Deletion of Stk11 and Fos in mouse BLA projection neurons alters intrinsic excitability and impairs formation of long-term aversive memory
Abstract
Conditioned taste aversion (CTA) is a form of one-trial learning dependent on basolateral amygdala projection neurons (BLApn). Its underlying cellular and molecular mechanisms remain poorly understood. RNAseq from BLApn identified changes in multiple candidate learning-related transcripts including the expected immediate early gene Fos and Stk11, a master kinase of the AMP-related kinase pathway with important roles in growth, metabolism and development, but not previously implicated in learning. Deletion of Stk11 in BLApn blocked memory prior to training, but not following it and increased neuronal excitability. Conversely, BLApn had reduced excitability following CTA. BLApn knockout of a second learning-related gene, Fos, also increased excitability and impaired learning. Independently increasing BLApn excitability chemogenetically during CTA also impaired memory. STK11 and C-FOS activation were independent of one another. These data suggest key roles for Stk11 and Fos in CTA long-term memory formation, dependent at least partly through convergent action on BLApn intrinsic excitability.
Introduction
Conditioned taste aversion (CTA) is a form of long-lasting aversive memory induced by a single pairing of exposure to an initially palatable taste with gastric malaise (Bures et al., 1998). Although multiple brain regions, including the brainstem, amygdala and the cortex, participate in various aspects of taste behavior (reviewed in Carleton et al., 2010), prior work suggests that the basolateral amygdala (BLA) plays a critical role in CTA memory. Disrupting neuronal activity within the BLA blocks the formation and retrieval of CTA memory (Yasoshima et al., 2006; Ferreira et al., 2005; Garcia-Delatorre et al., 2014; Molero-Chamizo and Rivera-Urbina, 2017). This may reflect the fact that BLA projection neurons (BLApn) provide the principal output pathway from the amygdala to forebrain structures including the gustatory cortex and the central amygdala (Duvarci and Pare, 2014) enabling it to distribute taste valance information to these regions (Piette et al., 2012; Samuelsen et al., 2012). Consistent with this view, BLA neurons change their activity and their functional connectivity with their down-stream targets during CTA learning (Grossman et al., 2008). However, whether the BLA is a site of cellular and molecular plasticity during CTA learning, as opposed to merely gating plasticity in other structures, is not known.
Stages of memory formation are typically distinguished on the basis of duration and molecular mechanism. Short-term memory, lasting minutes to hours, requires only post-translational modification of preexisting proteins, whereas long-term memory, lasting days or longer, requires gene transcription and RNA translation, typically occurring in the hours following memory acquisition (Matthies, 1989; Alberini, 2009; Gal-Ben-Ari et al., 2012; Kandel, 2001). Production of new proteins is required to produce lasting changes in the efficacy of synaptic connections and in the intrinsic excitability of neurons, which are thought to be the cellular correlates of memory (Zhang and Linden, 2003; Mozzachiodi and Byrne, 2010). The cellular correlates of CTA learning are less completely understood than those of some other forms of learning, but the involvement of both synaptic plasticity (Li et al., 2016) and intrinsic plasticity (Yasoshima and Yamamoto, 1998) have been demonstrated. CTA is known to require protein synthesis in the BLA (Josselyn et al., 2004) and to increase the expression of the activity-dependent transcription factor C-FOS (Uematsu et al., 2015). In other behavioral paradigms, neurons increasing C-FOS protein undergo changes in synaptic strength and intrinsic excitability (Yassin et al., 2010; Ryan et al., 2015; Pignatelli et al., 2019) and are thought to be essential parts of the neuronal network underlying long-term memory (Tonegawa et al., 2015). However, the role of neurons expressing C-FOS in the BLA during CTA is unclear. Also unknown is whether CTA learning requires new transcription, and if so, the identities of the required transcripts and cellular processes they promote are not known.
In this study, we found that new transcription in the BLA is required for CTA learning. Using RNA-seq from sorted neurons, we found that CTA changed the expression of many genes. In addition to the expected activation of Fos, expression of the kinase Stk11, also known as Lkb1, is altered following learning in BLA projection neurons (BLApn), but not in excitatory or inhibitory neurons within the GC. STK11 is known to act as a master regulator of growth, metabolism, survival and polarity by phosphorylating 13 down-stream members of the AMP-related kinase family (Lizcano et al., 2004; Shackelford and Shaw, 2009). Recent work also suggests roles for STK11 in the nervous system, where it controls axonal specification and dynamics during development (Barnes et al., 2007; Shelly et al., 2007) and synaptic remodeling during old age (Samuel et al., 2014). STK11 can also regulate synaptic transmission in forebrain neurons (Kwon et al., 2016), but it has not previously been studied in the context of learning and memory or in the regulation of intrinsic neuronal excitability.
We find that Stk11 is required for CTA since conditional knockout from BLApn prior to training impairs learning. However, the same deletion performed 2 days after training—that is, at a time when long-term memories have already been formed and stabilized—has no effect on subsequent memory retrieval. Deletion of Stk11 also increased the excitability of BLApn, but did not alter the ability of the BLA-GC circuit to become transcriptionally activated by training or to encode the palatability of gustatory stimuli. CTA training is associated with an opposing decrease in intrinsic excitability in BLA projection neurons expressing C-FOS following learning. BLApn knockout of Fos also increased excitability and impaired learning, and expression of C-FOS and STK11 did not depend on each other. Thus, two independent genetic manipulations that affect learning also increase excitability. Using chemogenetics to independently increase BLApn excitability during CTA training without knocking out either gene also impaired CTA memory. Together, these data identify key roles for Stk11 and Fos in CTA long-term memory formation and suggest they may act by convergently modulating the intrinsic excitability of BLApn.
Results
CTA long-term memory requires BLA transcription
In order to determine whether CTA requires new RNA transcription within the BLA, we inhibited transcription by injecting Actinomycin-D (1 µl, 50 ng, bilaterally, Figure 1), a widely used RNA polymerase two inhibitor (Alberini, 2009), into the BLA 20 min prior to CTA training, and tested memory 48 hr later. As a control, a separate group of mice received vehicle injection (1 µl of PBS, bilaterally). CTA training consisted of 30 min of access to 0.5% saccharin followed by an intraperitoneal injection of 0.15M LiCl, 2% body weight; (Figure 1—figure supplement 1). A two-way mixed ANOVA comparing vehicle and actinomycin-D injected mice before and after training revealed a significant effect of treatment and a significant interaction between treatment and training (Figure 1B). Post-hoc analysis revealed significant reduction in the consumption of saccharin (CTA vs. Test) only for the vehicle group, indicating impairment of learning in the actinomycin-D treated mice. As a convergent measure, we also assessed the strength of CTA memory by calculating the relative consumption of saccharin during the test day to that consumed on the training day (Neseliler et al., 2011). The differences between the groups (23% in the vehicle group vs. 80% for the actinomycin D group) were significant. Meanwhile, actinomycin-treated mice were neither impaired in their ability to detect the palatability of saccharin, nor in their drinking behavior—consumption of saccharin during CTA training was similar for the two groups, as was consumption of water 8 hr after the test (Figure 1D), suggesting that these nonspecific effects cannot account for the memory impairment. Thus, BLA transcription is essential for CTA memory formation. These results extend prior work showing the importance of BLA protein synthesis for CTA memory (Josselyn et al., 2004) and together show that training induces both transcription and translation important for CTA in the BLA.
Transcripts regulated in BLA projection neurons following CTA
In order to identify specific transcripts that might be necessary for CTA learning within the BLA, we used cell-type-specific RNA-seq to profile transcriptional changes in sorted BLA projection neurons (BLApn). We manually isolated fluorescently labeled BLApn neurons from YFP-H mice (Feng et al., 2000) in which YFP is expressed under the Thy1 promoter in a large population of excitatory projection neurons located in the anterior part of the nucleus (Sugino et al., 2006; Jasnow et al., 2013; McCullough et al., 2016). RNA sequencing was performed separately on YFP+ BLApn harvested 4 hr following training from mice undergoing CTA, and from taste-only controls (n = 4/group) (Figure 2 and Table 1). A single control condition consisting of the taste stimulus without the unconditioned stimulus was used since our principal aim was identify target genes that could then be tested for causal roles in learning in subsequent behavioral experiments and since a prior study Rappaport et al., 2015 used a microarray screen following CTA to show that LiCl treatment (compared to taste control treatment) induced more than half of the same genes induced by CTA treatment (compared to taste control treatment).
