked by the tACS intervention, across in-phase and anti-phase conditions, and independently from task performance. Online physiological effects of tACS are evident both under the aPFC electrode (black circle) and in other cortical regions. (D) Participants with stronger inhibitory responses to theta-band stimulation over aPFC (manifested as decreased BOLD signal; Scheeringa et al., 2011) improved their control over emotional actions (decreased congruency effect) during aPFC-SMC in-phase tACS (in green) but not during aPFC-SMC anti-phase tACS (in red), reflected in the interaction between emotional control (congruent, incongruent), stimulations phase (in-phase, anti-phase) and tACS-dose (BOLD signal in aPFC during stimulation versus sham [panel C]); F(1,39) = 9.3, p=0.004, partial eta2 = 0.19. Inset bar graphs illustrate group average parameter estimates of the congruency effects corrected for tACS-dose. The direction of the effect in the anti-phase condition tentatively suggests that stronger entrainment to the theta-band stimulation (decreased BOLD) increases congruency effects, as would be expected when aPFC-SMC communication is disrupted. (E) Without controlling for inter-participant variability in tACS-dose, the differential phase effect on emotional-actions control is less statistically reliable (p=0.06, partial eta2 = 0.088).
Results
We manipulated emotion-action control through a social-emotional approach-avoidance task, where human male participants use a joystick to rapidly approach or avoid happy or angry faces (Figure 1A). People tend to approach happy faces and avoid angry faces (Phaf et al., 2014). Overriding these affect-congruent action-tendencies and instead generating affect-incongruent actions (approach-angry and avoid-happy) requires control, which is implemented through the aPFC, parietal/SMC, and amygdala/hippocampal regions (Bramson et al., 2018; Volman et al., 2011a). Accordingly, in the sham condition of this study, participants’ error rates and aPFC BOLD activity increased when incongruent approach-avoidance responses are compared to affect-congruent responses, reproducing previous effect sizes (Bramson et al., 2018; Volman et al., 2011a) – Figure 1B,C.
We quantified the magnitude of the physiological effects of tACS on the underlying neural tissue through concurrent tACS-fMRI. We used BOLD effects of theta-band tACS stimulation over aPFC (in-phase + anti-phase stimulation epochs versus sham; Figure 2D), as a dose-dependent metric of tACS effects (tACS-dose; Evans et al., 2020), independent of task performance. In line with our expectations, higher tACS-dose on aPFC increased the emotion-control enhancement induced by in-phase stimulation (in-phase aPFC-theta/SMC-gamma tACS) [emotional control*phase condition*tACS-dose interaction: Figure 2E]. Interestingly, across participants, the local cortical (BOLD) effects of the inhibitory theta-band aPFC stimulation (Scheeringa et al., 2011) correlated positively with improved emotion-control (indexed by decreases in behavioral congruency-effects during the in-phase condition but not in the anti-phase condition –Figure 2D). These dose- and phase-dependent results, predicted on the knowledge that emotional-action control requires synchronization between specific endogenous rhythms (Bramson et al., 2018; Helfrich and Knight, 2016), indicate that in-phase aPFC-theta/SMC-gamma tACS increases participants’ control over their emotional action-tendencies.
The observed cognitive benefits of the tACS intervention could arise from direct modulation of aPFC-SMC connectivity, or be mediated by other regions (Figure 3A). We arbitrated between those possibilities using two complementary approaches. First, we tested whether the amygdala mediates these effects. This region is a prime candidate as it is connected to both aPFC and SMC, and is strongly involved in emotional-action control (Volman et al., 2011a; Etkin et al., 2015). We distinguished between a number of anatomically plausible circuit-level effects of the tACS intervention using dynamic causal modeling (Friston et al., 2003) on regional BOLD timeseries in the amygdala, aPFC and SMC, measured during performance of the approach-avoidance task. Model comparison supports a circuit where tACS modulates aPFC→SMC, aPFC→amygdala, and amygdala→SMC connections (Figure 3—figure supplement 1). Crucially, the effect of stimulation on emotional-action control was driven by tACS modulation of a specific component of that circuit, the task-related aPFC→SMC connectivity (emotional-control*phase-condition*aPFC→SMC effective connectivity; Figure 3). Figure 3 visualizes this behavioral- and connectivity-related difference in tACS effects, contrasting the behavioral effects evoked in participants with strong or weak in-phase tACS modulation of aPFC→SMC connectivity. Emotional control was enhanced in those participants with strong in-phase tACS modulation of aPFC-SMC connectivity: the congruency effect observed in the anti-phase and sham conditions disappeared during the in-phase condition. By contrast, emotional control remained unaffected in those participants without in-phase tACS modulation of aPFC→SMC connectivity: there were similar congruency effects across in-phase, anti-phase, and sham conditions. Second, we assessed whether inter-participant variance in the circuit-level effect of the tACS intervention can be understood in terms of inter-participant variance in structural connectivity between aPFC and SMC. In support of this notion, participants with higher fractional anisotropy (FA; a measure of white matter integrity) in the white matter beneath BA6 had stronger inhibitory coupling between aPFC and SMC during in-phase tACS (as compared to anti-phase tACS) (supplementary results).

Modulatory effects of dual-site phase-coupled tACS on emotional-action control depend on effective connectivity between aPFC and SMC.
(A) Model selection compared models with and without a direct connection between aPFC→SMC (dashed arrow), and tACS modulations on different connections (gray dashed oval arrows). (B) Model selection indicates that tACS affects multiple connections in the network (top panels; supplementary materials), but only tACS-related changes in connectivity between aPFC→SMC predict behavioral effects of the stimulation (interaction between emotional control [congruent, incongruent], stimulations phase [in-phase, anti-phase] and p=0.019, np2 = 0.13; lower panels). Those participants with stronger inhibitory influence of aPFC over SMC in the in-phase condition (lower left panel, n = 22) showed decreases in congruency effects in the in-phase condition but not in the anti-phase and sham condition. The rest of the participants did not show a differential effect between stimulation conditions. Asterisks: p<0.01.
Discussion
Using a combination of concurrent tACS-fMRI, cognitively precise behavioral outcomes, as well as effective and structural connectivity, we provide converging evidence for a causal role of aPFC-SMC connections in guiding action selection during emotional-action control (Koch et al., 2018). Emotional-action control improves when dual-site phase-coupled tACS is tuned to increase the efficacy of anterior prefrontal inhibition over sensorimotor cortex. Alternative interpretations of the findings, focused on transcutaneous entrainment (Asamoah et al., 2019) or retinal stimulation (Schutter, 2016), are ruled out by the experimental design. Namely, the in-phase and anti-phase conditions used identical stimulation parameters apart from their phase difference, thereby creating the same peripheral effects.
We did not observe the anticipated anti-phase tACS induced increase of congruency effects caused by a desynchronization of aPFC and SMC (Polanía et al., 2015). However, there was increased activity in contralateral aPFC and PPC for anti-phase trials as compared to in-phase trials. Speculatively, this increased BOLD signal could reflect the engagement of the control network contralateral to the stimulated circuit, compensating disruptive effects of anti-phase stimulation (Sack et al., 2005). Future studies could focus on characterizing these potential compensatory effects. This study grounds a human gain-of-function intervention on the known hierarchical organization of the frontal cortex (Voytek et al., 2015) and on phase-amplitude coupling as a mechanism for directional inter-regional neuronal communication (Voytek et al., 2015; Canolty and Knight, 2010). These findings provide the first stepping stone toward the development of interventions aimed to help patients suffering from social-emotional disorders. For instance, individuals with social anxiety are often unable to overcome avoidance tendencies, hampering interventions aimed to extinguish fear through exposure (Craske and Stein, 2016). Synchronizing aPFC-SMC theta-gamma coupling might temporally alleviate this lack of control (Voytek and Knight, 2015), allowing patients to benefit from exposure treatment. However, thus far it is unclear whether the findings reported here translate beyond healthy and highly educated male samples to more heterogeneous (healthy and patient) populations. Although the cognitive challenge provided by the AA task is rather mild, as is evidenced by the relatively low error rate (max. 20%), congruency effects elicited by the AA task do predict real-life emotional action control (Kaldewaij et al., 2019). Future studies could explore whether effects of in-phase stimulation hold for more challenging emotional control situations (e.g. Sack et al., 2005), extend to more heterogeneous populations, and can generate long-lasting effects through exposure treatment. Generally, the findings of this study pave the way for implementing physiologically-grounded non-invasive interventions aimed at reducing neural noise in cerebral circuits where communication relies on long-range phase-amplitude coupling.
