Maternal cortisol is associated with neonatal amygdala microstructure and connectivity in a sexually dimorphic manner

  1. David Q Stoye
  2. Manuel Blesa
  3. Gemma Sullivan
  4. Paola Galdi
  5. Gillian J Lamb
  6. Gill S Black
  7. Alan J Quigley
  8. Michael J Thrippleton
  9. Mark E Bastin
  10. Rebecca M Reynolds
  11. James P Boardman  Is a corresponding author
  1. MRC Centre for Reproductive Health, University of Edinburgh, United Kingdom
  2. Department of Radiology, Royal Hospital for Sick Children, United Kingdom
  3. Centre for Clinical Brain Sciences, University of Edinburgh, United Kingdom
  4. Centre for Cardiovascular Science, University of Edinburgh, United Kingdom

Abstract

The mechanisms linking maternal stress in pregnancy with infant neurodevelopment in a sexually dimorphic manner are poorly understood. We tested the hypothesis that maternal hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis activity, measured by hair cortisol concentration (HCC), is associated with microstructure, structural connectivity, and volume of the infant amygdala. In 78 mother-infant dyads, maternal hair was sampled postnatally, and infants underwent magnetic resonance imaging at term-equivalent age. We found a relationship between maternal HCC and amygdala development that differed according to infant sex. Higher HCC was associated with higher left amygdala fractional anisotropy (β = 0.677, p=0.010), lower left amygdala orientation dispersion index (β = −0.597, p=0.034), and higher fractional anisotropy in connections between the right amygdala and putamen (β = 0.475, p=0.007) in girls compared to boys. Furthermore, altered amygdala microstructure was only observed in boys, with connectivity changes restricted to girls. Maternal cortisol during pregnancy is related to newborn amygdala architecture and connectivity in a sexually dimorphic manner. Given the fundamental role of the amygdala in the emergence of emotion regulation, these findings offer new insights into mechanisms linking maternal health with neuropsychiatric outcomes of children.

eLife digest

Stress during pregnancy, for example because of mental or physical disorders, can have long-term effects on child development. Epidemiological studies have shown that individuals exposed to stress in the womb are at higher risk of developmental and mood conditions, such as ADHD and depression. This effect is different between the sexes, and the biological mechanisms that underpin these observations are poorly understood.

One possibility is that a baby’s developing amygdala, the part of the brain that processes emotions, is affected by a signal known as cortisol. This hormone is best known for its role in coordinating the stress response, but it also directs the growth of a fetus. Tracking fetal amygdala changes as well as cortisol levels in the pregnant individual could explain how stress during pregnancy affects development.

To investigate, Stoye et al. recruited nearly 80 volunteers and their newborn children. MRI scans were used to examine the structure of the amygdala, and how it is connected to other parts of the brain. In parallel, the amount of cortisol was measured in hair samples collected from the volunteers around the time of birth, which reflects stress levels during the final three months of pregnancy.

Linking the brain imaging results to the volunteers’ cortisol levels showed that being exposed to higher cortisol levels in the womb affected babies in different ways based on their sex: boys showed alterations in the fine structure of their amygdala, while girls displayed changes in the way that brain region connected to other neural networks.

The work by Stoye et al. potentially reveals a biological mechanism by which early exposure to stress could affect brain development differently between the sexes, potentially informing real-world interventions.

Introduction

Prenatal exposure to maternal stress is estimated to affect 10–35% of children worldwide, which is a major concern because early life stress may contribute to impaired cognitive development, negative affectivity, autism spectrum disorder (ASD), and psychiatric diagnoses including attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), addiction, depression, and schizophrenia (Van den Bergh et al., 2017). Neural correlates of prenatally stressed children include alternations in brain structural and functional connectivity, especially in networks involving the amygdala and prefrontal cortex (Scheinost et al., 2017).

Adaptation of the maternal hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis is a key mechanism by which maternal stress modulates offspring neurodevelopment (Moisiadis and Matthews, 2014), and there is evidence that this mechanism operates in a sexually dimorphic manner (Sutherland and Brunwasser, 2018). For example, higher waking maternal salivary cortisol in pregnancy is associated with increased internalising behaviours in female infants and reduced internalising behaviours in males (Braithwaite et al., 2017a; Braithwaite et al., 2017b). Higher maternal salivary cortisol in pregnancy is also associated with stronger amygdala functional connectivity with networks involved in sensory processing and integration in newborn girls, with weaker connectivity to these brain regions in boys (Graham et al., 2019); and in childhood, with larger amygdalae (Buss et al., 2012) and reduced segregation of structural networks in girls but not boys (Kim et al., 2016). The amygdala is further implicated as a neural target of prenatal stress exposure by observations from studies that have characterised maternal stress by symptomatology of depression and/or anxiety, which report alterations in amygdala volume (Wen et al., 2017), microstructure (Rifkin-Graboi et al., 2013), and functional and structural connectivity among offspring (Posner et al., 2016).

Candidacy of the amygdala as an important neural target of prenatal stress exposure comes from the following observations in pre-clinical and clinical studies. First, the amygdala develops early in embryonic life (Humphrey, 1968) and contains a high concentration of glucocorticoid receptors (Wang et al., 2014); second, increased maternal glucocorticoids modulate amygdala development and anxiety-like behaviours in experimental models (Welberg et al., 2000; Barbazanges et al., 1996); third, lesion studies in non-human primates support its critical role in early development of emotion regulation (Schumann et al., 2011); fourth, newborn amygdala functional connectivity is consistently linked with internalising behaviours in children up to the age of 2 years (Graham et al., 2019; Rogers et al., 2017); fifth, early disruption to cell composition of the amygdala is reported in a model of early life stress (Kraszpulski et al., 2006), and in children with autism (Avino et al., 2018); and sixth, in pre-clinical models, stress and glucocorticoid exposure induce dendritic arborisation, amygdala hypertrophy and induce anxiety-like behaviours (Vyas et al., 2003; Mitra and Sapolsky, 2008).

Neonatal magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) serves as an intermediate phenotype for investigating the impact of early life exposures on brain and health because it is distal to the aetiological process, in this case prenatal stress, and is proximal to cognitive, behavioural and disease outcomes. Structural and diffusion MRI (dMRI) have been used to characterise brain structural maturation and emerging network connectivity during the perinatal period, and to investigate pathways to atypical development (Batalle et al., 2018; Boardman and Counsell, 2020). It is a suitable tool to investigate the impact of prenatal stress exposure on the amygdala because age-specific templates enable accurate parcellation of the amygdala and associated structures (Alexander et al., 2017); and diffusion tensor imaging and neurite orientation and dispersion density imaging (NODDI) support inference about tissue microstructure and network connectivity, modelled by fractional anisotropy (FA), mean diffusivity (MD), orientation dispersion index (ODI) and neurite density index (NDI) (Galdi et al., 2020; Zhang et al., 2012).

Hair cortisol concentration (HCC) measured in 3 cm hair samples collected from close to the scalp reflects basal HPA axis activity over the 3 months prior to sampling, and in contrast to single measures from saliva or blood, it is not influenced by short-term activation of the HPA axis in response to acute stressors (Staufenbiel et al., 2013). Studies in pregnant women have shown HCC to be an efficient method of retrospective assessment of long-term cortisol secretion, and thus long-term HPA axis activity (Kirschbaum et al., 2009; D'Anna-Hernandez et al., 2011).