Sequencing results (Figure 2A) also confirmed the purity and molecular identity of YFP-H neurons in the BLA, as transcripts known to be expressed in BLApn and other forebrain excitatory projection neurons were enriched and transcripts known to be expressed in inhibitory interneurons, glia cells and other neurons in the vicinity of the BLA (lateral amygdala or central amygdala) were virtually absent. Moreover, this observed pattern of expression was comparable to that reported in other studies profiling the same population (Sugino et al., 2006; McCullough et al., 2016).
CTA training changed expression of many genes. Table 1 lists the 20 transcripts showing differential expression between CTA and taste control group based on robust expression criteria: 2 ≤ fold change≤0.5, p<0.01 (unpaired t-test), transcripts-per-million (TPM) ≥30. Included among these transcripts is the activity-dependent transcription factor, Fos which has previously been shown to be upregulated in BLApn following CTA and other learning paradigms (Zhang et al., 2002; Yasoshima et al., 2006; Mayford and Reijmers, 2015; Uematsu et al., 2015). Fos is an activity-dependent transcription factor that controls the transcription of downstream targets, which are capable of changing neuronal physiology (Malik et al., 2014). Among these, YFP+ BLApn, express Nptx2, a member of the pentraxin family involved in AMPA receptor clustering (Chang et al., 2010). Our sequencing results revealed a two-fold increase of Nptx2 levels following CTA (fold-change = 2.1; p=0.029; N = 4/group, unpaired t –test), which was further validated by qPCR in separate experiments (Fold-change = 2.4; p=0.028; N = 4/group; Figure 2—figure supplement 3).
Our screen for differentially expressed genes also identified Stk11 (also known as Lkb1; Figure 2B) a kinase well studied in the contexts of cancer, cell growth and development, but not previously studied in the contexts of learning and neuronal plasticity (Bardeesy et al., 2002; Alessi et al., 2006; Barnes et al., 2007; Gurumurthy et al., 2010; Courchet et al., 2013). Changes in the expression of both Fos and Stk11 were validated by qPCR in separate experiments, which revealed a 4.6-fold increase in Fos mRNA (t(6)=2.5; p=0.045) and a 1.9-fold decrease in Stk11 (t(6)=-2.5; p=0.046) (Figure 2C). To further examine the significance of these transcriptional changes, we also analyzed the levels of FOS and STK11 protein in the BLA. The fraction of C-FOS-expressing YFP-H neurons in the BLA 4 hr following CTA was significantly increased relative to lithium chloride-only and taste-only control groups (Figure 2—figure supplement 1). We measured STK11 4 hr following CTA or presentation of taste alone, by performing immunoblotting of proteins isolated from the anterior BLA. Surprisingly, we found a 1.8-fold increase (Figure 2—figure supplement 2) indicating that protein and mRNA expression are regulated in opposite directions at this time point.
STK11 is at the apex of the AMP-related kinase pathway and mediates its effects by phosphorylating one or more of 13 different downstream kinases, all of which share some homology with AMP-kinase (Lizcano et al., 2004; Shackelford and Shaw, 2009). Among these, YFP+ BLApn had moderate expression of Brsk2 (TPM = 39.9) and Mark2/3 (TPM = 59.3 and 73.7 respectively; Figure 2—figure supplement 3), which have known roles in establishing cell polarity during neuronal development (Barnes et al., 2007; Shackelford and Shaw, 2009). Expression of AMP kinase itself (Prkaa1/2), an important metabolic regulator in many cell types (Shackelford and Shaw, 2009) was lower (TPM = 16.4 and 22.3, respectively). Comparing kinase expression between CTA and control groups revealed nearly two-fold less expression of Mark2 following CTA (fold-change = 0.41; p=0.04, unpaired t-test; data not shown), but this was not significant after Bonferroni correction across the compared kinases.
We also analyzed the impact of CTA on the transcriptional profile of layer five pyramidal neurons labeled in strain YFP-H and Pvalb-expressing inhibitory interneurons in the GC, a brain region in which transcription is also known to be important for CTA learning (Inberg et al., 2016). RNA sequencing was performed on YFP+ and Pvalb+ neurons in the GC harvested from mice 4 hr following CTA training, and from taste-only controls (n = 3/4/group). Tables 2 and 3 list the transcripts showing the most differentially expressed genes using the same criteria used in the BLA: 2 ≤ fold change≤0.5, p<0.01 (unpaired t-test), transcripts-per-million (TPM) ≥30. While Pvalb+ neurons showed a robust transcriptional response, evident by 19 genes reaching the criteria, only one gene reached the same criteria in YFP+, suggesting a weaker CTA-driven transcriptional response in these neurons. Importantly, the expression of Fos and Stk11 in both YFP+ and Pvalb+ neurons, did not differ between CTA and control groups (Figure 2—figure supplement 4 and Tables 2 and 3). This suggests that the differential expression of Fos and Stk11 during CTA learning may be specific to a subset of cell-types within the circuit.
Fos and Stk11 expression in BLA projection neurons are necessary for memory formation
We next wished to determine whether any of the transcriptional changes in BLApns that correlate with learning are indeed necessary for learning to occur. Since both C-FOS and STK11 protein increase following CTA, we pursued a loss-of-function (LOF) strategy. To restrict LOF to BLApns, we performed conditional deletion by injecting Cre recombinase into mice carrying alleles of Fos (Zhang et al., 2002) or Stk11(Nakada et al., 2010) in which key exons are flanked by lox-p sites. In both cases, recombination leads to a functionally null allele and analyses were carried out in homozygous (f/f) animals. Cre was delivered by injecting AAV2/5-Camk2α::Cre-GFP into the BLA bilaterally. As expected, this led to GFP expression in excitatory projection neurons, but not in BLA interneurons or adjacent GABAergic neurons in the Central Amygdala (CEA; Figure 3, 5). Animals carrying the same genotypes but receiving AAV2/5-Camk2α::GFP served as controls. Injections were performed 10 days prior to analysis to allow time for LOF to occur.
To assess the efficacy of this approach for CTA learning, we first examined the necessity of Fos expression. Fos is known to contribute to multiple forms of learning and plasticity (Zhang et al., 2002; Tonegawa et al., 2015) and has previously been implicated in CTA (Lamprecht and Dudai, 1996; Yasoshima et al., 2006). To first confirm effective Cre-mediated recombination, we tested the ability of viral Cre to prevent widespread C-FOS expression in the BLA immediately following kainic-acid-induced seizures. C-FOS staining performed four hours after seizures revealed strong induction throughout the BLA of control mice injected with the control virus, and diminished C-FOS expression in mice injected with Cre (Figure 3A,B). We then tested the effect of Fos deletion on CTA in separate animals. Cre-GFP and GFP control AAV’s were injected into the BLA of Fosf/f mice, CTA training occurred 10 days later, and long-term memory was tested after an additional 2 days.
The results show that while both groups could form CTA memory, Cre injected mice showed significantly weaker memory (Figure 3C–E). This confirms our ability to manipulate memory by conditional knockout in BLApn neurons. The results refine those of a prior study using antisense injections and germline knockouts (Yasoshima et al., 2006).