Materials and methods
Participants
Forty-four male students of the Radboud University Nijmegen participated in this experiment after giving informed consent for participation and publication of anonymized data. Two participants were excluded because they did not attend the whole experiment; one participant was excluded because he failed to comply with the task instructions, yielding a total pre-determined sample of 41 participants. This sample size was determined on a priori estimates of statistical power calculated in Gpower 3.1 (Faul et al., 2007) according to an expected effect size of Cohen’s d = 0.4, as reported in- or calculated from Volman et al., 2011a; Alekseichuk et al., 2016. The a-priori sample size calculation, and other experimental details were preregistered before onset of data analysis, and after data collection, at the Open Science Framework, (https://osf.io/m9bv7/?view_only=18ccdf99a8e84afaaebee393debbabe3). The study was approved by the local ethics committee (CMO2014/288). Only male participants were recruited to avoid having to control for potential variability that could be introduced by sex-related differences in hormone levels known to be associated with this task (Volman et al., 2011b) (for a full discussion see Bramson et al., 2020). Participants reported no history of mental illness or use of psychoactive medication. All participants had normal- or corrected to normal vision and were screened for contra-indications for magnetic resonance imaging and transcranial alternating current stimulation. The mean age was 23.8 years, SD = 3.4, range 18–34.
Procedure
Request a detailed protocolData were acquired on 3 different days. On the first day, we acquired a structural T1 scan, a diffusion weighted imaging (DWI) scan, and a magnetic spectroscopy scan (MRS; not reported here). During the 2nd and 3rd day, electrodes were placed on the scalp, covering SMC and aPFC (see below for details on the precise localization). Afterward, participants performed a 5 min practice task, before starting an approach-avoidance task (35 min on each day) while receiving tACS stimulation and concurrently being scanned with fMRI.
Approach-avoidance (AA) task
Request a detailed protocolParticipants performed a social-emotional approach-avoidance task that has previously been shown to require control over prepotent habitual action-tendencies to approach happy- and avoid angry faces (Phaf et al., 2014; Roelofs et al., 2009). Overriding these action-tendencies requires a complex form of cognitive control that operates on the interaction between emotional percepts and the emotional valence of the required actions (Volman et al., 2011a; Roelofs et al., 2009; Volman et al., 2013), and depends on aPFC control over downstream regions (Koch et al., 2018; Volman et al., 2011a; Bramson et al., 2020), implemented by theta-gamma phase-amplitude coupling (Bramson et al., 2018). Participants responded through a joystick with one degree of freedom (along the participant’s midsagittal plane), holding the joystick with their right hand on top of their abdomen, while laying the MR-scanner and seeing a visual projection screen through a mirror system (see below). Participants were instructed to pull the joystick toward themselves when they saw a happy face, and push it away from themselves when they saw an angry face. These were the instructions in the congruent condition. In the incongruent condition, the participants were asked to push the joystick away from themselves when they saw a happy face, and pull it toward themselves when they saw an angry face (Figure 1). Written instructions were presented on the screen for a minimum of 10 s prior to the start of each block of 12 trials. Combined with the inter-block interval of around 20 s this gave at least 30 s washout of the stimulation. Congruent and incongruent conditions alternated between blocks. Trials started with a fixation cross presented in the centre of the screen for 500 ms, followed by the presentation of a face for 100 ms. Participants were asked to respond as fast as possible, with a maximum response time of 2000 ms. Movements exceeding 30% of the potential movement range of the joystick were taken as valid responses. Online feedback (‘you did not move the joystick far enough’) was provided on screen if response time exceeded 2000 ms. Each participant performed 288 trials on each of the 2 testing days, yielding 576 trials in total, equally divided over in-phase, anti-phase, and sham stimulation – as well as over congruent and incongruent conditions.
tACS stimulation parameters
Request a detailed protocolTranscranial alternating current stimulation (2 mA peak-to-peak) was applied using two sets of center-ring electrodes (80 mm inner Ø; 100 mm outer Ø; centre electrode had a Ø of 10 mm) (Saturnino et al., 2017). Stimulation was applied on-line during task performance in blocks of approximately 60 s (i.e. the length of a stimulus block; 12 trials), and consisted of electrical fields that changed polarity between the inner- and outer rings in theta-band (6 Hz) frequency over the aPFC and gamma-band (75 Hz), tapered with a 6 Hz sine wave over SMC. Gamma-band power was phase locked to peaks (in-phase) or troughs (anti-phase) of the theta-band signal, Figure 2B. Sham consisted of an initial stimulation of 10 s to mimic potential sensations related to the onset of stimulation, after which stimulation was terminated. Stimulation conditions were alternated between blocks in pseudo-random fashion with the constraint that stimulation condition could never be repeated for two consecutive blocks. The position of the electrodes on the skull was determined for each participant by using individual structural T1 scans to which we registered masks of the regions of interest (MNI [−28–32 64] for SMC [Bramson et al., 2018]; and MNI [26 54 0] for lateral frontal pole (FPl) [Bramson et al., 2020; Neubert et al., 2014]). Precise placement of the centre electrode was achieved using Localite TMS Navigator (https://www.localite.de/en/products/tms-navigator/; RRID:SCR_016126). Electrodes were attached using Ten20 paste (MedCat; https://medcat.ccvshop.nl/Ten20-Pasta-Topf-4-Oz,−3er-Set) and impedance was kept below 10 kOhm (mean = 3.6, SD = 2.7). Stimulation was applied using two Neuroconn DC-PLUS stimulators (https://www.neurocaregroup.com/dc_stimulator_plus.html; RRID:SCR_015520) that were placed inside a magnetically shielded box in the MR room. This box contained home-made electronics and BrainAmp ExG MR amplifiers (www.brainproducts.com) to continuously monitor the tACS output of the stimulator and filter out the RF pulses of the MR system.
tACS conditions (in-phase; anti-phase; sham) were applied within the same session with stimulation condition alternating between stimulus blocks (each lasting approximately 60 s; 12 trials), interleaved with periods of no stimulation (instructions between blocks; at least 30 s). Whereas previous studies have shown long lasting aftereffects of tACS, ranging up to 70 min (Kasten et al., 2016), we considered it unlikely that these would systematically bias the current results based on two arguments. First, reports showing strong aftereffects often implemented relatively long periods of continuous stimulation (Alekseichuk et al., 2016; Kasten et al., 2016; Ruhnau et al., 2016) necessary for the quantification of neural/behavioral stimulation effects. Short stimulation protocols comparable to the current study do not report aftereffects (Johnson et al., 2020; Strüber et al., 2015). tACS aftereffects would, by definition, influence neural activity in the sham epochs systematically succeeding each stimulation epoch. Yet, we observe strong differences in BOLD signal when comparing stimulation and sham epochs under the prefrontal electrodes (Figure 1C). Second, tACS aftereffects are thought to depend on synaptic plasticity rather than entrainment (Vossen et al., 2015). Possible offline plasticity aftereffects would build up across several minutes, that is, across stimulation periods with different phase relations, thus orthogonally to our experimental phase-based manipulation (Vossen et al., 2015). Aftereffects of entrainment are unlikely because entrainment is thought to fade out after several cycles (Johnson et al., 2020; Strüber et al., 2015; Vossen et al., 2015; Halbleib et al., 2012). Post-hoc analysis indeed revealed that behavioral congruency effects in the sham condition do not differ for trials preceded by in-phase as compared to anti-phase stimulation; t(40) = 0.4, p=0.7, arguing against systematic offline influence of tACS on behavioral congruency.
Materials and apparatus
Request a detailed protocolMagnetic resonance images were acquired using a 3T MAGNETROM Prisma MR scanner (Siemens AG, Healthcare Sector, Erlangen, Germany) using a 64-channel head coil with a hole in the top through which the electrode wires were taken out of the scanner bore.
The field of view of the functional scans acquired in the MR-sessions was aligned to a built-in brain-atlas to ensure a consistent field of view across days. Approximately 1800 functional images were continuously acquired in each scanning day using a multi-band six sequence, 2× 2× 2 mm voxel size, TR/TE = 1000/34 ms, Flip angle = 60°, phase angle p>>A, including 10 volumes with reversed phase encoding (A >> P) to correct image distortions.
High-resolution anatomical images were acquired with a single-shot MPRAGE sequence with an acceleration factor of 2 (GRAPPA method), a TR of 2400 ms, TE 2.13 ms. Effective voxel size was 1 × 1×1 mm with 176 sagittal slices, distance factor 50%, flip angle 8°, orientation A ≫ P, FoV 256 mm.
Diffusion-weighted images were acquired using echo-planar imaging with multiband acceleration factor of 3. We acquired 93 1.6 mm thick transversal slices with voxel size of 1.6 × 1.6 × 1.6 mm, phase encoding direction A >> P, FoV 211 mm, TR = 3350, TE = 71.20. 256 isotropically distributed directions were acquired using a b-value of 2500 s/mm2. We also acquired a volume without diffusion weighting with reverse-phase encoding (p>>A).