Previous studies have reported sex-specific differences between maternal stress and amygdala functional connectivity and behavioural outcomes among children (Braithwaite et al., 2017a; Braithwaite et al., 2017b; Graham et al., 2019; Buss et al., 2012; Kim et al., 2016), but study designs leave uncertainty about the mechanism linking maternal stress with amygdala development, the potential confounding role of events and environmental exposures during childhood, and the impact of stress on structural connectivity. Resolving these uncertainties is necessary for developing strategies designed to improve socio-emotional development of children born to women who are stressed during pregnancy. Based on studies of the imaging, biochemical, and clinical phenotype of prenatal stress exposure, we hypothesised that higher levels of maternal HPA activity in the final months of pregnancy ascertained from maternal HCC would impact amygdala development and structural connectivity of offspring infants in a sexually-dimorphic manner, and that these effects would be apparent around the time of birth.

Results

Participant characteristics

The parents of 102 infants consented to take part. Of these, two preterm infants died before term equivalent age, 12 did not complete the MRI protocol or images were not amenable processing due to movement artefact; one had an incidental structural anomaly detected at MRI; and nine withdrew before MRI scan. This left data from 78 mother-infant dyads for analysis, the maternal and infant characteristics for whom are shown in Table 1. The preterm group (n = 36) had the following co-morbidities: early onset sepsis (n = 1); late onset sepsis (n = 5), necrotising enterocolitis (n = 3); histological chorioamnionitis (n = 14); mechanical ventilation during NICU stay (n = 16, median period of ventilation 2 days [IQR, 1 to 5 days]). Maternal hair was sampled at mean 3.5 ± 2.5 days after delivery, and the median HCC concentration was 5.6 pg/mg (0.5–107.1). Maternal HCC was not associated with gestational age (GA) at birth (r = 0.200, p=0.094). HCC did not differ between mothers of male and female infants (p=0.997). MRI was carried out at term-equivalent age: median 41.9 weeks’ GA (range 38.6–45.9).

Table 1
Maternal and neonatal characteristics.
Maternal characteristics, n = 71
Age (years)33.1 ± 5.2
BMI (kg/m2)25.2 ± 4.2
Primiparous (%)
Multiparous (%)
41 (58%)
30 (42%)
SIMD 2016 quintile n (%)*
1
2
3
4
5

4 (6%)
14 (20%)
10 (14%)
14 (20%)
29 (41%)
Tobacco smoked during pregnancy, n (%)
No tobacco smoked during pregnancy, n (%)
5 (7%)
66 (93%)
Gestational diabetes (%)
Preeclampsia (%)
Receiving pharmacological treatment for depression
1 (1%)
4 (6%)
3 (4%)
Infant characteristics, n = 78
Birthweight (g)2895 (454–4248)
Birth weight z-score0.2 ± 1.1
Birth gestation (weeks)38.4 (24.0–42.0)
Male n (%)
Female
44 (56%)
34 (44%)
European ancestry n (%)
Other
68 (87%)
10 (13%)
Singleton n (%)
Twin
63 (81%)
15 (19%)
  1. Normally distributed data is presented as mean ± SD. Non-normally distributed data is presented as median (range).

    *Scottish Index of Multiple Deprivation (SIMD) 2016 quintile. First quintile indicates most deprived and fifth quintile the least deprived.

  2. Calculated according to INTERGROWTH-21st standards.

Amygdala microstructure

In univariate analysis, there were moderately strong correlations between both FA and MD, and GA at birth and age at scan (r = 0.41–0.64), and weak correlations with birth weight z-score and Scottish Index of Multiple Deprivation 2016 (SIMD2016) quintile (r = 0.24–0.30). There were no significant correlations between FA and MD in amygdalae and ethnicity or infant sex, or maternal parity, age or BMI. There were moderate-to-strong correlations between NDI in the amygdalae and GA at birth and age at scan (r = 0.43–0.74), and a weak correlation with SIMD2016 quintile (r = 0.23–0.26). Weak-to-moderate correlations were observed between ODI in amygdalae with GA at birth and age at scan (r = 0.28–0.42), Supplementary file 1.

In multiple linear regression models, there was a significant interaction effect between maternal HCC and infant sex in left amygdala FA (p=0.010) and ODI (p=0.034), with higher maternal HCC being associated with higher left amygdala FA and lower ODI in girls compared to boys (Table 2, Figure 1). When we stratified by sex, there were associations between maternal HCC and infant amygdala microstructure in boys, but not girls. Table 3 shows that in boys, higher maternal HCC was associated with lower left amygdala FA (β = −0.339), lower right amygdala FA (β = −0.287) and NDI (β = −0.215), and higher right amygdala MD (β = 0.264) and ODI (β = 0.309), after FDR correction.

Partial regression plots of HCC and (A) left amygdala FA, (B) left amygdala ODI and (C) FA of connections between the right amygdala and putamen.

Plots are residualised for GA at birth, GA at scan, birthweight z-score and SIMD quintile. Boys are shown in blue, and girls in orange, along with the 95% confidence intervals.

Table 2
Associations of maternal hair cortisol concentration (HCC) and its interaction with infant sex on amygdala microstructure.
Model 1Model 2Model 3
Side testedImage metricR2R2HCC βHCC p-valueR2‘HCC x sex’ β‘HCC x sex’ p-value
LeftFA0.2670.269−0.0480.8580.3590.6770.010
MD0.4050.4050.0180.8580.413−0.1910.358
ODI0.1050.1110.0820.8580.181−0.5970.034
NDI0.5300.534−0.0640.8580.5610.3750.052
RightFA0.2890.308−0.1480.2690.3420.4150.083
MD0.4920.5230.1890.1430.530−0.1830.326
ODI0.2140.2280.1280.2690.283−0.5270.083
NDI0.5540.562−0.0940.2690.5830.3300.083
  1. Model 1: Age at MRI, gestational age at birth, birth weight z-score, Scottish Index of Multiple Deprivation 2016 quintile, infant sex. Model 2: Model 1 + (maternal HCC). Model 3: Model 2 + (maternal HCC x infant sex interaction). p-values are FDR adjusted. FA, fractional anisotropy; MD, mean diffusivity; ODI, orientation dispersion index; NDI, neurite density index; HCC, hair cortisol concentration, R2, coefficient of determination; β, standardised beta coefficient; p-value, FDR adjusted probability value.

Table 3
Associations of maternal hair cortisol concentration (HCC) with amygdala microstructural parameters assessed separately in boys and girls.
BoysGirls
Model 1Model 2Model 1Model 2
Side testedImage metricR2R2HCC βHCC p-valueR2R2HCC βHCC p-value
LeftFA0.4330.537−0.3390.0230.1570.2390.3400.372
MD0.4890.4970.0900.4620.4450.449−0.0720.667
ODI0.1240.2150.3170.0850.1320.159−0.1940.648
NDI0.6220.654−0.1890.0890.5220.5300.1090.648
RightFA0.3680.443−0.2870.0470.3010.3060.0910.736
MD0.5080.5710.2640.0470.4970.5060.1110.736
ODI0.1490.2360.3090.0470.3620.378−0.1490.736
NDI0.5810.623−0.2150.0470.5710.5730.0500.736
  1. Model 1: Gestational age at MRI, gestational age at birth, birth weight z-score, Scottish Index of Multiple Deprivation 2016 quintile. Model 2: Model 1 + (maternal HCC). p-values are FDR adjusted. FA, fractional anisotropy; MD, mean diffusivity; ODI, orientation dispersion index; NDI, neurite density index; HCC, hair cortisol concentration; R2, coefficient of determination; β, standardised beta coefficient; p-value, FDR adjusted probability value.

Structural connectivity of the amygdala

For both hemispheres, the networks with the top 20% number of streamlines were connected to eight structures: thalamus, putamen, insula, superior temporal gyrus, inferior temporal gyrus, middle temporal gyrus, caudate, and lateral orbitofrontal cortex (Figure 2 and Figure 2—figure supplement 1). Quantification of streamline counts is given in Supplementary file 2 and illustrated in Figure 3. Maternal HCC was not associated with streamline counts of the left and right amygdala with these regions.