Next, we used the same strategy to test the necessity of Stk11 expression in BLApn for CTA memory. BLA of Stk11f/f mice were injected bilaterally with AAV expressing Cre- GFP, or GFP alone. Ten days later, both groups of mice were trained for CTA and tested after an additional 2 days. The results revealed a near complete loss of learning in the Stk11 KO mice (Figure 4A,B), which exhibited no significant reduction in the amount or ratio of saccharin consumed after training. Control mice exhibited significant reductions consistent with a similar degree of learning to that seen in previous control experiments. The differences in the consumption of saccharin during the test day were not attributable to overall reduced drinking, as both group of animals consumed comparable amounts of water 8 hr later (Figure 4C). Taken together, these results indicate that Stk11 expression in BLApn is essential for CTA memory.
-
Figure 4—source data 1
- https://cdn.elifesciences.org/articles/61036/elife-61036-fig4-data1-v2.docx
-
Figure 4—source data 2
- https://cdn.elifesciences.org/articles/61036/elife-61036-fig4-data2-v2.docx
-
Figure 4—source data 3
- https://cdn.elifesciences.org/articles/61036/elife-61036-fig4-data3-v2.docx
-
Figure 4—source data 4
- https://cdn.elifesciences.org/articles/61036/elife-61036-fig4-data4-v2.docx
Stk11 deletion prior to CTA training can potentially alter multiple memory stages including memory formation and retrieval (Levitan et al., 2016a; Levitan et al., 2016b). Because CTA memory is long-lasting after even a single training session, it is possible to distinguish an effect of Stk11 deletion on memory formation from an effect on retrieval by performing the deletion figure supplement 1). Both Cre-GFP and GFP injected mice developed CTA (Figure 4D) – there was no significant between-group difference in the intensity of memory, assessed from the ratio of saccharin consumed during the test (Figure 4E). Since the same deletion produces a profound effect when occurring prior to training, this suggests that deletion of Stk11 from BLApn does not affect the retention and retrieval of CTA memory, provided memory was already formed prior to performing the knockout. This argues that Stk11 is required for CTA memory formation.
The fact that the expression of both Fos and Stk11 in BLApn are needed for normal CTA long-term memory raises the question of their possible interaction. To address this question, we first examined the expression of C-FOS 4 hr following CTA training in Stk11f/f mice receiving Cre or control virus injection 10 days earlier. There was no difference between the control and knockout groups (t(4)=-0.42; p=0.69, N = 3/group. Figure 5—figure supplement 1a). In a complementary experiment, we examined the expression of STK11 4 hr following CTA in Fosf/f mice receiving Cre or control virus injection 10 days earlier. We also found no significant difference between the control and knockout groups in this experiment (t(4)=0.028; p=0.84, N = 3/group. Figure 5—figure supplement 1b). Thus, the expression of Stk11 or Fos is not dependent upon the expression of the other, and they likely represent signaling pathways that are at least initially separate.
Stk11 deletion in BLApn does not impact taste palatability coding in the GC
Since Stk11 deletion must occur prior to CTA training to have an effect on learning, we needed to rule out the possibility that its effect on memory came via disruption of either the responsiveness of BLA neurons to training stimuli, or the output of BLA neurons, which is known to be required for palatability coding within GC.
A more rigorous test of BLA function is to determine whether palatability coding in the GC is intact following knockout, since this is known to depend on intact output from the BLA (Piette et al., 2012; Samuelsen et al., 2012; Lin and Reilly, 2012; Lin et al., 2018). If Stk11 deletion disrupts gustatory activation of BLApn or their output to the GC, palatability coding recorded in the GC should be impaired. To test this, Stk11 deletion in BLApn was performed as before and 10 days later multi-channel in vivo recordings of GC taste responses were obtained. Recordings were targeted to the ventral part of GC, since BLA projects to these regions (Figure 5A; Maffei et al., 2012; Haley et al., 2016; Levitan et al., 2019). Palatability coding was assessed with a battery of four tastes with hedonic values ranging from palatable (sucrose and sodium-chloride) to aversive (citric-acid and quinine; for details see Levitan et al., 2019).
Figure 5 shows the results of GC taste responses following Stk11 deletions in BLA. Figure 5B illustrates the peri-stimulus histogram of a representative GC neuron responding to the taste battery. As observed previously in rats and mice (Katz et al., 2001; Sadacca et al., 2012; Levitan et al., 2019), different aspects of taste processing are encoded in firing rates sequentially. In the first five hundred milliseconds or so post-taste delivery, neurons show different firing rates to different tastes (i.e. reflecting taste identity coding), while later in the responses, differential response rates reflect the hedonic values of tastes (i.e. taste palatability coding). These properties were maintained in the mice studied here: as indicated by the dashed line, the magnitude of the correlation between the neuron’s stimulus evoked firing rates and the behaviorally determined palatability ranking rose significantly only after the first half a second following taste delivery. The averaged correlation magnitudes across neurons from Cre and GFP injected mice are shown in Figure 5C. Inspection of the figure suggests that BLA Stk11 deletion had little effects on GC taste palatability coding; the correlations in both GFP- and Cre-injected groups rise around half a second and peaks at about one second after taste delivery. An ANOVA found no significant group differences across each time bin, suggesting that Stk11 deletion in BLA has little detectable influence on GC taste processing.
Taken together, our molecular and electrophysiological analyses suggest that the memory deficit observed after Stk11 deletion is unlikely to be due to a deficit in basic taste processing. Rather, Stk11 deletion likely impairs memory by affecting the process of memory formation.
Stk11 and Fos deletion increase the intrinsic excitability of BLApn, while CTA decreases excitability
What cellular mechanisms mediate the effects of Fos and Stk11 on memory formation? Long-term memory is known to be accompanied by changes in both the intrinsic excitability of neurons (Zhang and Linden, 2003; Mozzachiodi and Byrne, 2010) and in the strength of their synaptic connections. Recent studies have shown that the intrinsic excitability of BLApn can be modulated bi-directionally during reinforcement learning, with positive reinforcement leading to increased and negative reinforcement to decreased intrinsic excitability (Motanis et al., 2014).
To determine whether Stk11 might influence memory formation by altering intrinsic excitability, we compared the excitability of BLApn recorded in ex vivo slices from mutant animals receiving Cre-GFP (Figure 6). The results reveal a marked increase in the intrinsic excitability of BLApn following deletion of Stk11, relative to GFP-only controls. Cre infected neurons had higher firing rates than GFP neurons for any given amount of current input resulting in a steeper slope of the firing rate vs. current (F-I) curve. Threshold firing was initiated at a lower level of current injection (i.e. the rheobase was lower; Figure 6C). This reflected a higher resting input resistance and a slightly lower voltage threshold. Some other electrophysiological properties also differed (see Table 4) including sag ratios, action potential amplitudes, and the medium and slow afterhyperpolarizations, while others, such as the degree of firing rate accommodation, spike widths and resting membrane potentials did not. Thus, Stk11 deletion from BLApn neurons increases overall intrinsic excitability, most likely by affecting multiple biophysical properties of these neurons.
Since Stk11 and Fos deletion both impair CTA learning, we wondered whether Fos deletion also increases the intrinsic excitability of BLApn. Analysis of Fos KO neurons revealed a similar increase in firing relative to control neurons (N for: WT = 14, KO = 22; Figure 7A–B). Two-way ANOVA indicated a significant effect of genotype (in addition to the expected effect of current level). Significant differences in input resistance and action potential threshold were also detected.Next, we asked whether we could detect an effect of learning itself on neuronal excitability in the BLA. A difficulty with this experiment is that learning may have different effects on different populations of BLApn as evidenced by the fact that some neurons increase C-FOS expression following training, while others do not (Figure 2—figure supplement 2). In order to separately examine these two populations following training, we made use of the reporter system developed by Reijmers and colleagues, in which elements of the Fos promoter are used to drive the tet transactivator (tTA) which can then prolong and amplify expression of a reporter marking Fos-activated cells, when Doxycycline (Dox) is absent (Reijmers et al., 2007). In this case, we provided the tet-dependent reporter via an AAV (TRE::mCherry) injected into the BLA 10 days prior to training. Animals were fed Dox until 24 hr prior to training to limit background activation (Figure 7C). Control experiments showed that CTA training resulted in a greater number of neurons expressing Fos 24 hr after training, compared to home cage controls (Figure 7D). Twenty-four hours following CTA training or taste-only control experiments, recordings were obtained from BLApn in acute slices. Two-way ANOVA revealed that BLApn in the CTA animals have lower input resistance than those in control animals and that Fos-activated neurons have lower input resistance than unlabeled neurons (Figure 7E). Notably, this change is in the opposite direction from that produced by deletions of Stk11 and Fos, two manipulations that impair learning. This suggests that these mutations may interfere with learning by impairing changes in intrinsic excitability that are required for learning to occur.