Behavioral analyses
Request a detailed protocolWe compared differences in error rates between congruent and incongruent trials for the different stimulation conditions whilst controlling for dose dependent effects of tACS (‘tACS-dose’) (Evans et al., 2020; Kasten et al., 2019). tACS-dose was estimated by taking BOLD contrast between stimulation and sham conditions extracted from right aPFC (Evans et al., 2020). This three way interaction (emotional-control*phase-condition*tACS-dose) was assessed using congruency effects estimated from participant-by-participant averages (with RM-ANOVA), as well as trial-by-trial data (with Bayesian mixed effect models). RM-ANOVA was used to facilitate comparisons with earlier studies reporting on this task (e.g. Bramson et al., 2018; Volman et al., 2011a; Volman et al., 2013) and implemented in JASP (https://jasp-stats.org/; RRID:SCR_015823). We used Bayesian mixed effect models (implemented in R 3.5.3 using the brms package [Bürkner, 2017]) because they are robust to potential violations of normality or homoscedasticity. The Bayesian mixed models included random intercepts for all subjects and random slopes for all fixed effects (congruency- and stimulation condition) and their interaction per participant. This model adheres to the maximal random effects structure (Barr, 2013). Outputs of these models are log odds with credible intervals (‘B’). In these analyses, an effect is seen as statistically significant if the credible interval does not contain zero with 95% certainty.
We hypothesized that the congruency effect in error rates would decrease for in-phase condition and increase for anti-phase stimulation and that the size of the effect per participant would depend on the BOLD effect of tACS versus sham, a measure of dose dependence that is orthogonal to the contrast of interest (in-phase versus anti-phase). These expectations were preregistered at the Open Science Framework: (https://osf.io/m9bv7/?view_only=18ccdf99a8e84afaaebee393debbabe3).
Modeling of stimulation currents
Request a detailed protocolCurrent density under the electrodes was simulated using SIMNIBS version 3.1 (https://simnibs.github.io/simnibs/build/html/index.html; Thielscher et al., 2015). We used the template head model and standard conductivities provided by SIMNIBS. Electrodes were placed over SMC and aPFC and direct currents of 1 mA were estimated to run from the inner electrode toward the outer ring. Electrode placement and current density estimates for both electrode pairs are visualized in Figure 2A.
fMRI analyses – data preprocessing
Request a detailed protocolAll processing of the images was performed using MELODIC 3.00 as implemented in FSL 6.0.0 (https://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk). Images were motion corrected using MCFLIRT (Jenkinson et al., 2002), and distortions in the magnetic field were corrected using TOPUP (Andersson et al., 2003). Functional images were rigid-body registered to the brain extracted structural image using FLIRT. Registration to MNI 2 mm standard space was done using the nonlinear registration tool FNIRT. Images were spatially smoothed using a Gaussian 5 mm kernel and high pass filtered with a cut-off that was automatically estimated based on the task structure. Independent component analysis was run with a pre-specified maximum of 100 components (Beckmann and Smith, 2004); these components were manually inspected to remove potential sources of noise; (Griffanti et al., 2017). fMRI analyses – signal-to-noise (SNR) artefacts resulting from electrode presence.
To assess whether the presence of electrodes on the scalp had an effect on the signal-to-noise ratio in the fMRI signal, we estimated temporal signal-to-noise ratio (tSNR) from the right aPFC (electrode present) to the left aPFC (electrode not present). tSNR was calculated by dividing the mean of the signal over time by the standard deviation, separately for the left and right aPFC. These estimates were extracted from a mask of the lateral frontal pole (Neubert et al., 2014). We also compared the mean signal extracted from the right to the left aPFC.
fMRI analyses - GLM
Request a detailed protocolFirst and second level GLM analyses were performed using FEAT 6.00 implemented in FSL 6.0.0. The first-level model consisted of 12 task regressors: Approach angry, approach happy, avoid angry, and avoid happy trials were modelled separately for each stimulation condition (in-phase, anti-phase and sham). In each regressor, each event covered the time interval from presentation of a face until the corresponding onset of the joystick movement. Estimated head translations/rotations during scanning (six regressors), temporal derivatives of those translations/rotations (six regressors), and MR-signals in white matter and cerebrospinal fluid (two regressors) were included to the GLM as nuisance covariates. We considered the following comparisons. Emotional control effects were estimated by comparing incongruent trials (approach angry and avoid happy) to congruent trials across the three stimulation conditions (total congruency effect), as well as separately for each stimulation condition (in-phase, out-of-phase, and sham congruency effects). Overall stimulation effects were estimated by comparing in-phase and anti-phase stimulation conditions to the sham condition, aggregated across congruent and incongruent conditions. We reasoned that this contrast would combine potential entrainment and compensatory mechanisms and thereby result in an unbiased estimate reflecting the extent to which the underlying neural tissue was perturbed by the stimulation; yielding a ‘tACS-dose’ measure. Phase-dependent stimulation effects were estimated by comparing in-phase stimulation to anti-phase stimulation, across congruent and incongruent conditions. First-level models of the two separate sessions were combined using fixed effects analyses implemented in FEAT. Group effects were assessed using FLAME one with outlier de-weighting (Woolrich et al., 2004), making family-wise error corrected cluster-level inferences using a cluster-forming threshold of z > 2.3. This threshold provides a false error rate of around 5% when using FSL’s FLAME 1 (Eklund et al., 2015).
fMRI analyses – dynamic causal modeling
Request a detailed protocolDynamic causal modeling (DCM 12.5), implemented in SPM 12, was used to make inferences on network effects of the tACS manipulation (Friston et al., 2003). Dynamic causal modeling is an approach that aims to infer hidden neuronal dynamics from neuroimaging data (Friston et al., 2003; Stephan et al., 2010). It requires the specification of plausible generative models describing how neural activity leads to observed neuroimaging data through hemodynamic functions. This approach allows formal comparison of different models explaining the same data (Stephan et al., 2009; Penny et al., 2010). DCM provides estimates of effective connectivity between neural populations, and its modulation by experimental conditions (e.g. in this case tACS present or absent). We preregistered the hypothesis that changes in connectivity between aPFC and SMC due to tACS might be mediated by the amygdala, a region linked to the regulation of social-emotional action tendencies (Volman et al., 2011a; Kaldewaij et al., 2019; Bramson et al., 2020). To test this hypothesis, we constructed a model space in which each model contained three regions; the left amygdala (see GLM results); the right aPFC; and the left SMC.
The regions of interest were defined separately for each participant based on their functional effects (from the GLM analyses) but constrained to a-priori determined regions; right aPFC, based on the FPl mask (den Uyl et al., 2015; Voytek et al., 2015); left SMC, based on a previous study involving the same task [MNI: −28–32 64] (Bramson et al., 2018); and the left amygdala, based on the automated anatomical labeling mask (Volman et al., 2011a; Kaldewaij et al., 2019; Tzourio-Mazoyer et al., 2002). The timeseries of the first eigenvariate across all significant voxels in the ROI for the amygdala and SMC were extracted from a sphere with 3 mm radius (amygdala) and 5 mm radius (SMC) around the peak voxel detected in each ROI in a GLM F-contrast across effects of interest. aPFC time series were extracted from a sphere with 3 mm radius around the peak voxel in the incongruent >congruent t-contrast in the right lateral frontal pole (Neubert et al., 2014).
Model structure for all models under comparison was based on the minimal architecture needed to dissociate whether synchronization between aPFC and SMC was gated by the amygdala or via other region(s). All models consisted of a directed connection between the amygdala and SMC (Grèzes et al., 2014) (indicated as amygdala→SMC, amygdala → aPFC, and aPFC → amygdala [Bramson et al., 2020; Folloni et al., 2019]). A subset of models also included aPFC → SMC (Figure 3), which captures the connectivity between aPFC and SMC gated via cerebral structures other than the amygdala given that the two structures do not share a monosynaptic connection (Neubert et al., 2014). Return connections SMC → amygdala and SMC → aPFC were not included. We assumed these to be unnecessary to explain the effects of synchronization given the hierarchical nature of prefrontal control (Badre and D'Esposito, 2009; Badre and Nee, 2018). The onset of the stimulus in each trial (presentation of the face) was taken as a driving input (DCM.C matrix) feeding into the amygdala node of each model. Emotional control (all incongruent trials) and tACS condition (all in-phase trials, all anti-phase trials, or a combination; Figure 3—figure supplement 1) were allowed to modulate different connections (DCM.B matrix).
We created 84 models that systematically varied in four dimensions of interest, which were compared using family-wise model comparison (Penny et al., 2010). Dimension 1 was the modulation of emotional control, where control was allowed to modulate either amygdala → aPFC connection or aPFC self-connections (Volman et al., 2013). Dimension 2 was the connectivity structure of the models. This dimension consisted of two different model types: with or without a direct connection from aPFC → SMC (Figure 3—figure supplement 1A). The presence of this connection can account for influence of aPFC on SMC that is not gated by the amygdala. Dimension 3 reflects the nature of the tACS effects (Figure 3—figure supplement 1B; Piray et al., 2017). We modeled no effect of tACS (Stim 0); only the in-phase condition (Stim 1); only the anti-phase condition (Stim 2); both conditions separately (Stim 3); both conditions with opposite sign and similar amplitude (Stim 4). Dimension 4 arbitrated over the location of tACS modulation (Figure 3A, Figure 3—figure supplement 1C), which was allowed to modulate the amygdala → SMC, aPFC → amygdala, and aPFC → SMC, or any combination of the three connections. We differentiated these possibilities by separating all models into model families for each dimension, allowing inferences per dimension while averaging over all other parameters (Figure 3—figure supplement 1; Penny et al., 2010).