Figure 2 with 1 supplement see all
Segmentations of the amygdalae and connected regions defined by the top 20% streamline counts.

Figure 1a shows the lateral view of the sagittal plane and 1b the medial view. The same eight regions had the highest streamline counts to the amygdalae bilaterally.

Chord diagram of the streamline counts between the amygdalae and unilateral regions of interest (ROIs).

The number of streamlines between ROIs are demonstrated by the corresponding arcs thickness. ROIs connected by the top 20% of streamlines are shown in green.

In fully adjusted analyses, the interaction between maternal HCC and infant sex was significant for mean FA of connections between the right amygdala and putamen. Higher maternal HCC was associated with higher FA for amygdala-putamen connectivity in girls compared with boys (p=0.007) (Figure 1). The interaction was also seen for connections to left thalamus, putamen, and insula, but the interaction term did not remain after correction for multiple tests (Supplementary file 3). In sex-stratified analysis, girls had higher FA values in association with high maternal HCC in connections between left amygdala with thalamus, putamen, and inferior temporal gyrus, and the right amygdala with putamen and inferior temporal gyrus, but these were not significant after correction for multiple tests (Supplementary file 3a).

Amygdala volume

Mean volumes of the left and right amygdala were 877 ± 111 mm3 and 823 ± 91 mm3, respectively. In univariate analysis, there were weak associations (r = 0.24–0.3) between amygdala volume and GA at birth and birth weight z-score, but not with age at scan, SIMD2016 quintile, sex, ethnicity, or maternal BMI, parity or age (Supplementary file 1). Maternal HCC was not associated with infant right or left amygdala volume in regression models adjusted for potential covariates, and interaction terms between maternal HCC and infant sex were not significant (Supplementary file 3b).

Sensitivity and subgroup analyses

There were seven twin sets in the whole sample. When we repeated analyses including only singletons and the first born of twin pairs, significant associations between maternal HCC, sex and image feature remained, with little change to the value of regression coefficients (Supplementary file 3c).

In subgroup analysis of preterm and term infants, the direction and magnitude of interaction effects for both groups were similar to those of the whole sample. Specifically, when tested in term and preterm infants, respectively, higher maternal hair cortisol concentration was associated with higher left amygdala fractional anisotropy (β = 0.735 and 0.640), lower left amygdala orientation dispersion index (β = −0.710 and −0.614), and higher fractional anisotropy in connections between the right amygdala and putamen (β = 0.733 and 0.426) in girls compared to boys (Supplementary file 3c).

Bronchopulmonary dysplasia, sepsis, and histological chorioamnionitis did not correlate with any image feature tested. The need for mechanical ventilation did correlate with lower FA in the right amygdala and its connections to putamen, but it had little influence on the interactive effect of sex and maternal cortisol on amygdala microstructure or connectivity (Supplementary file 3d).

Discussion

We report a mechanism that could explain the impact of maternal stress on infant brain development. We found that maternal HCC, a stable marker of chronic maternal HPA axis activity in pregnancy, is associated with microstructure and structural connectivity of the newborn amygdala, a region of functional importance for early social development and emotion regulation. Specifically, HCC interacts with infant sex to modify amygdala FA, ODI, and NDI, which supports the inference that maternal chronic HPA activity has an impact on dendritic structure, axonal configuration, and the packing density of neurites, in a sexually dimorphic manner (Jespersen et al., 2012; Grussu et al., 2017; Sato et al., 2017; Nazeri et al., 2020).

The findings are consistent with recent reports from the GUSTO (Growing Up in Singapore Towards Health Outcomes) cohort that describe associations between maternal depressive symptoms and alterations in offspring amygdala development (Wen et al., 2017; Rifkin-Graboi et al., 2013). That study highlighted the role of maternal mental health on newborn brain development, and focussed attention on the amygdala. Here, we provide mechanistic insights into the relationship between maternal health and wellbeing and amygdala development by using maternal HCC to characterise chronic HPA activity, and the NODDI model for inference about tissue microstructure. We chose to measure NODDI parameters for assessing microstructure because ODI and NDI in grey matter appear to be functionally tractable. For example, diffusion markers of dendritic density and arborisation in grey matter predict differences in intelligence (Genç et al., 2018), reduced ODI in grey matter is reported in psychosis and in neurodegenerative disease, and reduced grey matter NDI is reported in Parkinson’s disease, Alzheimer’s disease, autism spectrum disorder, and temporal lobe epilepsy (for review see Nazeri et al., 2020).

Maternal HCC was also related to structural connectivity of the amygdala in a sex-discordant manner. Higher maternal HCC was associated with higher FA in girls than boys in tracts between right amygdala and putamen. These observations were not explained by differences in streamline counts in relation to maternal HCC. Furthermore, in sex-stratified analysis, there were consistent trends for girls born to women with higher HCC to have higher mean FA between the left amygdala and left thalamus, putamen and inferior temporal gyrus, and between right amygdala and right putamen and right inferior temporal gyrus, although these did not survive FDR correction. During the neonatal period, higher FA in white matter tracts is typically taken to imply microstructural maturation, through increased axon diameter, density, or myelination. Therefore, increased mean FA demonstrated in connections between the amygdala and putamen, in girls exposed to higher cortisol, could be interpreted as increased maturation of these connections.

Several plausible biological mechanisms could underlie sex differences in associations between HCC and fetal neurodevelopment. First, the capacity of the placenta to regulate the passage of cortisol from mother to fetus differs according to fetal sex, evidenced by sex-discordant expression of enzymes controlling glucocorticoid metabolism within the placenta (Carpenter et al., 2017; Meakin et al., 2017). Second, evidence from gene expression studies indicate that fetal sex influences the direct actions of cortisol in the placenta (Rosenfeld, 2015). Third, the same level of cortisol exposure likely holds sex-different actions at the level of the fetal brain, given that male and female fetuses have different glucocorticoid and mineralocorticoid expression across development (Owen and Matthews, 2003). Finally, there may be sex differences in placental release of corticotropin-releasing hormone (CRH), which influence fetal cortisol exposure and neurodevelopment (Bangasser and Wiersielis, 2018; Sandman et al., 2013).

To our knowledge, this is the first study to investigate a physiological measure of chronic maternal HPA activity with quantitative biomarkers of brain development, and to include infants born very preterm. The relationships we describe appear to apply across the whole GA range because GA at birth was included as a covariate in regression models, and in sub-group analyses the magnitude and direction of ‘HCC x sex’ interaction effects existed in term and preterm groups. This suggests that maternal health and wellbeing across gestation influences neurodevelopment in preterm and term infants. This is important because preterm birth has previously been associated with both exposure to maternal HPA axis dysregulation (Duthie and Reynolds, 2013), and an increased risk of inattention and affective disorders (Johnson, 2007). Furthermore, the finding that maternal HCC is associated with neonatal amygdala development in preterm and term infants is consistent with the observation that maternal cortisol in early gestation predicts amygdala volume at the age of 7 years (Buss et al., 2012). Given that mothers’ HPA axis shows considerable trait-stability across pregnancy, whereby women with higher cortisol concentrations in the second trimester also tend to have higher cortisol in the third trimester (Graham et al., 2019; Stoye et al., 2020), the data lend further support to strategies designed to optimise pre- and early pregnancy health for optimising fetal neurodevelopment.

Strengths of this study are the use of biophysical tissue modelling (NODDI) to enable inference about neurite density and organisation in the amygdala; and use of a data-driven approach to investigate amygdala structural connectivity. A second strength is use of maternal HCC to operationalise stress because it is a quantitative stable marker of cortisol secretion that represents HPA activity over 3 months; as such HCC is unlikely to reflect transient stresses that can occur in pregnancy, and it overcomes the problems of diurnal variation that occur with plasma and saliva measurements.