To more directly test the hypothesis that changes in intrinsic excitability of BLApn play a key role in the formation of CTA memory, we virally expressed the excitatory DREADD (Designer Receptors Exclusively Activated by Designer Drugs) hM3Dq and then transiently increased the intrinsic excitability of BLApn during memory formation. hM3Dq is a Gq-coupled muscarinic receptor containing a modified binding site permitting activation by the synthetic ligand CNO (Clozapine-N-Oxide) but not by endogenous ligands (Alexander et al., 2009). Whole-cell patch clamp recordings in acute brain slices from BLApn infected in vivo with AAV-Camk2α::hM3Dq confirmed the ability of CNO to increase their intrinsic excitability (8-figure supplement 1 A-C: higher firing rate for CNO conditions compared to control with 100 pA current input, paired sample t-test, t = −4.73, p=1×10−4; Resting membrane potentials of neurons are significantly higher in CNO conditions than in control (paired sample t-test, t = −8.52, p=2×10−8). In addition, we confirmed that systemic injection of 0.3 mg/kg CNO to mice infected with Camk2α::hM3Dq increased C-FOS expression 60 min later (Figure 8—figure supplement 1). Mice infected with Camk2α::hM3Dq/GFP were then trained for CTA 60 min after systemic injection of 0.3 mg/kg CNO and were tested 48 hr later. We found that while both groups of mice developed CTA reflected by significantly reduced saccharin consumption, hM3Dq-injected mice consumed more saccharin during the test then mice receiving the control GFP virus, but did not differ in their consumption of saccharin during training in comparison to GFP injected mice (Figure 8A) resulting in significantly stronger CTA (Figure 8B). These data show that increasing excitability during CTA training impairs CTA memory formation. Together with our previous data, this suggests that that Stk11 and Fos expression play key roles in CTA long-term memory formation, by modulating the intrinsic excitability of BLApn.
Discussion
This study is the first to identify a role for Stk11, a master kinase at the top of the AMP-related kinase pathway, in long-term memory. Using CTA as a behavioral paradigm, we first established a causal requirement for transcription in BLApn during establishment of long-term memory, and went on to show that changes in Stk11 and Fos transcription and translation accompany CTA learning. Cell-type-specific conditional knock-out of Stk11 in BLApn revealed it to be necessary for CTA memory formation but not for retrieval, once memories were established. Slice recordings revealed that Stk11 modulated the intrinsic excitability of these neurons and further investigations suggested the general importance of excitability changes for memory—deletion of the immediate early gene Fos in BLApn altered excitability similarly to Stk11 deletion, and conversely activation of Fos during learning reduced excitability. Finally, a transient chemogenetic increase in BLApn excitability during CTA training also impaired CTA memory. This suggests a model in which Stk11 and Fos expression plays important role in CTA long-term memory formation by modulating the intrinsic excitability of BLApn.
BLA projection neurons undergo transcription important for CTA learning
It is well established that BLA neurons play a necessary role in CTA learning. Multiple studies confirm that activity in the BLA is required for memory formation and retrieval (Ferreira et al., 2005; Garcia-Delatorre et al., 2014; Molero-Chamizo and Rivera-Urbina, 2017). CTA also requires protein synthesis in the BLA (Josselyn et al., 2004), but whether new transcription is also required, and if so, the identities of the required transcripts and the cellular processes they promote were not previously known.
Here, we show that BLA projection neurons (BLApn) undergo transcriptional changes important for CTA memory. Inhibiting transcription during CTA training impairs memory tested 48 hr later. Using cell-type-specific RNA sequencing, we go beyond this simple insight to identify the transcripts that are altered in expression in BLApn four hours after pairing of the conditioned and unconditioned stimuli. For comparison, we also examined changes in transcript levels in pyramidal neurons and parvalbumin-positive interneurons in gustatory cortex. These profiling experiments provide a resource for future investigations of other molecules potentially involved in CTA in BLA and GC.
Perhaps the strongest case for new transcription in BLApn involved in learning can be made for the immediate early gene Fos. It is well known that Fos transcription and translation are activated in the forebrain by a variety of memory paradigms (Mayford and Reijmers, 2015), and more specifically by CTA in BLApn (Uematsu et al., 2015). The YFP-H neurons studied here include the majority of BLApn in the anterior portion of the nucleus (Feng et al., 2000; Sugino et al., 2006; Jasnow et al., 2013; McCullough et al., 2016) and the fact that many of these neurons express Fos protein (Figure 2—figure supplement 1) and project to GC (Figure 5A and Haley et al., 2016) supports the suggestion that they are among the population of BLApn transcriptionally activated by training and participating in the BLA-GC circuit implicated in learning by prior studies (Grossman et al., 2008). Since Fos transcript and protein are short-lived (Spiegel et al., 2014; Chowdhury and Caroni, 2018), the most parsimonious explanation is that training induces new transcription and translation, and that it is these effects that are disrupted by the Fos KO in BLApn (Figure 3).
The results of selectively knocking Fos out in BLApn clarify the results of earlier studies in which Fos was manipulated with infusion of antisense oligonucleotides (Lamprecht and Dudai, 1996; Yasoshima et al., 2006) or via global knockout (which had no effect on CTA; Yasoshima et al., 2006). Loss of memory has previously been attributed to inhibition of Fos in central amygdala (Lamprecht and Dudai, 1996), or in the amygdala as a whole along with the GC (Yasoshima et al., 2006). Our demonstration that knockout restricted to BLApn is sufficient to impair memory does not contradict these earlier studies, but suggests that these projection neurons may be a nexus or bottleneck vital for learning in the circuit.
There is still much to be learned about transcription of Stk11 in CTA. This transcript is presumably less transient than that of immediate early genes, and may be regulated through a process with complex dynamics. This issue is brought into focus by the fact that, across the time points measured, the transcript in profiled cells was decreased, while in anatomically sub-dissected portions of BLA, STK11 protein was increased. Improved temporal and spatial mapping of transcript and protein levels may help clarify these differences.
Regardless, however, the finding that protein levels of STK11 increase are consistent with the fact that Stk11 deletion before CTA training impairs CTA memory (Figure 4) and with the finding that Stk11 deletion increases BLApn intrinsic excitability (Figure 6), which is reduced after CTA training (Figure 7) and is needed for CTA memory (Figure 8). Opposing changes in STK11 protein and mRNA levels could reflect homeostatic control of this powerful signaling pathway. Translation-dependent feedback of some other proteins is known to reduce mRNA levels through nonsense-mediated decay (Giorgi et al., 2007; Paolantoni et al., 2018) or other forms of mRNA degradation (Lin et al., 2020; Shoshani and Cleveland, 2020). Complex feedback control involving changes in RNA export and stability has also been described for MAP kinase signaling (Sugiura et al., 2003; Prieto-Ruiz et al., 2020), but whether this type of regulation exists for Stk11 and the AMP-related kinase pathway remains unknown.