After model comparison, we extracted connectivity estimates that were altered by tACS from the winning model and used those parameters to predict tACS effects on behaviour. The estimated changes in connectivity due to tACS are estimated independently of the behavioral effects of tACS, and were used to test for an interaction between emotional control (congruent, incongruent), stimulations phase (in-phase, anti-phase) and effective connectivity (DCM.B matrix) using RM-ANOVA and Bayesian mixed effects models.
DWI analyses
Request a detailed protocolAll analyses of diffusion data were performed in FSL’s FDT 3.0 (https://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk). We used TOPUP to estimate susceptibility artifacts using additional b = 0 volumes with reverse-phase coding direction (Andersson et al., 2003). Next, EDDY correction was used (using the fieldmap estimated by TOPUP) to correct distortions caused by eddy currents and movement (Andersson and Sotiropoulos, 2016). We used BedpostX to fit a crossing fiber model with three fiber directions (Behrens et al., 2007). Connections between aPFC and BA6 were reconstructed using probtrackx2. Seed mask was the lateral frontal pole (Neubert et al., 2014), which is a mask that contains voxels bordering white matter. Target mask was area BA6 contained within the Juelich atlas in FSL. This probabilistic mask was thresholded to contain only voxels that were at least 50% likely to be in BA6. Tractography was run twice with the standard settings recommended in probtrackX and an exclusion mask in the midline. Estimated connection strengths were normalized to unit length within participants. We averaged all connections over participants and thresholded this volume at 5 to create a mask of likely pathways linking FPl (within aPFC) and BA6. In the second step, we constrained the tractography to either only include connections via the thalamus, or only via medial and lateral pathways through the prefrontal cortex.
Tract-based spatial statistics (TBSS; Smith et al., 2006) was used to assess whether the white matter integrity in the voxels in this connection mask explained part of the variance in response to tACS. TBSS was run using the default settings provided by FSL. In short, FA images were eroded and nonlinearly registered to the standard space. Afterward, we derived a mean skeleton based on all participants and thresholded the skeletonized FA values at 2. We then used randomize (Winkler et al., 2014) with threshold-free cluster enhancement (Smith and Nichols, 2009) to make inferences on the correlation between FA integrity and DCM effective connectivity estimates (in-phase versus anti-phase). To reduce the search space, the comparison was constrained to voxels that were part of the mask linking FPl to BA6 with and without the constrained connectivity (see Figure 3—figure supplement 2).
Appendix 1
Supplementary Results
Participants performed a social-emotional approach-avoidance task in which they approached or avoided happy and angry faces using a joystick. Approaching angry- and avoiding happy faces is incongruent with automatic action tendencies to approach appetitive and avoid threatening stimuli, and requires control over these prepotent habitual action tendencies (Figure 1; Volman et al., 2011a; Bramson et al., 2020; Roelofs et al., 2009). fMRI was acquired during task performance whilst participants received on-line dual-site theta-gamma tACS over aPFC and SMC, Figure 2A;B. Control over social-emotional action tendencies involves phase-amplitude coupling between prefrontal theta- and SMC gamma-band rhythmic activity (Bramson et al., 2018), the phase organisation of which is best reflected by the in-phase condition. In-phase, anti-phase and sham conditions (Figure 2B), were alternated throughout the task, allowing comparison of on-line, within-subject effects of stimulation conditions.
Signal quality below the electrodes
There was a reduction in absolute signal in right aPFC below the frontal electrode as compared to the contralateral aPFC, t(40) = 5.5, p<0.001. This consisted of an average reduction in signal intensity of 11% (SD 10%). However, there was no difference in temporal SNR in right aPFC as compared to left; t(40) = 0.2, p=0.7. These results are in line with earlier reports on noise induced by the presence of electrodes and stimulation equipment in the scanner bore (Antal et al., 2014). Although the absolute MR signal is reduced due to the presence of the tACS electrode, there is no effect on the SNR of the underlying brain region, suggesting that condition differences can be estimated reliably. This is supported by the consistency between the results obtained in the congruency contrast across stimulation conditions, and the results obtained in earlier reports using the same paradigm (Figure 1C, Figure 2—figure supplement 1; Volman et al., 2011a; Bramson et al., 2020; Roelofs et al., 2009)
Behavioral and neural costs of controlling social-emotional action tendencies
Across all conditions (stimulation and sham combined), reaction times were longer in the incongruent (M = 687 ms, SD = 141) than in the congruent condition (M = 637 ms, SD = 111), brms estimate B = 23.1 ms, credible interval (CI) [15 31]; paired t-test congruent versus incongruent t(40) = 5.9, p<0.001, Cohen’s d (M1-M2/SDpooled) = 0.39 [.22 1]. Participants also made more errors in the incongruent condition (94.8% correct, SD = 3.4%) than in the congruent (96.9% correct, SD = 2.3%), this was significant for both aggregated scores: t(40) = 5.4, p<0.001, d = 0.7 [.01 1.35], as well as on the trial-level: B = 0.29 CI [.16. 42]. The latter parameter is a log odds and indicates that participants are more likely to make correct responses in the congruent than incongruent trials. These effects illustrate the behavioral cost of controlling automatic action tendencies.
Whole-brain cluster-corrected fMRI analyses show that controlling social-emotional action tendencies elicited stronger BOLD activity in bilateral prefrontal cortex: MNI [−18 54–16] and [40 42 32], extending into lateral frontal pole (Bramson et al., 2020; Neubert et al., 2014); bilateral posterior parietal cortex (PPC) MNI: [38 -58 38] and [−56–44 46]; precuneus [6 -62 72], temporal cortex [−66–48 0]; and inferior frontal gyrus [60 20 4], Figure 1C, Figure 2—figure supplement 1C. Activity in the parahippocampal gyrus [−24–40 −14], extending into hippocampus and amygdala, medial prefrontal cortex [0 66 2], and lateral occipital cortex [52 -74 -10] was stronger for congruent than for incongruent trials. Similar effects have been reported in several earlier reports (Volman et al., 2011a; Kaldewaij et al., 2019; Bramson et al., 2020), with prefrontal effects depending crucially on the interaction between the emotional content of the action and the emotional content of the percept, over and above the emotional value of the action or stimulus alone (Volman et al., 2011a; Volman et al., 2011b; Roelofs et al., 2009).
Cerebral effects of dual-site phase-coupled tACS versus Sham
During task performance each participant received concurrent transcranial alternating current stimulation on two locations; aPFC (Bramson et al., 2020; Neubert et al., 2014) and sensorimotor cortex, Figure 2A;B. These locations, frequency, and phase relationship were based directly on a previous study showing aPFC-SMC phase-amplitude coupling in theta-gamma band rhythmic signals (Bramson et al., 2018). Whole-brain cluster-corrected analyses contrasting both stimulation conditions versus sham showed increased activity in several brain regions, most of which overlapped with task relevant regions: right prefrontal cortex [42 16 48], extending into FPl [22 60 0]; bilateral posterior parietal cortex [50 -58 50] and [−56–44 46], Figure 2C; Figure 2—figure supplement 1C;D. These effects support previous experimental and theoretical work suggesting that tACS effects on-going neural activity and effects are task-dependent (Ruhnau et al., 2016).
Cerebral effects of in-phase versus anti-phase dual-site phase-coupled tACS
Contrasting in-phase with anti-phase conditions showed stronger activity in left aPFC [−28 50–10], contralateral to stimulated right aPFC, and left PPC [−56–46 52], Figure 2—figure supplement 1E. These results suggest that desynchronising right aPFC and left SMC might induce compensatory effects in the contralateral homotopic control regions. These findings illustrate the importance of measuring online effects of brain stimulation because stimulation might have distal effects on neural tissue (de Graaf and Sack, 2018).
Dose dependent effects of tACS on social-emotional control
To assess whether the tACS intervention increases control over social-emotional behaviour, we compared behavioral congruency effects between different stimulation conditions whilst controlling for dose dependent effects. Dose dependent responses were calculated by extracting the differential BOLD signal under the prefrontal stimulated region (right aPFC, [22 60 0]) when comparing stimulation versus sham epochs. This BOLD effect provides a participant-by-participant index of aPFC response to the tACS intervention that is orthogonal to the in-phase versus anti-phase comparison.