The study has some limitations: first it was not powered to detect both sex and birth gestation interactions, but this should be considered in future study design. Second, follow-up studies that include measures of socio-emotional development are needed to understand functional consequences of these findings. Finally, the newborn amygdalae are relatively small anatomical regions so could be susceptible to partial volume effects influencing microstructural characteristics. To mitigate this risk, we used an age-specific atlas for segmentation, and excluded voxels with a υiso < 0.5. Third, due to the close proximity of the amygdala and putamen the corresponding connectivity FA may to some degree reflect microstructure of the regions themselves.

Longitudinal studies that evaluate socio-emotional development are needed to understand the functional consequences of these findings. In addition, as the network architecture underlying socio-emotional function in early life becomes more certain with technological advances, it may be useful to focus image analyses on individual networks. The subcortical-gaze pathway is of specific interest because in adults there is evidence that structural connectivity of the superior colliculus and amygdala is related to processing of facial expressions (McFadyen, 2019), which is one of the earliest social cognitive abilities to develop and is foundational to other trajectories of cognitive development.

In conclusion, dMRI and HCC were used to investigate mechanisms underlying the transmission of prenatal stressors on infant development. Maternal HCC in pregnancy is associated with newborn amygdala microstructure and structural connectivity, in a sex-dimorphic manner. These findings reveal that the amygdala, a structure of known importance for child development, is susceptible to variations in the prenatal stress environment, and that cortisol imparts sex-specific effects on human fetal neurodevelopment.

Materials and methods

Key resources table
Reagent type
(species) or
resource
DesignationSource or
reference
IdentifiersAdditional
information
Software, algorithmMarchenko-
Pastur-PCA-based algorithm
PMID:27523449See Materials and methods, section Image Pre-processing
Software, algorithmMinimal processing
pipeline of the developing human connectome project (dHCP)
PMID:29409960See Materials and methods, section Image Pre-processing
Software, algorithmEddy current, head movement and EPI geometric distortions correction with outlier replacement and slice-to-volume registrationDW-MRI registration in FSLRRID:SCR_009461See Materials and methods, section Image Pre-processing
Software, algorithmBias field inhomogeneity correctionPMID:20378467See Materials and methods, section Image Pre-processing
Software, algorithmBoundary-based registrationPMID:19573611See Materials and methods, section Image Pre-processing
Software, algorithmAffine and symmetric normalisation (SyN) part of ANTS - Advanced Normalisation ToolSPMID:17659998RRID:SCR_004757105105105See Materials and methods, section Tissue Segmentation and Parcellation
Software, algorithmJoint label fusionPMID:22732662See Materials and methods, section Tissue Segmentation and Parcellation
Software, algorithmNODDINODDI Matlab ToolboxRRID:SCR_006826See Materials and methods, section Microstructure and volumetric assessments
Software, algorithmConstrained spherical deconvolution (CSD); MRtrixPMID:31473352RRID:SCR_006971See Materials and methods, section Network Construction Analysis
Software, algorithm• Anatomically-constrained tractography (ACT)PMID:22705374See Materials and methods, section Network Construction Analysis
Software, algorithmFiber orientation distribution (FOD)PMID:25109526See Materials and methods, section Network Construction Analysis
Software, algorithmSpherical-deconvolution informed filtering of tractograms twoPMID:26163802See Materials and methods, section Network Construction Analysis
Software, algorithmSPSSSPSSRRID:SCR_002865
Software, algorithmSeabornPython data visualisation library https://seaborn.pydata.org/ http://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.883859RRID:SCR_018132Figure 1
Software, algorithmParaViewParaViewRRID:SCR_002516Figure 2
Software, algorithmITK-SNAPITK-SNAPRRID:SCR_002010Figure 2
Software, algorithmCircosCircosRRID:SCR_011798Figure 3
OtherLiquid chromatography-tandem mass spectrometry (LC-MS/MS)PMID:23584040See Materials and methods, section Maternal hair cortisol concentrations (HCC)

Participants

The ‘Stress Response Systems in Mothers and Infants’ cohort recruited mother-infant dyads from the Royal Infirmary, Edinburgh, between March 2018 and August 2019. It prospectively tests associations of perinatal glucocorticoid exposure with brain development, and early life exposures including preterm birth with infant HPA axis regulation. We recruited mother-infant dyads who presented to NHS services with threatened preterm labour and delivered at ≤32 completed weeks of gestation, and women who delivered ≥37 weeks’ gestation and received routine care with their infant on postnatal wards. No participants were recruited from high-risk antenatal clinics. Exclusion criteria were congenital fetal abnormality, chromosomal abnormality or regular maternal corticosteroid use. All women gave written informed consent. Ethical approval was granted by South East Scotland 01 Regional Ethics Committee (18/SS/0006).

Maternal hair cortisol concentrations (HCC)

Request a detailed protocol

Maternal hair was sampled within 10 days of delivery. Hair was cut close to the scalp, at the posterior vertex, and stored in aluminium foil at −20°C. The proximal 3 cm of hair were analysed by liquid chromatography-tandem mass spectrometry (LC-MS/MS), at Dresden Lab Service GmbH (Dresden, Germany), using an established protocol (Gao et al., 2013). Adult hair commonly grows at 1 cm/month (Wennig, 2000) and thus hair segments represented maternal HPA axis activity over the last 3 months of pregnancy.

Demographic and clinical information

Request a detailed protocol

Participant demographic information was collected through maternal questionnaire and review of medical records. Collected maternal information included: age at delivery (years), parity (primiparous/multiparous), clinical diagnosis of gestational diabetes, pre-eclampsia, pharmacological treatment for depression during pregnancy, antenatal corticosteroid exposure for threatened preterm birth; body mass index (BMI) calculated at antenatal booking; smoking status defined as having smoked any tobacco in pregnancy; SIMD 2016 quintile rank, a score generated by the Scottish government which measures localities’ deprivation according to local income, employment, health, education, geographic access to services, crime and housing. Infant demographics included whether participants were a singleton or twin, ethnicity, GA at birth (weeks), and birth weight z-score calculated according to intergrowth standards (Villar et al., 2014).

Magnetic resonance imaging

Image acquisition

Request a detailed protocol

Infants underwent MRI at term-equivalent age, at the Edinburgh Imaging Facility, RIE. Infants were fed, wrapped and allowed to sleep naturally in the scanner. Flexible earplugs and neonatal earmuffs (MiniMuffs, Natus) were used for acoustic protection. Scans were supervised by a doctor, or nurse trained in neonatal resuscitation.

A Siemens MAGNETOM Prisma 3 T MRI clinical scanner (Siemens Healthcare Erlangen, Germany) and 16-channel phased-array paediatric head and neck coil were used for acquisition (Boardman et al., 2020). In brief, we acquired 3D T1-weighted MPRAGE (T1w) (acquired voxel size = 1 mm isotropic) with TI 1100 ms, TE 4.69 ms and TR 1970 ms; 3D T2-weighted SPACE (T2w) (voxel size = 1 mm isotropic) with TE 409 ms and TR 3200 ms; and axial dMRI. dMRI was acquired in two separate acquisitions to reduce the time needed to re-acquire any data lost to motion artefact: the first acquisition consisted of 8 baseline volumes (b = 0 s/mm2 [b0]) and 64 volumes with b = 750 s/mm2, the second consisted of 8 b0, three volumes with b = 200 s/mm2, six volumes with b = 500 s/mm2 and 64 volumes with b = 2500 s/mm2; an optimal angular coverage for the sampling scheme was applied (Caruyer et al., 2013). In addition, an acquisition of 3 b0 volumes with an inverse phase encoding direction was performed. All dMRI images were acquired using single-shot spin-echo echo planar imaging (EPI) with 2-fold simultaneous multislice and twofold in-plane parallel imaging acceleration and 2 mm isotropic voxels; except where stated above, all three diffusion acquisitions had the same parameters (TR/TE 3400/78.0 ms).