Necessity of Stk11 implicates the AMP-related kinase pathway in learning
Prior studies of CTA and other forms of aversive learning in the BLA have implicated a number of kinases: including those in the cAMP-dependent protein kinase, protein kinase C, extracellular signal-regulated, and mitogen-activated protein kinase pathways (Johansen et al., 2011; Adaikkan and Rosenblum, 2012). Each of these also have well-established roles in other forms of forebrain learning and plasticity (Alberini, 2009). Here, we reveal the likely involvement of another kinase cascade, well studied in the contexts of cell growth, metabolism, cancer and polarity (Shackelford and Shaw, 2009) but hitherto unstudied in the context of learning and memory. That this pathway should have a role in learning is perhaps not shocking given the ubiquity of its previously demonstrated roles in 1) axonal development Barnes et al., 2007; Shelly et al., 2007; 2) synaptic remodeling during aging Samuel et al., 2014; 3) regulation of presynaptic neurotransmission Kwon et al., 2016; and perhaps most tellingly 4) regulation of glucose metabolism, feeding and obesity through actions in multiple tissues including hypothalamus (Xi et al., 2018; Fei-Wang et al., 2012; Claret et al., 2011). Given the involvement of hypothalamus in coding of taste palatability, and the connectivity between hypothalamus and gustatory cortex (Li et al., 2013), it is tempting to speculate that the role of Stk11 signaling pathways in feeding may be functionally related to its role in gustatory learning. Further studies will be needed to distinguish whether the involvement of Stk11 in memory is specific to forms of learning regulating consumption, and whether its role in CTA learning is confined to the basolateral amygdala.
STK11 is a master kinase that regulates the activity of 13 downstream AMP-related kinases with diverse roles (Lizcano et al., 2004). PRKAA1/2 (also known as AMPK) is crucial for metabolic regulation during altered levels of nutrients and intracellular energy. BRSK and MARK regulate cell polarity during development (Barnes et al., 2007; Shackelford and Shaw, 2009). We found that, while Prkaa1/2 and several other downstream kinase transcripts have low levels of expression in BLApn, others, including Mark2 and 3, are expressed at higher levels (Figure 2—figure supplement 3). Furthermore, changes in Mark2 and Stk11 expression were correlated during CTA learning raising the possibility that both kinases in the pathway contribute to this novel signaling role in learning and memory.
Deletion of Stk11 prior to training profoundly impaired memory, but deletion two days after training—when memory formation and consolidation have already occurred (Alberini, 2009; Gal-Ben-Ari et al., 2012; Levitan et al., 2016b) —did not. This suggests that Stk11 expression in BLApn promotes memory formation, rather than memory maintenance or retrieval. It is clear that Stk11 deletion left much of the machinery of taste processing and learning intact, however. Activation of Fos by training in the BLA was not impaired after Stk11 deletion, implying that at least the initial stages of transcriptional activation associated with learning are intact. Also left intact was the ability of the BLA to convey palatability information to the GC. Prior studies have shown that silencing of BLA neurons (Piette et al., 2012) impair palatability coding during the late phase of GC gustatory responses. We found that these responses were still present in GC following knockout, implying that this critical function of the BLA for CTA learning remained intact.
Intrinsic excitability of BLApn as a candidate mechanism for Fos and Stk11’s effects on CTA memory
Learning paradigms that support synaptic plasticity also frequently induce changes in neuronal excitability, and such plasticity of intrinsic excitability has long been known to accompany classical conditioning in the neocortex, olfactory cortex, hippocampus, amygdala and cerebellum (for reviews see Zhang and Linden, 2003; Frick and Johnston, 2005; Mozzachiodi and Byrne, 2010; Titley et al., 2017; Debanne et al., 2019).
Although we cannot rule out the possibility that Stk11 or Fos expression in BLApn promotes other forms of plasticity (such as changes in synaptic strength as was suggested in Yassin et al., 2010) for Fos expression in neurons), our work provide evidence that in BLApn both genes promote changes in intrinsic excitability important for CTA memory. Two genetic manipulations of BLApn that impair learning (deletion of Stk11 and Fos) also increase intrinsic excitability, whereas BLApn involved in normal conditioning appear to undergo the opposite change. Moreover, transiently increasing BLApn intrinsic excitability during training impaired CTA memory, suggesting a model in which Stk11 and Fos promote changes in BLApn intrinsic excitability necessary for CTA memory. This suggests an important role in learning for changes in excitability, and begs the question of mechanism. Although there are likely several such mechanisms, the increase in excitability partly reflects an increase in the resting input resistance and a corresponding decrease in the threshold current needed to evoke firing. It is worth noting that most prior studies of intrinsic plasticity have reported increases in excitability with learning in various brain regions and different animal models (Zhang and Linden, 2003; Mozzachiodi and Byrne, 2010; Yiu et al., 2014; Whitaker et al., 2017; Chandra and Barkai, 2018; Pignatelli et al., 2019). Our results are not without precedent, however; and are similar in polarity to those found in BLApn following olfactory fear conditioning, another form of negative reinforcement learning (Motanis et al., 2014). Decreased excitability of Fos-activated neurons was also found using an earlier reporter of Fos promoter activation (Yassin et al., 2010).
Although both Stk11 and Fos expression influence BLApn intrinsic excitability, several observations argue this reflects convergence of two at least partially separate signaling pathways in the BLApn neurons. First, deletion of Stk11 did not block the ability of training to elicit C-FOS expression, and deletion of Fos did not block STK11 expression. Additionally, Stk11 deletion in BLApn had a stronger effect on CTA memory and on intrinsic excitability then deletion of Fos. However, our data do not exclude the possibility for synergy between the STK11 and C-FOS signaling pathways. Each signaling pathway could influence excitability by modulating different ion-channels. Alternatively, both signaling pathways may converge on the same ion-channels but influence complementary processes such as expression (Kourrich, 2005), post-translational modification (Vernon and Irvine, 2016) or trafficking (Steele et al., 2007). If future studies identify the specific ion channels targeted by STK11 and C-FOS related signaling, these questions of synergy and convergence can be more precisely addressed. Our data do not exclude the possibility that the neurons expressing Fos represent a sub-population BLApn expressing Stk11. Future studies will be aimed to determine with a much finer resolution the distribution of these genes among the population of BLApn.
In conclusion, our data suggest the following model of CTA (Figure 9). Training induces the activation of STK11 and C-FOS by increasing their protein levels in BLApn. STK11 activation results in downstream activation (through phosphorylation) of AMP-related kinases, most likely MARK2/3, BRSK2, SIK3, which are abundant in BLApn. C-FOS, an activity dependent transcription factor, induces the transcription of effector genes such as Nptx2 which is increased following CTA training. NPTX2 may modulate intrinsic excitability through its ability to bind ion-channels (Duzhyy et al., 2005; Chang et al., 2010). The two pathways jointly reduce the intrinsic excitability of BLApn, which plays a key role in gating the formation of CTA memory.
Materials and methods
Subjects
Male and Female mice (total of 209 animals) were used for behavior at age 60–80 days, or for electrophysiology at 25–35 days. Strains: wild-type; WT (C57BL/6J), YFP-H (B6.Cg-Tg(Thy1-YFP)HJrs/J), Stk11f/f (B6(Cg)-Stk11tm1.1Sjm/J, Lkb1fl), Fosf/f (B6;129-Fostm1Mxu/Mmjax), Fos-tTA (B6.Cg-Tg(Fos-tTA,Fos-EGFP*)1Mmay/J) all purchased from Jackson Laboratories (Bar Harbor, ME). Mice were placed on a 12 hr light-dark cycle, and given ad libitum access to food and water except during training, at which time water access was restricted, while food remained available ad libitum (note that animals reliably consume less food when thirsty). All procedures were approved by the Brandeis University Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (IACUC, Protocol #20002) in accordance with NIH guidelines.
Surgery
BLA cannulation for RNA synthesis inhibition experiments
Request a detailed protocolWT Mice were anesthetized via ip injections of 100 µg ketamine, 12.5 µg xylazine, 2.5 µg acepromazine per gram (KXA). Guide cannulae (23-gauge, 10 mm length) were implanted above the BLA (mm from bregma, AP = −1.4, DV = 4.2, ML=±3.4; Allen Institute for Brain Science, 2019) and stabilized using Vetbond and dental acrylic. Stainless steel stylets (30-gage, 10 mm) were inserted into the guide cannula to ensure patency. Mice received postsurgical metacam (5 µg/g), penicillin (1500 Units/g) and saline (5% bodyweight) per day for three days and recovered a total of 7 days prior to training. Twenty minutes prior to CTA training, mice were infused with either 50 ng of actinomycin-D or vehicle control (PBS) bilaterally (in 1 μl over 2 min) via infusion cannulae extending 0.5 mm below the guide cannulae to reach the BLA. Each cannula was connected to a 10 μl Hamilton syringe on a syringe pump (Harvard Apparatus, MA, USA).