Following the expectation preregistered at the Open Science Framework: (https://osf.io/m9bv7/?view_only=18ccdf99a8e84afaaebee393debbabe3), there was a three way interaction between emotional control, phase condition, and tACS-dose, both on the trial level; B = 0.1 CI [.007. 2], and in aggregated error rates; RM-ANOVA, F(1,39) = 9.3, p=0.004, partial eta2 = 0.19, indicating that participants performed differently in the two stimulation conditions as a function of the strength of the tACS influence. Post-hoc correlation analyses between BOLD change due to tACS and congruency effects showed that these effects are mainly driven by the in-phase condition: Spearman’s Rho = 0.32, p=0.04, Figure 2B. This finding indicates that, in the in-phase condition, larger BOLD responses to the inhibitory theta-band rhythm corresponded to smaller behavioral congruency effects. In the anti-phase condition, the correlation between tACS-dose and behavioral congruency effects was not significant (Spearman’s Rho = −0.23, p=0.14). However, the sign of the correlation matches the preregistered expectations: increased entrainment during anti-phase stimulation (reflected in lower BOLD response during theta-band stimulation; Behrens et al., 2007) would lead to larger congruency effects due to desynchronization between aPFC and SMC. Differences in BOLD signal between anti-phase and in-phase conditions (Figure 2—figure supplement 1E) could reflect the engagement of the control network contralateral to the stimulated circuit. This engagement might reflect compensatory mechanisms counteracting disruptive effects of anti-phase stimulation (Sack et al., 2005; de Graaf and Sack, 2018), thereby obscuring the relationship between tACS-dose and increases in behavioral congruency. However, the BOLD signal difference between anti-phase and in-phase conditions extracted from aPFC and PPC (Figure 2—figure supplement 1E) did not correlate with anti-phase behavioral congruency effects (both r(39) <.14, p>0.3). Controlling for baseline congruency effects (in the sham condition) did not change these results; Rho = 0.32, p=0.044 for in-phase; Rho = −0.26, p=0.09 for anti-phase. These results suggest that theta-gamma phase-coupled tACS increases control over social-emotional behaviour in participants that respond to stimulation. Contrasting error-rate congruency effects (% correct for congruent - % correct for incongruent trials) without accounting for the dose dependent responses did not show a statistically reliable group effect of stimulation condition on the trial level, B = 0.05 CI [−0.05. 14], nor on the aggregated scores RM-ANOVA; F(1,39) = 3.75, p=0.06, partial eta2 = 0.088, Figure 2—figure supplement 1A.
Reaction times did not show differential effects in the different stimulation conditions, emotional control*phase condition*tACS-dose; F(1,40) = 0.05, p=0.8, congruency*stimulation; F(1,41) = 2.3, p=0.14, Figure 2—figure supplement 1B. The lack of effects on reaction times is in line with expectations preregistered at the Open Science Framework: (https://osf.io/m9bv7/?view_only=18ccdf99a8e84afaaebee393debbabe3) as well as earlier stimulation studies using this same task (Volman et al., 2011a). This finding suggests that the aPFC control required during the AA task operates at the level of action selection.
tACS changes aPFC-SMC effective connectivity, which correlates with behavioral effects
To assess the network effects of dual-site tACS, we used dynamic causal modeling (DCM; Friston et al., 2003) verifying whether aPFC→SMC connectivity was gated by the amygdala. We created a set of 84 models with two different connectivity structures; with and without direct connection between aPFC and SMC, Figure 3—figure supplement 1A. These models differed in the way tACS could affect connectivity, Figure 3—figure supplement 1B. Possibilities consisted of no effect of stimulation (Stim 0), effects of one stimulation condition (Stim 1 and Stim 2, Sup Figure 2B), independent effects of both conditions (Stim 3) or opposite sign but equal amplitude effects of both stimulation conditions (Stim 4). Finally, models varied with respect to the location of the tACS modulation: amygdala → SMC, aPFC → amygdala or aPFC → SMC, two of those three connections, or all three connections, Figure 3—figure supplement 1C.
Family comparisons showed that the structural model containing a direct connection from aPFC →SMC was more likely than a model without this connection, Figure 3—figure supplement 1A. This suggests that not all effects of aPFC→ SMC are mediated by the amygdala. Comparing the different possible effects of stimulation showed that models in which both stimulation conditions were modeled separately had the strongest model evidence, Figure 2B. Finally, comparisons over possible locations where tACS could modulate connectivity showed tACS effects on all three modeled connections, Figure 3—figure supplement 1C
We extracted connectivity parameters from all three modulated connections and used them to account for differences in behavioral response. This showed that differences in effective connectivity between in- and anti-phase on the aPFC → SMC connection predicted differences in behavioral effects between stimulation conditions both on the trial level; B = 0.11 CI [.02. 2] and on aggregated congruency-effects; RM-ANOVA; F(1,39) = 6.01 p = 0.019, np2 = 0.13. Over the whole group connectivity parameters did not differ between in- and anti-phase conditions, t(40),=1.1, p=0.27. Post-hoc tests showed that those participants that showed more negative connectivity between aPFC→SMC in the in-phase as compared to anti-phase condition showed a reduction in congruency effects in the in-phase as compared to the anti-phase condition, t(21) = −2.9, p=0.008. This was not the case for participants with no difference, or more negative connectivity for anti-phase versus in-phase, p=0.35; main Figure 3B. This improvement of control in the in-phase condition remains when outliers in behavioral congruency are removed, t(20),=−2.7, p=0.012, and if we remove the participants that did not show a response to amygdala input in the DCM analysis (n = 3), t(19) = −2.8, p=0.01. Strikingly, for the participants with stronger negative connectivity between aPFC→SMC the congruency effect was no longer significantly different from zero, t(21),=0.99 p=0.33 in the in-phase condition, indicating that they did no longer perform worse when having to override automatic action-tendencies. Participants that did not show tACS modulation of aPFC→SMC connections in the in-phase condition did show a congruency effect t(18) = 2.6, p=0.018. These effects suggest that successful synchronization of aPFC-SMC rhythmic activity increases control over social-emotional behavior by increasing aPFC inhibition over SMC.
To explore whether structural characteristics might underlie individual differences in response to stimulation, we tested whether differences between aPFC→SMC connectivity parameters between in-phase and anti-phase conditions correlated with FA integrity in voxels potentially connecting aPFC to SMC. We observed a correlation between FA integrity and DCM connectivity estimates for in-phase versus anti-phase in white matter regions leading up to right BA6, tfce corrected for voxels in a thresholded mask connecting FPl (aPFC) and BA6; p=0.046, Figure 3—figure supplement 2. Here increased inhibition in the in-phase condition (relative to anti-phase) is related to higher FA integrity in white matter leading up to BA6. Further exploration showed that this correlation is present when considering cortico-thalamo-cortical connections; corrected p<0.048 but not when only considering cortico-cortical connections. These findings suggest that tACS synchronization of aPFC→SMC connectivity might partly depend on the structural integrity in aPFC-thalamic-SMC connectivity (Halassa and Kastner, 2017). However, given that these results are exploratory, seem to be driven by a relatively small number of participants (Figure 3—figure supplement 2B), and depend on the threshold chosen for creating the masks, they need to be considered with caution and warrant replication.
Data availability
Data used for all reported analyses are available from the donders data repository: data.donders.ru.nl, collection identifier: di.dccn.DSC_3023010.01_369.
-
Donders RepositoryID DSC_3023010.01_369. Influencing control over social-emotional actions using tACS-fMRI.