Conventional images were reported by an experienced paediatric radiologist (A.J.Q.) using a structured system (Woodward et al., 2006). Images with focal parenchymal injury (defined as post-haemorrhagic ventricular dilatation, porencephalic cyst or cystic periventricular leukomalacia) were not included in the final sample.

Image pre-processing

Request a detailed protocol

Diffusion MRI processing was performed as follows: for each subject the two dMRI acquisitions were first concatenated and then denoised using a Marchenko-Pastur-PCA-based algorithm (Veraart et al., 2016); the eddy current, head movement and EPI geometric distortions were corrected using outlier replacement and slice-to-volume registration (Smith et al., 2004; Andersson et al., 2003; Andersson and Sotiropoulos, 2016; Andersson et al., 2017); bias field inhomogeneity correction was performed by calculating the bias field of the mean b0 volume and applying the correction to all the volumes (Tustison et al., 2010).

The T2w images were processed using the minimal processing pipeline of the developing human connectome project (dHCP) to obtain the bias field corrected T2w, the brain masks and the different tissue probability maps (Makropoulos et al., 2018). The mean b0 EPI volume of each subject was co-registered to their structural T2w volume using boundary-based registration (Greve and Fischl, 2009).

Tissue segmentation and parcellation

Request a detailed protocol

The 10 manually labelled subjects of the M-CRIB atlas (Alexander et al., 2017) were registered to the bias field corrected T2w using rigid, affine and symmetric normalisation (SyN) (Avants et al., 2008). Next, the registered labels of the 10 atlases were merged using joint label fusion (Wang et al., 2013), resulting in a parcellation containing 84 regions of interest (ROIs).

Microstructure and volumetric assessments

Request a detailed protocol

Volumes were calculated from ROIs derived in the structural images. ROIs were propagated to the diffusion native space using the previously computed transformation.

To calculate the tensor derived metric, only the first shell was used. NODDI metrics were calculated using the recommended values for neonatal grey-matter of the parallel intrinsic diffusivity (1.25 μm2·ms−1) (Zhang et al., 2012; Guerrero et al., 2019). The obtained metrics are: neurite density index (NDI), isotropic volume fraction (υiso) and orientation dispersion index (ODI). The mean FA, MD, ODI, and NDI were calculated for the left and right amygdalae M-CRIB ROIs, after exclusion of voxels with a υiso < 0.5. Voxels with a υiso < 0.5 were excluded, in order to minimise partial volume effects (Batalle et al., 2019).

Network construction and analysis

Request a detailed protocol

Tractography was performed using constrained spherical deconvolution (CSD) and anatomically constrained tractography (Tournier et al., 2019; Smith et al., 2012) The required 5-tissue type file, was generated by combining the tissue probability maps obtained from the dHCP pipeline with the subcortical structures derived from the parcellation process. Multi-tissue response function was calculated, with a FA threshold of 0.1. The average response functions were calculated. Then, the multi-tissue fiber orientation distribution (FOD) was calculated (Jeurissen et al., 2014), and global intensity normalisation on the FODs images was performed. Finally, the tractogram was created, generating 10 million streamlines, with a minimum length of 20 mm and a maximum of 200 mm and a cut-off of 0.05 (default), using backtrack and a dynamic seeding (Smith et al., 2015a). To be able to quantitatively assess connectivity, spherical-deconvolution informed filtering of tractograms two (SIFT2) was applied to the resulting tractograms (Smith et al., 2015a). The connectivity matrix was constructed using a robust approach, a 2 mm radial search at the end of the streamline was performed to allow the tracts to reach the GM parcellation (Smith et al., 2015b). The final connectivity matrices were multiplied by the μ coefficient obtained during the SIFT2 process.

These connectomes gave a quantification of the SIFT2 weights (referred to as the streamline counts), and the mean FA of connections, between both the left and right amygdala to 41 unilateral regions of interest defined through M-CRIB parcellation. In order to focus analysis on to amygdala’s most structurally connected areas, these 82 ROIs were thresholded according to the number of streamlines connecting them to the left or right amygdala, with the top 20% (N = 16) of connections taken forward for further analysis testing relationships with maternal HCC.

Statistical analysis

Request a detailed protocol

Analyses were performed using IBM SPSS Statistics Version 25 Armonk, NY: IBM Corp. Continuous data are summarised as mean ± SD if they had a normal distribution, and median (range) if skewed. Maternal HCC was positively skewed, and log-10 transformed for analysis. The relationship between maternal HCC with infant characteristics was tested using independent t-test and Pearson’s correlation for categorical and continuous variables, respectively. Associations between maternal HCC with (i) left and right amygdala microstructure (FA, MD, NDI, ODI), (ii) structural connectivity (number of streamlines and mean FA of connections), (iii) amygdalae volumes were tested using multiple linear regression. In all models, image feature was the dependent variable and maternal HCC was an independent variable. Covariates included infant sex and clinical or demographic factors that were correlated with either left or right amygdala microstructure or volume using Pearson’s correlation. Associations with the following were tested: GA at birth, age at scan, birth weight z-score, SIMD2016 quintile, infant ethnicity, infant sex, and maternal parity, BMI and age. Antenatal corticosteroid treatment for threatened preterm birth was not included as a covariate because it was given to n = 36 (100%) women in the preterm group, was highly correlated with GA at birth (r = 0.958, p<0.001), so its inclusion as a covariate would have introduced multicollinearity in regression analysis. For descriptive purposes, correlations of infant and maternal factors considered as potential covariates are described as weak if r < 0.3, moderate if r = 0.3–0.7, and strong if r > 0.7.

Sex differences in the relationship between maternal HCC and newborn imaging features were assessed by adding an interaction term between maternal HCC and infant sex in the whole group regression model. If a significant interaction was present, sex stratified analysis was conducted independently in boys and girls. Benjamini and Hochberg false discovery rate (FDR) correction was used to adjust p-values for multiple testing. FDR corrections were conducted separately for assessments of left amygdala microstructure (n = 4), right amygdala microstructure (n = 4), left amygdala connectivity (n = 8) and right amygdala connectivity (n = 8).

One sensitivity analysis was carried out to assess whether associations between maternal HCC and image features might be enhanced by inclusion of twins. We repeated analysis of features with a significant ‘HCC x sex’ interaction in the whole sample, using only singleton pregnancies and the first-born infant of twin pairs. One sub-group analysis of preterm (GA at birth ≤32 weeks) and term infants (GA at birth ≥37 weeks) was carried out because the relationship between maternal HCC and infant brain development may be gestation specific.

Data availability

All data generated or analysed during this study are included in the manuscript and supporting files.

References

Decision letter

  1. Tamar R Makin
    Senior Editor; University College London, United Kingdom
  2. Jonathan O'Muicheartaigh
    Reviewing Editor; King’s College London, United Kingdom
  3. Jonathan O'Muicheartaigh
    Reviewer; King’s College London, United Kingdom

In the interests of transparency, eLife publishes the most substantive revision requests and the accompanying author responses.

Acceptance summary:

The work demonstrates that sex-specific differences in the influence of maternal cortisol, previously shown in childhood and adolescence, are present in the neonatal brain. The use of term age neonates allows the exclusion of the confound of childhood experience and exposure, seen in studies looking at older children. This study can therefore specifically implicate prenatal maternal cortisol exposure as an influence on this early maturing and behaviourally important structure.