BLA viral infection
Request a detailed protocolStk11f/f, Fosf/f and Fos-tTA mice were anesthetized with KXA. The skull was exposed, cleaned, and bilateral craniotomies were made at stereotactic coordinates (AP = −1.4, ML=±3.4). BLA were injected bilaterally with AAV2/5-Camk2α::Cre-GFP or AAV2/5-Camk2α::GFP (UNC, vector core) for Stk11f/f and Fosf/f mice, Camk2α::hM3Dq-mCherry or AAV2/5-Camk2α::GFP (UNC, vector core) for Stk11f/f mice and AAV2/5-TRE::mCherry for Fos-tTa mice, 10 days prior to CTA training using sterile glass micropipettes (10–20 µm diameter) attached to a partially automated microinjection device (Nanoject III Microinjector, Drummond Scientific). The micropipettes were lowered to 4.3 mm and 4.6 mm from the dura to reach the BLA. At each depth, virus (200 nl) was delivered via 10 pulses of 20 delivered every 10 s, with 10 min between each injection. Postsurgical treatment and recovery were as above.
Conditioned taste aversion (CTA)
Request a detailed protocolMice were housed individually with free access to food and maintained on a 23.0 hr water deprivation schedule for the duration of training and experimentation. Three days prior to CTA training, water bottles were removed from the cages and water was given twice a day (10 am and six pm) for a duration of 30 min. On the day of CTA mice were given 30 min to consume 0.5% saccharin, which was followed by intraperitoneal (I.P) injection of lithium-chloride (0.15M, 2% of body weight, unless indicated differently) 30 min later. Taste control groups received I.P injection of saline (0.9% sodium chloride) instead of lithium and lithium control group received lithium injection alone 24 hr following Saccharin consumption. CTA testing: Mice were kept on watering schedule twice a day and 48 hr after CTA training mice received CTA testing which consisted of 30 min consumption of 0.5% saccharin. 8 hr following testing mice were given 30 min water consumption.
Seizure induction
Request a detailed protocolFosf/f mice were housed individually with free access to water and food and received BLA viral infection with Cre and control viruses as described above. After 10 days, mice were injected I.P. with 20 mg/kg kainic acid in PBS. Four hours after injection, mice were perfused for Fos immunohistochemistry.
CNO treatment
Request a detailed protocolCNO (sigma) was diluted in saline to 1 mg/ml. CNO was systemically injected at a dose of 0.3 mg/kg 1 hr prior to CTA training. We chose this dose as we notice that higher concentration (2 mg/kg) causes seizures as was reported (Alexander et al., 2009) and reduction of saccharin consumption. A dose of 0.3 mg/kg did not cause any effect on basal drinking behavior and could strongly activate infected neurons (Figure 8—figure supplement 1), as reported previously by Alexsander et al.
Immunohistochemistry
Request a detailed protocolMice were deeply anesthetized with an overdose of KXA and perfused transcardially with phosphate buffered solution (PBS) followed by 4% paraformaldehyde (PFA). Brains were post-fixed in PFA for 1–2 day, and coronal brain slices (60 µm) containing the BLA (−1 mm to −2.5 mm anterior-poterior axis) were sectioned on a vibratome. Slices were rinsed with PBS and incubated in a blocking solution (PBS/.3%TritionX-100/5% Bovine serum albumin) for 12–24 hr at 4°C. Blocking solution was removed and replaced with the primary antibody solution which consists of 1:100 C-FOS polyclonal rabbit IgG (SC-52G; Santa Cruz Biotechnology) for 24 hr at 4°C. After incubation, slices were rinsed using a PBS/.3% Triton X-100 solution followed by the secondary antibody incubation of 1:500 C-FOS Alexa Flour 546 Goat-Anti-Rabbit IgG (H+L) (Life Technologies) and 5% natural goat serum for 12–24 hr at 4°C. Sections were then rinsed 5–6 times over 90 min (1XPBS/.3% Triton X-100), counterstained with DAPI, mounted with antifade mounting medium (Vectashield), and viewed by confocal fluorescence microscopy (Leica Sp5 Spectral confocal microscope/Resonant Scanner). Imaging and quantification were performed blind to experimental group.
C-FOS quantification and analysis
Request a detailed protocolTo minimize systematic bias, C-FOS counts were performed blind and semi-automatically, using FiJi (University of Wisconsin-Madison; Schindelin et al., 2012). Eight-bit images were binarized and particles smaller than 10 µm2 were rejected. Each cell count was from a separate animal and was the average of counts from six sections through the anterior, middle and posterior regions of the BLA of both hemispheres.
RNA sequencing experiment
Request a detailed protocolRNA sequencing was performed on YFP+ BLApn harvested from male YFP-H mouse line (Feng et al., 2000; Sugino et al., 2006; Jasnow et al., 2013; McCullough et al., 2016) which expresses YFP under the Thy1 promoter in the majority of excitatory projection neurons located in the anterior part of the nucleus. The mice underwent CTA training or taste-only controls (n = 4/group) and 4 hr following training were subjected to manual cell-sorting, performed as previously described (Sugino et al., 2006; Hempel et al., 2007; Shima et al., 2016) by dissociating 150–200 fluorescently labeled neurons in 300-µm-thick brain slices and manually purifying them through multiple transfer dishes with the aid of a pipette viewed under a fluorescence dissection microscope. Total RNA was extracted from sorted cells using Pico-pure RNA isolation kit (Thermo fisher). Amplified cDNA libraries are prepared from isolated, fragmented RNA using the NuGen Ovation RNAseq V.2 kit (NuGEN, San Carlos, CA) and followed by purification using the Beckman coulter Genomic’s Agencourt RNA Clean XP kit and Zymo DNA Clean and Concentrator. Sequencing adaptors are ligated per Illumina protocols and 50 bp single-ended reads are obtained from Illumina Hi-Seq machine. Libraries sequenced usually results in 25–30 million unique reads using eightfold multiplexing.
Analysis
Reads are trimmed and then aligned to the mouse genome using TopHat (Trapnell et al., 2012). Sam files are converted to binary format using Samtools and visualized at the sequence level using IGV. Script written in python and R statistical package are used to convert unique reads to gene expression values and to filter genes by relative expression and statistical significance. Sequencing data was uploaded to GEO (accession number: GSE138522).
qPCR validation
Request a detailed protocolThe brains of a separate group of YFP-H mice receiving CTA (taste+lithium, N = 4) or taste control (taste+saline, N = 4) were harvested 4 hr following the end of the training and subjected to fax sorting. RNA was extracted using pico-pure kit and reverse transcribed to cDNA using iScript cDNA synthesis kit. qPCR was performed on Rotor-Gene qPCR machine using PCR master mix and transcript-specific sets of primers for Fos, Stk11 as target genes and Snap47 as loading control.