References
-
Is the rostro-caudal Axis of the frontal lobe hierarchical?Nature Reviews Neuroscience 10:659–669.https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2667
-
Frontal cortex and the hierarchical control of behaviorTrends in Cognitive Sciences 22:170–188.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2017.11.005
-
ConferenceProbabilistic independent component analysis for functional magnetic resonance imagingIEEE Transactions on Medical Imaging. pp. 137–152.https://doi.org/10.1109/TMI.2003.822821
-
Cortical oscillatory mechanisms supporting the control of human Social-Emotional actionsThe Journal of Neuroscience 38:5739–5749.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3382-17.2018
-
Human lateral frontal pole contributes to control over emotional Approach-Avoidance actionsThe Journal of Neuroscience 40:2925–2934.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2048-19.2020
-
The functional role of cross-frequency couplingTrends in Cognitive Sciences 14:506–515.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2010.09.001
-
The neural bases of emotion regulationNature Reviews Neuroscience 16:693–700.https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn4044
-
G*power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciencesBehavior Research Methods 39:175–191.https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03193146
-
Thalamic functions in distributed cognitive controlNature Neuroscience 20:1669–1679.https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-017-0020-1
-
Survival of the friendliest: Homo sapiens Evolved via Selection for ProsocialityAnnual Review of Psychology 68:155–186.https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010416-044201
-
Oscillatory dynamics of prefrontal cognitive controlTrends in Cognitive Sciences 20:916–930.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2016.09.007
-
Frontal control over automatic emotional action tendencies predicts acute stress responsivityBiological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging 4:975–983.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsc.2019.06.011
-
Sustained aftereffect of α-tACS lasts up to 70 min after stimulationFrontiers in Human Neuroscience 10:245.https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2016.00245
-
Emotional control, reappraisedNeuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 95:528–534.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.11.003
-
Comparing families of dynamic causal modelsPLOS Computational Biology 6:e1000709.https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000709
-
Low-Intensity electrical stimulation affects network dynamics by modulating population rate and spike timingJournal of Neuroscience 30:15067–15079.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2059-10.2010
-
Working memory revived in older adults by synchronizing rhythmic brain circuitsNature Neuroscience 22:820–827.https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-019-0371-x
-
Emotion in action : a predictive processing perspective and theoretical synthesis emotion in action : an integrative viewEmotion Review : Journal of the International Society for Research on Emotion 9:319–325.https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073916661765
-
On the neural control of social emotional behaviorSocial Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 4:50–58.https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsn036
-
Stochastic resonance effects reveal the neural mechanisms of transcranial magnetic stimulationJournal of Neuroscience 31:3143–3147.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4863-10.2011
-
Bayesian model selection for group studiesNeuroImage 46:1004–1017.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.03.025
-
Ten simple rules for dynamic causal modelingNeuroImage 49:3099–3109.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.11.015
-
On the possible role of stimulation duration for after-effects of transcranial alternating current stimulationFrontiers in Cellular Neuroscience 9:1–7.https://doi.org/10.3389/fncel.2015.00311
-
Reduced serotonin transporter availability decreases prefrontal control of the amygdalaJournal of Neuroscience 33:8974–8979.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5518-12.2013
-
Oscillatory dynamics coordinating human frontal networks in support of goal maintenanceNature Neuroscience 18:1318–1324.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4071
Decision letter
-
Alexander ShackmanReviewing Editor; University of Maryland, United States
-
Richard B IvrySenior Editor; University of California, Berkeley, United States
In the interests of transparency, eLife publishes the most substantive revision requests and the accompanying author responses.
Acceptance summary:
Bramson et al. investigate the neural basis of emotional-action control. Building on their prior work, the authors hypothesized that coupling between the anterior prefrontal cortex (aPFC) and sensorimotor cortex (SMC) subserves emotion-action control. Here, they set to manipulate this hypothesized mechanism using dual-site transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS). Results reveal a dose-dependent effect of in-phase tACS on emotional action control, suggesting that aPFC-SMC coupling does indeed play a mechanistically crucial role in emotional action selection.
Decision letter after peer review:
Thank you for submitting your article "Improving emotional-action control by targeting long-range phase-amplitude neuronal coupling" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by two peer reviewers, and the evaluation has been overseen by a Reviewing Editor and Richard Ivry as the Senior Editor. The reviewers have opted to remain anonymous.
The reviewers have discussed the reviews with one another and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this decision to help you prepare a revised submission.
We would like to draw your attention to changes in our revision policy that we have made in response to COVID-19 (https://elifesciences.org/articles/57162). Specifically, we are asking editors to accept without delay manuscripts, like yours, that they judge can stand as eLife papers without additional data, even if they feel that they would make the manuscript stronger. Thus the revisions requested below only address clarity and presentation.
Summary:
Building on recent work by Voytek and others, Bramson et al. investigate the neural basis of emotional-action control continuing their previous line of research. In prior work (Bramson et al., 2018), the authors hypothesized that theta-gamma coupling between the anterior prefrontal cortex (aPFC) and sensorimotor cortex (SMC) subserves emotion-action control. Here, they set out to manipulate this mechanism to show its causal involvement. The authors use the approach-avoidance task separating emotionally-congruent and incongruent action tendencies while applying dual-site transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS) and monitoring neural activity using fMRI. Importantly, stimulation is set to either facilitate aPFC-SMC theta-gamma phase-amplitude coupling (in-phase) or disrupt it (anti-phase). Results reveal a dose-dependent effect of in-phase tACS on action control, suggesting that aPFC-SMC coupling does indeed play a critical role in action selection.
The reviewers and I were enthusiastic about the report.…
• The study is compelling and innovative. The authors use a multimethod approach combining brain stimulation, functional neuroimaging, diffusion-weighted imaging, and dynamic causal modeling to assemble an in-depth picture of emotional-action control mechanism in the human brain.
• The findings lay out the path for the potential application of tACS in anxiety therapy.
• I find the study interesting and contributing to scientific knowledge
• This report is an elegant and novel demonstration of a gain-of-function manipulation that improves control over emotional action tendencies via the delivery of alternating transcranial current to the cortical elements of a circuit hypothesized to play a role in emotional regulation.
• Particular strengths include an intuitive and established behavioral assay, multiple within-subject controls for stimulation parameters, and the simultaneous use of multiple techniques allowing the authors to establish a 'dose-dependent' effect of aPFC inhibition.
• Overall, the data presented here are a strong addition to the literature
Essential revisions:
Despite their enthusiasm, we do have a few questions about the interpretation, and some suggestions for further strengthening the report.
1) Approach. The decision to intermix stimulation protocols within a session needs to be better motivated.
• The authors use a within-subjects design without separating different stimulation protocols. The three stimulation conditions – in-phase, anti-phase, and sham tACS – are applied within the same session, in a block-design manner. Typically, within-subjects studies separate stimulation sessions by 48 hours or more to avoid carryover effects (Heise et al., 2019; Mansouri et al., 2019; Amador de Lara et al., 2018; Alekseichuk et al., 2016).
• It has been shown that tACS can induce aftereffects (for review, Vosskuhl et al., 2018).
• If here the authors made a design decision to mix together different stimulation conditions within a session, this decision needs to be justified. Providing the rationale behind the experimental design would benefit the paper and increase the credibility of the findings.
2) Approach/Results. Authors should provide evidence that the key results are not contaminated by potential carry-over effects from preceding blocks
3) Results. Figure 2D raises the possibility that the anti-phase condition produces a correlation between aPFC BOLD and the congruency effect that is roughly equal and opposite of what is observed in the in-phase condition (as opposed to producing no correlation, for instance). If this is a reasonable interpretation, then this potentially intriguing effect should be noted in the Results and noted in the Discussion.
4) Results. In general, the paper should be revised to clearly indicate the polarities and valence of tACS stimulation, BOLD effects, and changes in congruency effect.
• What is the difference between negative tACS dose in-phase and positive tACS dose anti-phase stimulation? The scatter plot and regression line in Figure 2D do not clearly communicate the difference between in-phase and anti-phase tACS with complementary tACS dose.
5) Discussion. Null results, and potential implications for major take-home points. Authors need to provide some discussion of their unsupported predictions.
• In their preregistered hypothesis (Open Science Framework), the authors predict an increase of congruency effect in the anti-phase stimulation condition (subsection “Behavioural analyses”, last paragraph), however, this expectation was not confirmed by the results (subsection “Dose dependent effects of tACS on social-emotional control”, second paragraph). The absence of the effect of anti-phase tACS that was expected to be in the opposite direction to the effect of in-phase stimulation raises questions about the causal involvement of aPFC-SMC coupling in emotional-action control.
• It is clear from the results that strengthening aPFC-SMC coupling enhances control.
• However, if the disruption of such in-phase coupling by the anti-phase stimulation doesn't lead to the reduction in control it suggests that some other mechanisms might play a compensatory role. I recommend addressing this issue in the discussion of the results.
6) Discussion/Abstract. The authors emphasize the clinical implications, but this discussion should be qualified and softened.
• In this experiment, a substantial change in brain activity (as measured by BOLD) leads to a reliable, but relatively subtle behavioral change in a task that asks participants to override a mild response tendency. Indeed, according to Figure 1B, even subjects performing relatively poorly in the incongruent condition are still getting it right more than 80% of the time, suggesting that this condition presents no more than a mild challenge.
• Emotional control will inevitably be harder in a real world scenario in which individuals will certainly perceive there to be far more at stake – this will be doubly true for any clinical population.
• Thus, claims like "Similar interventions could help patients suffering from emotional-social disorders" and others should be softened. The authors present fascinating research that should be characterized more as a first step in developing gain-of-function therapeutics, instead of a treatment ready for, or even on the cusp of, application in the clinic.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.59600.sa1Author response
Essential revisions:
Despite their enthusiasm, we do have a few questions about the interpretation, and some suggestions for further strengthening the report.
1) Approach. The decision to intermix stimulation protocols within a session needs to be better motivated.
• The authors use a within-subjects design without separating different stimulation protocols. The three stimulation conditions – in-phase, anti-phase, and sham tACS – are applied within the same session, in a block-design manner. Typically, within-subjects studies separate stimulation sessions by 48 hours or more to avoid carryover effects (Heise et al., 2019; Mansouri et al., 2019; Amador de Lara et al., 2018; Alekseichuk et al., 2016).
• It has been shown that tACS can induce aftereffects (for review, Vosskuhl et al., 2018).