Decision letter after peer review:

Thank you for submitting your article "Maternal cortisol is associated with neonatal amygdala microstructure and connectivity in a sexually dimorphic manner" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by three peer reviewers, including Jonathan O’Muicheartaigh as the Reviewing Editor and Reviewer #1, and the evaluation has been overseen by Tamar Makin as the Senior Editor.

The reviewers have discussed the reviews with one another and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this decision to help you prepare a revised submission.

As the editors have judged that your manuscript is of interest, but that, as described below, revisions are required before it is published. We would like to draw your attention to changes in our revision policy that we have made in response to COVID-19 (https://elifesciences.org/articles/57162). First, because many researchers have temporarily lost access to the labs, we will give authors as much time as they need to submit revised manuscripts. We are also offering, if you choose, to post the manuscript to bioRxiv (if it is not already there) along with this decision letter and a formal designation that the manuscript is "in revision at eLife". Please let us know if you would like to pursue this option. (If your work is more suitable for medRxiv, you will need to post the preprint yourself, as the mechanisms for us to do so are still in development.)

Summary:

This study investigates the relationship between maternal cortisol levels (measured from hair) and infant amygdala microstructure in a cohort of 78 mother-infant dyads. The neonates were a mix of those born at term (n=42) and those born prematurely (n=36) but all were scanned at term equivalent age. The authors demonstrate sex-stratified relationships, with a strong relationship between cortisol and amygdala microstructure in males and a relationship between amygdala and other temporal/subcortical regions in females.

The reviewers agreed that the manuscript is both interesting and timely. The imaging methods are well performed. However, we shared a series of concerns that should be addressed before we can consider publication.

Essential revisions:

Sample:

– The incidence of preterm birth is ~10%, while in this cohort the incidence is much higher. Were the women recruited from a high-risk pregnancy clinic (which may be a high stress population) or do the gestational ages largely reflect twins included in the sample?

– All infants were imaged at term for this protocol. However, NICU-related procedures occurring between birth and scan (primarily days of mechanical ventilation and infection) are associated with alterations subcortical development in preterms. Was the preterm cohort a critically-ill cohort? Were these clinical variables available?

– Maternal education is an important predictor of brain development and outcome. Had the authors considered to adjust for this variable in their analyses, particularly for the volumetric analyses?

Stress and cortisol:

– The last line of the Abstract implicates that the amygdala cortisol relationship gives an insight into the relationship between maternal stress and child outcome. Cortisol levels are shown here to covary with microstructure, but do they reflect actual maternal stress in pregnancy in this sample? Are there any maternal stress (anxiety or depression) questionnaires that could be reported to address this?

Sex Divergence:

– It's not just one sex that is affected but rather sex-specific effects dependent on the outcome examined (amygdala microstructure in male babies and microstructure of connecting white matter from the amygdala in female babies). The Abstract doesn't describe this divergence, focusing on results only with respect to female neonates but actually the interaction effects both sexes in different ways/regions. It would also be very helpful to have plots to illustrates the relationships (residualised for covariates).

Interpretation of measures:

– The major conclusions concerning HCC, the interaction with infant biological sex and the diffusion measures revealing evidence for alterations in "dendritic structure, axonal configuration, and the packing density of neurites…" aren't entirely supported by the findings based on the results from volumetric analyses. The alterations in ODI seen with HCC should be paralleled by changes in the volume data.

Given that brain volume differences are also believed to underlie alterations in dendritic structure, the authors' conclusions wouldn't entirely be supported. Modifying the central claims would be recommended but more data aren't required to support the findings.

– The lack of a significant association between macrostructural changes in the amygdala and HCC was surprising. This is in consideration of previous work in the area in the GUSTO cohort, which focused primarily on amygdalar volumes. The discussion of the results related to the volume data should be expanded upon.

– How inter-related were the measures – e.g. is there a negative association between amygdala microstructure and amygdala connections that could explain the split in sex associations and directionality.

Discussion:

– What potential biological mechanism do the authors propose underlies these sex specific results. There are only really two vague sentences on this, but the complex results need more.

– The authors present an extremely comprehensive overview of the connectivity of the amygdala. Given the authors' conclusions regarding future social cognition assessments, it's surprising to see that the subcortical gaze pathway was not examined (amygdala, thalamus, superior colliculus) as this pathway rapidly processing eye/face processing. Examining connectivity with midbrain structures might be infeasible in the neonatal brain; however, including a discussion of this pathway would be useful for future research in the area.

– For the HCC relationships with microstructure, maternal HCC values for the preterm infants reflects exposure during (coarsely) a different trimester to the term-born infants. The subgroup analyses (term/preterm) indicate that the results are largely independent of this so does this implicate that the influences on amygdala development are from early gestation?

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.60729.sa1

Author response

Essential revisions:

Sample:

– The incidence of preterm birth is ~10%, while in this cohort the incidence is much higher. Were the women recruited from a high-risk pregnancy clinic (which may be a high stress population) or do the gestational ages largely reflect twins included in the sample?

The incidence of preterm birth is higher in the study group compared of the general population because the study is designed to investigate the impact of perinatal glucocorticoid exposure on brain development, and perinatal factors (including preterm birth) on infant HPA axis regulation. As such, we recruited term and preterm mother-infant dyads. Preterm dyads were recruited when mothers presented with threatened preterm labour (or immediately after birth if preterm delivery happened without time for antenatal informed consent). The analysis included infants born at term in order to understand generalisability of findings, and because existing literature suggests maternal HPA activity impacts fetal development across the whole of pregnancy. No participating woman was recruited from a high risk or multiple pregnancy clinic.

We have added the following clarification to recruitment section of the Materials and methods:

“We recruited mother-infant dyads who presented to NHS services with threatened preterm labour and delivered at ≤32 completed weeks of gestation, and women who delivered ≥37 weeks’ gestation and received routine care with their infant on postnatal wards. No participants were recruited from high risk antenatal clinics.”

– All infants were imaged at term for this protocol. However, NICU-related procedures occurring between birth and scan (primarily days of mechanical ventilation and infection) are associated with alterations subcortical development in preterms. Was the preterm cohort a critically-ill cohort? Were these clinical variables available?

Thank you. We agree that co-morbidities of preterm birth can affect brain development that is manifest on MRI at term equivalent age. Within the preterm group (n=36) there were the following co-morbidities: early onset sepsis (n=1); late onset sepsis (n=5), necrotizing enterocolitis (n=3); histological chorioamnionitis (n=14); mechanical ventilation during NICU stay (n=16, of which the median period of ventilation was 2 days [IQR 1 to 5 days]).

To investigate whether postnatal co-morbidities influenced associations between maternal cortisol and image metrics we have taken the following steps. First, we tested for univariate associations between the perinatal exposure and image metrics. Where significant univariate associations were identified, we performed additional sensitivity analyses by including the relevant perinatal variable in regression models testing associations between maternal cortisol and image metrics.

Univariate analyses are shown in Author response table 1. Bronchopulmonary dysplasia (BPD), chorioamnionitis and late onset sepsis were not associated with amygdala volume, microstructure or the FA of connections to the putamen. However, infants who received mechanical ventilation (coded as any vs. no ventilation) did have lower right amygdala FA (r=-0.332*, p=0.048) and lower FA in connections with the putamen (r=-0.339**, p=0.043).

Author response table 1: Pearson’s correlations between perinatal exposures and amygdala volume, microstructure and connectivity

Therefore, we included mechanical ventilation as a variable in regression models and found it had little influence on the interactive effect of sex and maternal cortisol on left amygdala FA (β=0.660, previously β=0.677), left amygdala ODI (β=-0.584, previously β=-0.597) and the FA of connections between the right amygdala and putamen (β=0.590, β=0.583).