Acute slice electrophysiology
Request a detailed protocolTen days after virus injection, acute brain slices were prepared from P28-35 mice. Animals were deeply anesthetized with KXA and transcardially perfused with ice-cold oxygenated cutting solution containing (in mM): 10 N-methyl-D-glucamine (NMDG), 3 KCl, 1 NaH2PO4, 25 NaHCO3, 20 HEPES, 2 Thiourea, 3 Sodium Pyruvate, 12 N-acetyl-L-cysteine, 6 MgCl2, 0.5 CaCl2, 5 Sodium Ascorbate, 10 Glucose (pH: 7.25–7.4, adjusted using HCl). 300 µm coronal slices containing the BLA were cut on a vibratome (Leica), and then recovered for 15 min at 33°C and for 15 min at room temperature in oxygenated recovery solution containing (in mM): 74 NaCl, 3 KCl, 1 NaH2PO4, 25 NaHCO3, 6 MgCl2, 0.5 CaCl2, 5 Sodium Ascorbate, 75 Sucrose, 10 Glucose, followed by at least another 1 hr at room temperature in oxygenated ACSF containing (in mM): 126 NaCl, 3 KCl, 1 NaH2PO4, 25 NaHCO3, 2 MgCl2, 2 CaCl2, 10 Glucose. During recordings, slices were perfused with oxygenated 34–35°C ASCF. Target neurons in BLA were identified based on the presence of viral GFP reporter. ACSF included 35 µM d,l-2-amino-5-phosphonovaleric acid (APV) and 20 µM 6,7-dinitroquinoxaline-2,3-dione (DNQX) to block ionotropic glutamate receptors, and 50 µM picrotoxin to block ionotropic GABA receptors. Whole-cell recording pipettes (6–8 MΩ) were filled with internal solution containing (in mM): 100 K-gluconate, 20 KCl, 10 HEPES, 4 Mg-ATP, 0.3 Na-GTP, 10 Na-phosphocreatine, and 0.1% biocytin. Recordings were amplified (Multiclamp 700B, Molecular Devices) and digitized at 10 kHz using a National Instruments Board under control of IGOR Pro (WaveMetrics). Resting membrane potentials were adjusted to −70 mV and steady state series resistance was compensated. Series resistance and input resistance were calculated using −5 mV (voltage clamp) or 25 pA (current clamp) seal tests before each trial of recording. Measurements of input resistance in Fos reporter labeled neurons (Figure 7) were measured in voltage clamp, all other recordings were performed in current clamp. The calculated liquid junction potential (−10 mV) was compensated post hoc. Neurons with high series resistance (>30 MΩ current clamp) or membrane potentials that changed by >10 mV were excluded. Hyperpolarization activated sag was measured from responses to −100 pA current steps. Action potential (AP) threshold, amplitude, afterhyperpolarization (AHP) and full width at half-height were averaged from the 5th-10th APs in trials with 10 to 20 Hz firing rates. AP threshold is the membrane potential at which the slope first exceeds 10 V/s, and AP amplitude was measured relative to threshold. Sag ratio is defined as the fraction by which the membrane potential depolarized at steady-state from its maximum hyperpolarization during a −100 pA current step. Medium AHP was measured as the peak hyperpolarization after the APs mentioned above relative to threshold. The slow AHP was measured from the peak hyperpolarization following positive current steps generating 10–20 Hz firing.
In vivo recording of GC taste responses
Surgery
Request a detailed protocolStk11f/f mice were anesthetized and prepared for stereotaxic surgery and viral infection as above. Each mouse was also implanted bilaterally with multi-channel electrode bundles (16 formvar-coated, 25 µm diameter nichrome wires) in GC (Distance from Bregma: AP=+1.2 mm; ML = ± 3 mm; DV of −2.25 mm from the pia mater) and a single intraoral cannula (IOC; flexible plastic tubing) was inserted into the cheek to allow controlled delivery of taste stimuli. 24 hr before recording sessions began electrode bundles were then further lowered by 0.75–1.00 mm to reach ventral GC (see Figure 5A).
IOC fluid delivery protocol
Request a detailed protocolExperiments began with three days of habituation to the recording setup and to receiving liquid through the IOC. Sixty 15 µl aliquots (hereafter, ‘trials’) of water were delivered across 30 min. To ensure adequate hydration, mice were given two 30 min period of access to additional water.
On the following day, recording commenced and water trials were replaced with four different taste stimuli: sweet (0.2 M sucrose), salty (0.1 M sodium chloride), sour (0.02 M citric acid), and bitter (0.001 M quinine). A total of 15 trials were delivered for each taste in random order. These tastes and concentrations were chosen because they provided a broad range of hedonic values for palatability assessment. Fluid delivery through a nitrogen-pressurized system of polyethylene tubes was controlled by solenoid valves via a Raspberry Pi computer (construction details and code available on request from [https://github.com/narendramukherjee/blech_clust] Mukherjee et al., 2020; copy archived at https://github.com/elifesciences-publications/blech_clust]).
Taste palatability coding
Request a detailed protocolTo determine whether a neuron displays palatability activity, we performed a moving window analysis (window size: 250 ms; step size: 25 ms) to trace the dynamics of taste processing in GC. For each time window, we calculated a Spearman product-moment correlation between the ranked firing rates to each taste and the palatability rankings obtained previously in separate experiments (Levitan et al., 2019).
Research resource identifiers (RRIDs)
Software
TopHat, RRID:SCR_013035;
Samtools, RRID:SCR_002105; IGV, RRID:SCR_011793; IGOR Pro, RRID:SCR_000325; Fiji, RRID:SCR_002285.
Antibodies
FOS-Santa Cruz Biotechnology Cat# sc-52, RRID:AB_2106783.
FOS- Santa Cruz Biotechnology Cat# sc-166940, RRID:AB_10609634.
STK11-Millipore Cat# 09–495, RRID:AB_10807178;
STK11-Sigma-Aldrich Cat# SAB4502888 RRID:AB_10746328
Alexa Fluor 647 Goat Anti-Rabbit IgG (H+L)- Thermo Fisher Scientific Cat# A-21245, RRID:AB_2535813.
Viruses
AAV-CaMKIIa-GFP-Cre; AAV-CaMKIIa-EYFP and AAV-CamkIIa-hM3Dq-mCherry, (AAV5)-UNC Joint Vector Laboratories, RRID:SCR_002448.
Mice strains
Request a detailed protocolWild type-RRID:IMSR_JAX:000664; YFP-H (B6.Cg-Tg(Thy1-YFP)HJrs/J)- RRID:IMSR_JAX:003782;
Stk11f/f (B6(Cg)-Stk11tm1.1Sjm/J, Lkb1fl)- RRID:IMSR_JAX:014143.
Fosf/f (B6;129-Fostm1Mxu/Mmjax)- RRID:MMRRC_037115-JAX.
Fos-tTA (B6.Cg-Tg(Fos-tTA,Fos-EGFP*)1Mmay/J)- RRID:IMSR_JAX:018306.
Statistical analysis
Request a detailed protocolThe results are expressed as means ± s.e.m unless otherwise stated. All effects were evaluated using either paired t-test or one- or two-way ANOVA test with post hoc t-tests corrected (Bonferroni) for all possible pair-wise comparisons. Two-way mixed ANOVAs were performed using a mixture of between-subjects and repeated-measures factors depending on the design (e.g. repeated measures for multiple current injections to the same neurons in Figures 6 and 7). No power analysis was performed and numbers of replicates performed were the minimum needed to demonstrate reproducibility, consistent with practices in similar published studies.
Data availability
Sequencing data was uploaded to GEO (accession number: GSE138522).
-
NCBI Gene Expression OmnibusID GSE138522. Cell type-specific RNA-seq to profile transcriptional changes in sorted BLA projection neurons during conditioned taste aversion learning.