• If here the authors made a design decision to mix together different stimulation conditions within a session, this decision needs to be justified. Providing the rationale behind the experimental design would benefit the paper and increase the credibility of the findings.
We agree that we could better clarify the design decision to mix different stimulation conditions within a session, and why tACS aftereffects are unlikely to explain the findings. To reiterate the context, the study is focused on measuring behavioral and fMRI effects during the delivery of tACS. Since tACS can induce aftereffects 1-70 minutes post stimulation (e.g. Strüber et al., 2015; Vossen et al., 2015; Kasten et al., 2016; Vosskuhl et al., 2018), it is relevant to discuss whether those aftereffects contribute to the reported findings.
tACS aftereffects are thought to depend mostly on synaptic plasticity (Vossen et al., 2015), and they are not observed for very short periods of stimulation as used in the current study (Strüber et al., 2015). The current study combines short periods of stimulation (1 min) with washout periods of at least 30 seconds between successive stimulation blocks. Studies that report aftereffects of tACS (e.g. Neuling et al., 2013; Alekseichuk et al., 2016; Kasten et al., 2016) use relatively long periods of continuous stimulation (at least > 10 minutes, reviewed in Strüber et al., 2015). The empirical evidence collected during this experiment confirms that plasticity induced aftereffects, if present, are marginal. Namely, tACS aftereffects would influence neural activity in the sham epochs systematically succeeding each stimulation epoch. Yet, we observe strong differences in BOLD signal when comparing stimulation and sham epochs under the prefrontal electrodes (Figure 1C).
Importantly, the experiment was designed to avoid systematic aftereffect biases in the main finding of this study. The different conditions (in-phase, anti-phase, and sham) were rapidly alternated. Plasticity aftereffects would require several minutes to build up (Vossen et al., 2015), and that implies that potential aftereffects would stretch over both in-phase and anti-phase conditions. New analyses show that behavioral congruency effects in the sham condition do not differ for trials preceded by in-phase as compared to anti-phase stimulation; t(40) = 0.4, p = 0.7, arguing against systematic offline influence of tACS on behavioral congruency. Aftereffects due to rhythmic entrainment are also unlikely. Entrainment is thought to fade out after a few cycles following the entraining stimulation (Halbleib et al., 2012; Strüber et al., 2015; Vossen et al., 2015), i.e. over a time-frame much shorter than the wash-out epochs interposed between the different stimulation conditions.
We have incorporated these considerations in the manuscript:
“tACS conditions (in-phase; anti-phase; sham) were applied within the same session with stimulation condition alternating between stimulus blocks (each lasting approximately 60 seconds; 12 trials), interleaved with periods of no stimulation (instructions between blocks; at least 30 seconds). Whereas previous studies have shown long lasting aftereffects of tACS, ranging up to 70 minutes (Kasten et al., 2016), we considered it unlikely that these would systematically bias the current results based on two arguments. […] Post-hoc analysis indeed revealed that behavioral congruency effects in the sham condition do not differ for trials preceded by in-phase as compared to anti-phase stimulation; t(40) = 0.4, p = 0.7, arguing against systematic offline influence of tACS on behavioral congruency. ”
2) Approach/Results. Authors should provide evidence that the key results are not contaminated by potential carry-over effects from preceding blocks
We consider it unlikely that the results we report are contaminated by carry-over effects for several reasons (see response to comment #1), most notably that we do not find any significant differences in sham periods following either in phase or anti phase stimulation. Additionally, stimulation conditions were varied pseudo-randomly between stimulus blocks, further averaging out any potential systematic carry-over effects.
We have added a control analysis, testing whether behavioral congruency effects for the sham session depend on the condition of the preceding stimulus block, and further clarified the pseudo-randomised structure of the stimulation protocol.
“Post-hoc analysis indeed revealed that behavioral congruency effects in the sham condition do not differ for trials preceded by in-phase as compared to anti-phase stimulation; t(40) = 0.4, p = 0.7, arguing against systematic offline influence of tACS on behavioral congruency.”
“Stimulation conditions were alternated between blocks in pseudo-random fashion with the constraint that stimulation condition could never be repeated for two consecutive blocks.”
3) Results. Figure 2D raises the possibility that the anti-phase condition produces a correlation between aPFC BOLD and the congruency effect that is roughly equal and opposite of what is observed in the in-phase condition (as opposed to producing no correlation, for instance). If this is a reasonable interpretation, then this potentially intriguing effect should be noted in the Results and noted in the Discussion.
This interpretation matches our a-priori formulated hypothesis on the effect of anti-phase stimulation (see also main point 5). That hypothesis is grounded on the observation that stronger power in the theta-band leads to stronger neuronal inhibition and stronger negative correlation with BOLD (Scheeringa et al., 2011). Accordingly, in the anti-phase condition, stronger theta-entrainment in aPFC (indexed by reduced tACS-related BOLD) would lead to stronger desynchronization between aPFC and SMC, and thus larger congruency effects. As indicated by the reviewers, the data in Figure 2D are consistent with this hypothesis. However, the correlation between tACS-dose in aPFC and behavioural congruency effects was significant in the in-phase condition (Spearman’s Rho = 0.32, p = 0.04) but not in the anti-phase condition (Spearman’s Rho = –0.23, p = 0.14). Figure 2—figure supplement 1E suggests that the weaker effects of anti-phase tACS on behavior might be due to enhanced activity in localized regions contralateral to the prefrontal stimulation. Namely, anti-phase stimulation (as compared to in-phase) enhanced BOLD responses in anatomically connected nodes of the emotional control circuit (aPFC, PPC (Mars et al., 2011; Koch et al., 2018)). Speculatively, this enhanced BOLD response might be interpreted as the emotional control circuit partially compensating for the aPFC-SMC desynchronization caused by the anti-phase stimulation. This compensation might lead to a weaker tACS-behavior relation in the anti-phase condition. However, post-hoc exploration did not reveal a straightforward relationship between local effects (in left aPFC, PPC) and behavioral congruency in the anti-phase condition. A dedicated study might test whether network-level effects explain the behavioral consequences of those enhanced responses in the emotional control circuit following anti-phase stimulation. We have added these considerations and added post-hoc analyses to the manuscript:
“We did not observe the anticipated anti-phase tACS induced increase of congruency effects caused by a desynchronization of aPFC and SMC (Polanía et al., 2015). […] Future studies could focus on characterizing these potential compensatory effects.”
“In the anti-phase condition, the correlation between tACS-dose and behavioural congruency effects was not significant (Spearman’s Rho = –0.23, p = 0.14). […] However, the BOLD signal difference between anti-phase and in-phase conditions extracted from aPFC and PPC (Figure 2—figure supplement 1E) did not correlate with anti-phase behavioural congruency effects (both r(39) < 0.14 , p > 0.3)”.
4) Results. In general, the paper should be revised to clearly indicate the polarities and valence of tACS stimulation, BOLD effects, and changes in congruency effect.
• What is the difference between negative tACS dose in-phase and positive tACS dose anti-phase stimulation? The scatter plot and regression line in Figure 2D do not clearly communicate the difference between in-phase and anti-phase tACS with complementary tACS dose.
Polarities of tACS stimulation: The polarity of a tACS intervention changes sinusoidally over time within each cycle. We have clarified this:
“Stimulation was applied on-line during task performance in blocks of approximately 60 seconds (i.e. the length of a stimulus block; 12 trials), and consisted of electrical fields that changed polarity between the inner- and outer rings in theta-band (6 Hz) frequency over the aPFC and gamma-band (75 Hz), tapered with a 6 Hz sine wave over SMC”
Valence of tACS stimulation: It is not clear to us what the reviewers mean by “valence” of the tACS stimulation. Assuming the issue is about the phase relation between the two stimulation sites, then this information is provided in Figure 2A, B and in the subsection “tACS stimulation parameters”.
BOLD effects: We considered two independent BOLD effects. First, we analysed BOLD signals to verify that emotional congruency demands evoked activity in brain locations (notably aPFC) and with effect sizes matching previous reports (Figure 1). Second, we used BOLD signal analyses to quantify the effect of tACS on aPFC. To provide an unbiased estimate of how strongly aPFC is perturbed by stimulation, we computed a tACS-dose estimate, i.e. the degree to which aPFC BOLD response differs between stimulation (both in-phase and anti-phase conditions combined) and sham. This tACS-dose estimate is a combination of potential entrainment and compensatory effects acting on the right aPFC under the electrodes. We have tried to clarify this reasoning,
“Overall stimulation effects were estimated by comparing in-phase and anti-phase stimulation conditions to the sham condition, aggregated across congruent and incongruent conditions. We reasoned that this contrast would combine potential entrainment and compensatory mechanisms and thereby result in an unbiased estimate reflecting the extent to which the underlying neural tissue was perturbed by the stimulation; yielding a “tACS-dose” measure.”