Since perinatal co-morbidities either did not hold univariate associations with amygdala volume, microstructure or connectivity, or change reported associations between HCC and image metrics, we have not amended the analysis reported in the main manuscript but we have added the following lines to the Results section and we have included the analysis detailed above in Supplementary file 3D:

“The preterm group (n=36) had the following co-morbidities: early onset sepsis (n=1); late onset sepsis (n=5), necrotizing enterocolitis (n=3); histological chorioamnionitis (n=14); mechanical ventilation during NICU stay (n=16, median period of ventilation 2 days [IQR, 1 to 5 days]).”

“Bronchopulmonary dysplasia, sepsis, and histological chorioamnionitis did not correlate with any image feature tested. The need for mechanical ventilation correlated with lower FA in the right amygdala and its connections to putamen, but it had little influence on the interactive effect of sex and maternal cortisol on amygdala microstructure or connectivity (Supplementary file 3D).”

– Maternal education is an important predictor of brain development and outcome. Had the authors considered to adjust for this variable in their analyses, particularly for the volumetric analyses?

Maternal education (binarized as university or college educated (yes [56], no [20]) was not associated with left amygdala (r -0.49) or right amygdala (r -0.148) volume.

Stress and cortisol:

– The last line of the Abstract implicates that the amygdala cortisol relationship gives an insight into the relationship between maternal stress and child outcome. Cortisol levels are shown here to covary with microstructure, but do they reflect actual maternal stress in pregnancy in this sample? Are there any maternal stress (anxiety or depression) questionnaires that could be reported to address this?

The literature linking maternal cortisol with psychological stress/psychiatric disease (‘stress’) in pregnancy does not report a consistent relationship between the two. Our own analysis of the ABCD cohort (Reynolds RM), showed that maternal cortisol is mainly associated with biological and lifestyle factors, and not with psychosocial factors (Blekker et al. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2017).

We have clarified this point of terminology by changing the term ‘…stress…’ to ‘…health…’ in the Abstract.

Sex Divergence:

– It's not just one sex that is affected but rather sex-specific effects dependent on the outcome examined (amygdala microstructure in male babies and microstructure of connecting white matter from the amygdala in female babies). The Abstract doesn't describe this divergence, focusing on results only with respect to female neonates but actually the interaction effects both sexes in different ways/regions. It would also be very helpful to have plots to illustrates the relationships (residualised for covariates).

Thank you for these helpful suggestions. The following sentences have been added to the Abstract:

“We found a relationship between maternal HCC and amygdala development that differed according to infant sex.”

“…Furthermore, altered amygdala microstructure was only observed in boys, with connectivity changes restricted to girls.”

Residualised plots showing sex-discordant associations between HCC, and left amygdala FA, left amygdala ODI and the FA of connections between the right amygdala and right putamen have been added to the manuscript, as requested (Figure 1).

Interpretation of measures:

– The major conclusions concerning HCC, the interaction with infant biological sex and the diffusion measures revealing evidence for alterations in "dendritic structure, axonal configuration, and the packing density of neurites…" aren't entirely supported by the findings based on the results from volumetric analyses. The alterations in ODI seen with HCC should be paralleled by changes in the volume data.

Given that brain volume differences are also believed to underlie alterations in dendritic structure, the authors' conclusions wouldn't entirely be supported. Modifying the central claims would be recommended but more data aren't required to support the findings.

The microstructural measures we used (NDI, ODI, FA and MD) are generally considered to support inference about dendritic structure, axonal configuration, and the packing density of neurites (see citations in manuscript). In the neonatal period, it is very common for microstructural alternations to be present without volumetric correlates, and in adulthood, alterations in dMRI parameters in the contexts of disease and cognition and are not necessarily accompanied by changes in macrostructure (Nazari et al., 2020). Therefore, we think there are insufficient data in the literature to be certain that changes in dMRI parameters, including ODI, should necessarily be paralleled by changes in volume.

Further considerations are: first, the neonatal brain has a higher water content than at other times in life, and this is likely to influence the relationship between microstructure and macrostructure; and second, it is plausible that maternal factors influence the developmental trajectory of the amygdala (Tottenham and Sheridan, 2010) by affecting microstructure in the newborn period, which might in turn affect network connectivity through development, manifesting as volumetric alterations in later childhood. Please also see the next response, which is related to this point.

– The lack of a significant association between macrostructural changes in the amygdala and HCC was surprising. This is in consideration of previous work in the area in the GUSTO cohort, which focused primarily on amygdalar volumes. The discussion of the results related to the volume data should be expanded upon.

The publications arising from the GUSTO cohort that are directly relevant to our study are listed below. There are consistencies between our study and the GUSTO studies when timing of neuroimaging is taken into account. Associations between maternal depressive symptoms and amygdala volume in the GUSTO cohort were found when MRI was conducted in childhood (Wen et al., 2017). However, when the relationship was assessed in the newborn period, maternal depression was associated with amygdala FA, but not with amygdala volume (Rifkin-Graboi et al., 2013). Furthermore, the GUSTO cohort provides supportive evidence that volumetric deficits in sub-cortical structures (hippocampus) in the context of maternal depression may not be apparent at birth but emerge over time (Qiu et al., 2013).

Although we do not see contradictions between the results reported in this manuscript and those of the GUSTO study when timing of imaging is considered, we question the expectation that the two studies should necessarily yield similar results because there are major differences between study designs. The GUSTO investigators used maternal depressive symptoms as the predictor variable in the analyses, whereas we used maternal cortisol; as noted in the response to the point ‘Stress and cortisol’, this is determined by biological and lifestyle factors and is only inconsistently correlated with mental health problems in pregnant women.

– How inter-related were the measures – e.g. is there a negative association between amygdala microstructure and amygdala connections that could explain the split in sex associations and directionality.

Measures of microstructure and connectivity are correlated with varying degrees of strength (see Author response image 1). It can be seen that correlations between microstructural measures are broadly similar in boys and girls.

Author response image 1
Pearson’s correlations between amygdala microstructural and the putamen to amygdala connectivity, in boys at the bottom left and girls at the top right.

Discussion:

– What potential biological mechanism do the authors propose underlies these sex specific results. There are only really two vague sentences on this, but the complex results need more.

Thank you. We have expanded the Discussion to include the following:

“Several plausible biological mechanisms could underlie sex differences in associations between HCC and fetal neurodevelopment. […] Finally, there may be sex differences in placental release of corticotropin-releasing hormone (CRH) which influence fetal cortisol exposure and neurodevelopment (Bangasser and Wiersielis, 2018; Sandman, Glynn and Davis, 2013).”

– The authors present an extremely comprehensive overview of the connectivity of the amygdala. Given the authors' conclusions regarding future social cognition assessments, it's surprising to see that the subcortical gaze pathway was not examined (amygdala, thalamus, superior colliculus) as this pathway rapidly processing eye/face processing. Examining connectivity with midbrain structures might be infeasible in the neonatal brain; however, including a discussion of this pathway would be useful for future research in the area.

We agree this is an interesting future direction to discuss. We have added the following to the Discussion:

“Longitudinal studies that evaluate socio-emotional development are needed to understand the functional consequences of these findings. […] The subcortical-gaze pathway is of specific interest because in adults there is evidence that structural connectivity of the superior colliculus and amygdala is related to processing of facial expressions (McFadyen, 2019), which is one of the earliest social cognitive abilities to develop and is foundational to other trajectories of cognitive development.”

– For the HCC relationships with microstructure, maternal HCC values for the preterm infants reflects exposure during (coarsely) a different trimester to the term-born infants. The subgroup analyses (term/preterm) indicate that the results are largely independent of this so does this implicate that the influences on amygdala development are from early gestation?