References
-
The role of protein phosphorylation in the gustatory cortex and amygdala during taste learningExperimental Neurobiology 21:37–51.https://doi.org/10.5607/en.2012.21.2.37
-
Transcription factors in long-term memory and synaptic plasticityPhysiological Reviews 89:121–145.https://doi.org/10.1152/physrev.00017.2008
-
LKB1-dependent signaling pathwaysAnnual Review of Biochemistry 75:137–163.https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.biochem.75.103004.142702
-
Coding in the mammalian gustatory systemTrends in Neurosciences 33:326–334.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2010.04.002
-
A non-synaptic mechanism of complex learning: modulation of intrinsic neuronal excitabilityNeurobiology of Learning and Memory 154:30–36.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2017.11.015
-
Narp regulates homeostatic scaling of excitatory synapses on parvalbumin-expressing interneuronsNature Neuroscience 13:1090–1097.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.2621
-
Plasticity of intrinsic neuronal excitabilityCurrent Opinion in Neurobiology 54:73–82.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2018.09.001
-
A secretory-type protein, containing a pentraxin domain, interacts with an A-type K+ channelThe Journal of Biological Chemistry 280:.https://doi.org/10.1074/jbc.M500111200
-
Basolateral amygdala glutamatergic activation enhances taste aversion through NMDA receptor activation in the insular cortexEuropean Journal of Neuroscience 22:2596–2604.https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04440.x
-
Plasticity of dendritic excitabilityJournal of Neurobiology 64:100–115.https://doi.org/10.1002/neu.20148
-
Consolidation and translation regulationLearning & Memory 19:410–422.https://doi.org/10.1101/lm.026849.112
-
Role of glutamate receptors of central and basolateral amygdala nuclei on retrieval and reconsolidation of taste aversive memoryNeurobiology of Learning and Memory 111:35–40.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2014.03.003
-
Laminar- and Target-Specific amygdalar inputs in Rat primary gustatory cortexThe Journal of Neuroscience 36:2623–2637.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3224-15.2016
-
Thy1-expressing neurons in the basolateral amygdala may mediate fear inhibitionJournal of Neuroscience 33:10396–10404.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5539-12.2013
-
Inducible repression of CREB function disrupts amygdala-dependent memoryNeurobiology of Learning and Memory 82:159–163.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2004.05.008
-
Dynamic and multimodal responses of gustatory cortical neurons in awake ratsThe Journal of Neuroscience 21:4478–4489.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-12-04478.2001
-
Antagonistic negative and positive neurons of the basolateral amygdalaNature Neuroscience 19:1636–1646.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4414
-
Single and population coding of taste in the gustatory cortex of awake miceJournal of Neurophysiology 122:1342–1356.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00357.2019
-
Lateral hypothalamus contains two types of palatability-related taste responses with distinct dynamicsThe Journal of Neuroscience 33:9462–9473.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3935-12.2013
-
The effects of amygdala and cortical inactivation on taste neophobiaNeurobiology of Learning and Memory 155:322–329.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2018.08.021
-
Amygdala-gustatory insular cortex connections and taste neophobiaBehavioural Brain Research 235:182–188.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2012.07.040
-
Neural processing of gustatory information in insular circuitsCurrent Opinion in Neurobiology 22:709–716.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2012.04.001
-
Genome-wide identification and characterization of functional neuronal activity–dependent enhancersNature Neuroscience 17:1330–1339.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3808
-
In search of cellular mechanisms of memoryProgress in Neurobiology 32:277–349.https://doi.org/10.1016/0301-0082(89)90024-5
-
Exploring memory representations with Activity-Based geneticsCold Spring Harbor Perspectives in Biology 8:a021832.https://doi.org/10.1101/cshperspect.a021832
-
Effects of lesions in different nuclei of the amygdala on conditioned taste aversionExperimental Brain Research 235:3517–3526.https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-017-5078-1
-
More than synaptic plasticity: role of nonsynaptic plasticity in learning and memoryTrends in Neurosciences 33:17–26.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2009.10.001
-
Genetically induced cholinergic hyper-innervation enhances taste learningFrontiers in Systems Neuroscience 5:97.https://doi.org/10.3389/fnsys.2011.00097
-
Arc 3′ UTR Splicing Leads to Dual and Antagonistic Effects in Fine-Tuning Arc Expression Upon BDNF SignalingFrontiers in Molecular Neuroscience 11:145.https://doi.org/10.3389/fnmol.2018.00145
-
Sodium concentration coding gives way to evaluative coding in cortex and amygdalaJournal of Neuroscience 32:9999–10011.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6059-11.2012
-
LKB1 and AMPK regulate synaptic remodeling in old ageNature Neuroscience 17:1190–1197.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3772
-
Fiji: an open-source platform for biological-image analysisNature Methods 9:676–682.https://doi.org/10.1038/nmeth.2019
-
The LKB1-AMPK pathway: metabolism and growth control in tumour suppressionNature Reviews Cancer 9:563–575.https://doi.org/10.1038/nrc2676
-
Mechanisms of cardiac potassium channel traffickingThe Journal of Physiology 582:17–26.https://doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.2007.130245
-
Molecular taxonomy of major neuronal classes in the adult mouse forebrainNature Neuroscience 9:99–107.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1618
-
Memory Engram storage and retrievalCurrent Opinion in Neurobiology 35:101–109.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2015.07.009
-
c-fos regulates neuronal excitability and survivalNature Genetics 30:416–420.https://doi.org/10.1038/ng859
-
The other side of the Engram: experience-driven changes in neuronal intrinsic excitabilityNature Reviews Neuroscience 4:885–900.https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1248
Article and author information
Author details
Funding
National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders (DC006666)
- Donald B Katz
National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (NS109916)
- Sacha B Nelson
The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.
Ethics
Animal experimentation: All procedures were approved by the Brandeis University Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (IACUC, Protocol #20002) in accordance with NIH guidelines.
Copyright
© 2020, Levitan et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
-
- 1,487
- views
-
- 198
- downloads
-
- 10
- citations
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.
Download links
Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)
Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)
Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)
Further reading
-
- Neuroscience
Rats are believed to communicate their emotional state by emitting two distinct types of ultrasonic vocalizations. The first is long ‘22-kHz’ vocalizations (>300 ms, <32-kHz) with constant frequency, signaling aversive states, and the second is short ‘50-kHz’ calls (<150 ms, >32 kHz), often frequency-modulated, in appetitive situations. Here, we describe aversive vocalizations emitted at a higher pitch by male Wistar and spontaneously hypertensive rats (SHR) in an intensified aversive state – prolonged fear conditioning. These calls, which we named ‘44-kHz’ vocalizations, are long (>150 ms), generally at a constant frequency (usually within 35–50-kHz range) and have an overall spectrographic image similar to 22-kHz calls. Some 44-kHz vocalizations are comprised of both 22-kHz-like and 44-kHz-like elements. Furthermore, two separate clustering methods confirmed that these 44-kHz calls can be separated from other vocalizations. We observed 44-kHz calls to be associated with freezing behavior during fear conditioning training, during which they constituted up to 19.4% of all calls and most of them appeared next to each other forming uniform groups of vocalizations (bouts). We also show that some of rats’ responses to the playback of 44-kHz calls were more akin to that of aversive calls, for example, heart rate changes, whereas other responses were at an intermediate level between aversive and appetitive calls. Our results suggest that rats have a wider vocal repertoire than previously believed, and current definitions of major call types may require reevaluation. We hope that future investigations of 44-kHz calls in rat models of human diseases will contribute to expanding our understanding and therapeutic strategies related to human psychiatric conditions.
-
- Neuroscience
Unsupervised segmentation in biological and non-biological images is only partially resolved. Segmentation either requires arbitrary thresholds or large teaching datasets. Here, we propose a spatial autocorrelation method based on Local Moran’s I coefficient to differentiate signal, background, and noise in any type of image. The method, originally described for geoinformatics, does not require a predefined intensity threshold or teaching algorithm for image segmentation and allows quantitative comparison of samples obtained in different conditions. It utilizes relative intensity as well as spatial information of neighboring elements to select spatially contiguous groups of pixels. We demonstrate that Moran’s method outperforms threshold-based method in both artificially generated as well as in natural images especially when background noise is substantial. This superior performance can be attributed to the exclusion of false positive pixels resulting from isolated, high intensity pixels in high noise conditions. To test the method’s power in real situation, we used high power confocal images of the somatosensory thalamus immunostained for Kv4.2 and Kv4.3 (A-type) voltage-gated potassium channels in mice. Moran’s method identified high-intensity Kv4.2 and Kv4.3 ion channel clusters in the thalamic neuropil. Spatial distribution of these clusters displayed strong correlation with large sensory axon terminals of subcortical origin. The unique association of the special presynaptic terminals and a postsynaptic voltage-gated ion channel cluster was confirmed with electron microscopy. These data demonstrate that Moran’s method is a rapid, simple image segmentation method optimal for variable and high noise conditions.