Changes in congruency effect: The tACS-related changes in behavioural congruency effects are reported in Figures 2 and 3. The behavioral congruency effect was illustrated in Figure 1, as the difference in percentage correct between the incongruent and congruent conditions. Following the reviewers’ comments, we now introduce the congruency effect (Figure 1B) as error rate, i.e. in a manner consistent with its use in Figures 2, 3.
The scatter plot and regression line in Figure 2D do not clearly communicate the difference between in-phase and anti-phase tACS with complementary tACS dose: The interpretation of the relationship between behavioral congruency in each condition, and the BOLD response depends on the negative relationship between theta-band activity and BOLD effects (Scheeringa et al., 2011). Accordingly, reduced BOLD during theta-band stimulation is taken as an indication of stronger entrainment of the endogenous theta-band activity. This interpretational framework yields contrasting predictions for the two in-phase and anti-phase conditions. In the in-phase condition, increased aPFC theta-band entrainment would lead to reduced congruency effects, due to increased phase-amplitude coupling between aPFC and SMC. This situation would result in a positive correlation between tACS dose and behavioral congruency effects. In the anti-phase condition, increased entrainment would bias aPFC theta phase away from SMC dynamics. This situation would lead to larger congruency effects, i.e. a negative correlation between tACS dose and behavior. This is the pattern we observe (although not significantly for the anti-phase condition).
We could not find a better way to graphically present individual datapoints as well as summary statistics than Figure 2D. However, we have clarified the figure legend describing panel 2D as follows:
“D) Participants with stronger inhibitory responses to theta-band stimulation over aPFC (manifested as decreased BOLD signal; Scheeringa et al., 2011) improved their control over emotional actions (decreased congruency effect) during aPFC-SMC in-phase tACS (in green), but not during aPFC-SMC anti-phase tACS (in red), reflected in the interaction between Emotional Control (congruent, incongruent), Stimulations Phase (in-phase, anti-phase) and tACS-Dose (BOLD signal in aPFC during stimulation vs. sham (panel C)); F(1,39) = 9.3, p = 0.004, partial eta2 = 0.19. […] The direction of the effect in the anti-phase condition tentatively suggests that stronger entrainment to the theta-band stimulation (decreased BOLD) increases congruency effects, as would be expected when aPFC-SMC communication is disrupted.”
5) Discussion. Null results, and potential implications for major take-home points. Authors need to provide some discussion of their unsupported predictions.
• In their preregistered hypothesis (Open Science Framework), the authors predict an increase of congruency effect in the anti-phase stimulation condition (subsection “Behavioural analyses”, last paragraph), however, this expectation was not confirmed by the results (subsection “Dose dependent effects of tACS on social-emotional control”, second paragraph). The absence of the effect of anti-phase tACS that was expected to be in the opposite direction to the effect of in-phase stimulation raises questions about the causal involvement of aPFC-SMC coupling in emotional-action control.
• It is clear from the results that strengthening aPFC-SMC coupling enhances control.
• However, if the disruption of such in-phase coupling by the anti-phase stimulation doesn't lead to the reduction in control it suggests that some other mechanisms might play a compensatory role. I recommend addressing this issue in the discussion of the results.
This issue largely overlaps with point #3, and we refer the reviewers to the arguments put forward there to interpret the unsupported prediction of the effect of anti-phase tACS.
We have amended the Discussion section to more clearly emphasize these points (see also point #3):
“We did not observe the anticipated anti-phase tACS induced increase of congruency effects caused by a desynchronization of aPFC and SMC (Polanía et al., 2015). […] Future studies could focus on characterizing these potential compensatory effects.”
6) Discussion/Abstract. The authors emphasize the clinical implications, but this discussion should be qualified and softened.
• In this experiment, a substantial change in brain activity (as measured by BOLD) leads to a reliable, but relatively subtle behavioral change in a task that asks participants to override a mild response tendency. Indeed, according to Figure 1B, even subjects performing relatively poorly in the incongruent condition are still getting it right more than 80% of the time, suggesting that this condition presents no more than a mild challenge.
• Emotional control will inevitably be harder in a real world scenario in which individuals will certainly perceive there to be far more at stake – this will be doubly true for any clinical population.
• Thus, claims like "Similar interventions could help patients suffering from emotional-social disorders" and others should be softened. The authors present fascinating research that should be characterized more as a first step in developing gain-of-function therapeutics, instead of a treatment ready for, or even on the cusp of, application in the clinic.
We agree with the reviewers that our claims were a bit strong and the manipulation needs further study before clinical applications are possible. We have amended the Discussion section accordingly:
“These findings provide a first stepping stone towards the development of interventions aimed to help patients suffering from social-emotional disorders. […] Future studies could explore whether effects of in-phase stimulation hold for more challenging emotional control situations (e.g. Kaldewaij et al., 2019) and generate long-lasting effects through exposure treatment.”
References:
Mars RB, Jbabdi S, Sallet J, O’Reilly JX, Croxson PL, Olivier E, Noonan MP, Bergmann C, Mitchell AS, Baxter MG (2011) Diffusion-weighted imaging tractography-based parcellation of the human parietal cortex and comparison with human and macaque resting-state functional connectivity. J Neurosci 31:4087–4100.
Neuling T, Rach S, Herrmann CS (2013) Orchestrating neuronal networks: sustained after-effects of transcranial alternating current stimulation depend upon brain states. Front Hum Neurosci 7:161.
Vosskuhl J, Strüber D, Herrmann CS (2018) Non-invasive Brain Stimulation : A Paradigm Shift in Understanding Brain Oscillations. 12:1–19.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.59600.sa2Article and author information
Author details
Funding
Nederlandse Organisatie voor Wetenschappelijk Onderzoek ((#453-12-001)
- Karin Roelofs
European Commission (ERC_CoG-2017_772337)
- Karin Roelofs
The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.
Acknowledgements
The authors would like to thank Freek Nieuwhof and Uriel Plones for their help in setting up the tACS-fMRI setup and Amy Abelmann for her assistance in data collection. This work was supported by a VICI grant (#453-12-001) from the Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (NWO) and a consolidator grant from the European Research Council (ERC_CoG-2017_772337) awarded to Karin Roelofs.
Ethics
Human subjects: The study was approved by the local ethics committee (CMO2014/288). All participants gave informed consent for participation and publication of anonymized data.
Senior Editor
- Richard B Ivry, University of California, Berkeley, United States
Reviewing Editor
- Alexander Shackman, University of Maryland, United States
Version history
- Received: June 3, 2020
- Accepted: October 6, 2020
- Version of Record published: October 27, 2020 (version 1)
Copyright
© 2020, Bramson et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
-
- 2,022
- Page views
-
- 262
- Downloads
-
- 15
- Citations
Article citation count generated by polling the highest count across the following sources: Crossref, PubMed Central, Scopus.
Download links
Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)
Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)
Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)
Further reading
-
- Genetics and Genomics
- Neuroscience
A hexanucleotide repeat expansion in C9ORF72 is the most common genetic cause of amyotrophic lateral sclerosis (ALS) and frontotemporal dementia (FTD). A hallmark of ALS/FTD pathology is the presence of dipeptide repeat (DPR) proteins, produced from both sense GGGGCC (poly-GA, poly-GP, poly-GR) and antisense CCCCGG (poly-PR, poly-PG, poly-PA) transcripts. Translation of sense DPRs, such as poly-GA and poly-GR, depends on non-canonical (non-AUG) initiation codons. Here, we provide evidence for canonical AUG-dependent translation of two antisense DPRs, poly-PR and poly-PG. A single AUG is required for synthesis of poly-PR, one of the most toxic DPRs. Unexpectedly, we found redundancy between three AUG codons necessary for poly-PG translation. Further, the eukaryotic translation initiation factor 2D (EIF2D), which was previously implicated in sense DPR synthesis, is not required for AUG-dependent poly-PR or poly-PG translation, suggesting that distinct translation initiation factors control DPR synthesis from sense and antisense transcripts. Our findings on DPR synthesis from the C9ORF72 locus may be broadly applicable to many other nucleotide repeat expansion disorders.
-
- Cell Biology
- Neuroscience
The amyloid beta (Aβ) plaques found in Alzheimer’s disease (AD) patients’ brains contain collagens and are embedded extracellularly. Several collagens have been proposed to influence Aβ aggregate formation, yet their role in clearance is unknown. To investigate the potential role of collagens in forming and clearance of extracellular aggregates in vivo, we created a transgenic Caenorhabditis elegans strain that expresses and secretes human Aβ1-42. This secreted Aβ forms aggregates in two distinct places within the extracellular matrix. In a screen for extracellular human Aβ aggregation regulators, we identified different collagens to ameliorate or potentiate Aβ aggregation. We show that a disintegrin and metalloprotease a disintegrin and metalloprotease 2 (ADM-2), an ortholog of ADAM9, reduces the load of extracellular Aβ aggregates. ADM-2 is required and sufficient to remove the extracellular Aβ aggregates. Thus, we provide in vivo evidence of collagens essential for aggregate formation and metalloprotease participating in extracellular Aβ aggregate removal.