Yes, the analysis supports this inference. We have emphasised this point for clarity in the Discussion (fifth paragraph).

References:

Nazeri A et al., in vivo Imaging of Gray Matter Microstructure in Major Psychiatric Disorders: Opportunities for Clinical Translation. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. 5, 9, 855-864 (2020).

Qiu A, Rifkin-Graboi A, Chen H, Chong YS, Kwek K, Gluckman PD, et al. Maternal anxiety and infants' hippocampal development: timing matters. Transl psychiatry. 2013;3(9):e306-e306.Rifkin-Graboi A et al., Prenatal maternal depression associates with microstructure of right amygdala in neonates at birth. Biological psychiatry 74, 837-844 (2013).

Tottenham N, Sheridan MA., A review of adversity, the amygdala and the hippocampus: a consideration of developmental timing. Front Hum Neurosci. 3, 68 (2010).

Wen DJ et al., Influences of prenatal and postnatal maternal depression on amygdala volume and microstructure in young children. Transl psychiatry 7, e1103-e1103 (2017).

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.60729.sa2

Article and author information

Author details

  1. David Q Stoye

    MRC Centre for Reproductive Health, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, United Kingdom
    Contribution
    Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing - original draft, Writing - review and editing
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0002-2444-3573
  2. Manuel Blesa

    MRC Centre for Reproductive Health, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, United Kingdom
    Contribution
    Formal analysis, Writing - review and editing
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
  3. Gemma Sullivan

    MRC Centre for Reproductive Health, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, United Kingdom
    Contribution
    Investigation, Writing - review and editing
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
  4. Paola Galdi

    MRC Centre for Reproductive Health, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, United Kingdom
    Contribution
    Formal analysis, Methodology, Writing - review and editing
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
  5. Gillian J Lamb

    MRC Centre for Reproductive Health, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, United Kingdom
    Contribution
    Investigation, Writing - review and editing
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
  6. Gill S Black

    MRC Centre for Reproductive Health, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, United Kingdom
    Contribution
    Investigation, Writing - review and editing
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
  7. Alan J Quigley

    Department of Radiology, Royal Hospital for Sick Children, Edinburgh, United Kingdom
    Contribution
    Formal analysis
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
  8. Michael J Thrippleton

    Centre for Clinical Brain Sciences, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, United Kingdom
    Contribution
    Software, Investigation, Writing - review and editing
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
  9. Mark E Bastin

    Centre for Clinical Brain Sciences, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, United Kingdom
    Contribution
    Investigation, Methodology, Writing - review and editing
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
  10. Rebecca M Reynolds

    Centre for Cardiovascular Science, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, United Kingdom
    Contribution
    Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Writing - review and editing
    Contributed equally with
    James P Boardman
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0001-6226-8270
  11. James P Boardman

    1. MRC Centre for Reproductive Health, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, United Kingdom
    2. Centre for Clinical Brain Sciences, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, United Kingdom
    Contribution
    Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Supervision, Methodology, Writing - review and editing
    Contributed equally with
    Rebecca M Reynolds
    For correspondence
    James.boardman@ed.ac.uk
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0003-3904-8960

Funding

Theirworld

  • James P Boardman

Medical Research Council (MRC G1002033)

  • James P Boardman

British Heart Foundation (RE/18/5/34216)

  • Rebecca M Reynolds

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Acknowledgements

The work was funded by Theirworld (www.theirworld.org) and was undertaken in the MRC Centre for Reproductive Health, which is funded by MRC Centre Grant (MRC G1002033). RMR acknowledges the support of the British Heart Foundation (RE/18/5/34216). Participants were scanned in the University of Edinburgh Imaging Research MRI Facility at the Royal Infirmary of Edinburgh which was established with funding from The Wellcome Trust, Dunhill Medical Trust, Edinburgh and Lothians Research Foundation, Theirworld, The Muir Maxwell Trust and many other sources; we thank the University’s imaging research staff for providing the infant scanning.

Ethics

Human subjects: All mothers gave written informed consent. Ethical approval was granted by South East Scotland 01 Regional Ethics Committee (18/SS/0006).

Senior Editor

  1. Tamar R Makin, University College London, United Kingdom

Reviewing Editor

  1. Jonathan O'Muicheartaigh, King’s College London, United Kingdom

Reviewer

  1. Jonathan O'Muicheartaigh, King’s College London, United Kingdom

Publication history

  1. Received: July 4, 2020
  2. Accepted: October 25, 2020
  3. Version of Record published: November 24, 2020 (version 1)

Copyright

© 2020, Stoye et al.

This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.

Metrics

  • 5,090
    Page views
  • 413
    Downloads
  • 16
    Citations

Article citation count generated by polling the highest count across the following sources: Crossref, PubMed Central, Scopus.

Download links

A two-part list of links to download the article, or parts of the article, in various formats.

Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)

Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)

Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)

  1. David Q Stoye
  2. Manuel Blesa
  3. Gemma Sullivan
  4. Paola Galdi
  5. Gillian J Lamb
  6. Gill S Black
  7. Alan J Quigley
  8. Michael J Thrippleton
  9. Mark E Bastin
  10. Rebecca M Reynolds
  11. James P Boardman
(2020)
Maternal cortisol is associated with neonatal amygdala microstructure and connectivity in a sexually dimorphic manner
eLife 9:e60729.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.60729

Further reading

    1. Neuroscience
    Xiaosha Wang, Bijun Wang, Yanchao Bi
    Research Article Updated

    One signature of the human brain is its ability to derive knowledge from language inputs, in addition to nonlinguistic sensory channels such as vision and touch. How does human language experience modulate the mechanism by which semantic knowledge is stored in the human brain? We investigated this question using a unique human model with varying amounts and qualities of early language exposure: early deaf adults who were born to hearing parents and had reduced early exposure and delayed acquisition of any natural human language (speech or sign), with early deaf adults who acquired sign language from birth as the control group that matches on nonlinguistic sensory experiences. Neural responses in a semantic judgment task with 90 written words that were familiar to both groups were measured using fMRI. The deaf group with reduced early language exposure, compared with the deaf control group, showed reduced semantic sensitivity, in both multivariate pattern (semantic structure encoding) and univariate (abstractness effect) analyses, in the left dorsal anterior temporal lobe (dATL). These results provide positive, causal evidence that language experience drives the neural semantic representation in the dATL, highlighting the roles of language in forming human neural semantic structures beyond nonverbal sensory experiences.

    1. Neuroscience
    Ayako Yamaguchi, Manon Peltier
    Research Article Updated

    Across phyla, males often produce species-specific vocalizations to attract females. Although understanding the neural mechanisms underlying behavior has been challenging in vertebrates, we previously identified two anatomically distinct central pattern generators (CPGs) that drive the fast and slow clicks of male Xenopus laevis, using an ex vivo preparation that produces fictive vocalizations. Here, we extended this approach to four additional species, X. amieti, X. cliivi, X. petersii, and X. tropicalis, by developing ex vivo brain preparation from which fictive vocalizations are elicited in response to a chemical or electrical stimulus. We found that even though the courtship calls are species-specific, the CPGs used to generate clicks are conserved across species. The fast CPGs, which critically rely on reciprocal connections between the parabrachial nucleus and the nucleus ambiguus, are conserved among fast-click species, and slow CPGs are shared among slow-click species. In addition, our results suggest that testosterone plays a role in organizing fast CPGs in fast-click species, but not in slow-click species. Moreover, fast CPGs are not inherited by all species but monopolized by fast-click species. The results suggest that species-specific calls of the genus Xenopus have evolved by utilizing conserved slow and/or fast CPGs inherited by each species.