Optogenetic inhibition-mediated activity-dependent modification of CA1 pyramidal-interneuron connections during behavior
Abstract
In vitro work revealed that excitatory synaptic inputs to hippocampal inhibitory interneurons could undergo Hebbian, associative, or non-associative plasticity. Both behavioral and learning-dependent reorganization of these connections has also been demonstrated by measuring spike transmission probabilities in pyramidal cell-interneuron spike cross-correlations that indicate monosynaptic connections. Here we investigated the activity-dependent modification of these connections during exploratory behavior in rats by optogenetically inhibiting pyramidal cell and interneuron subpopulations. Light application and associated firing alteration of pyramidal and interneuron populations led to lasting changes in pyramidal-interneuron connection weights as indicated by spike transmission changes. Spike transmission alterations were predicted by the light-mediated changes in the number of pre- and postsynaptic spike pairing events and by firing rate changes of interneurons but not pyramidal cells. This work demonstrates the presence of activity-dependent associative and non-associative reorganization of pyramidal-interneuron connections triggered by the optogenetic modification of the firing rate and spike synchrony of cells.
Introduction
It is increasingly recognized that plastic reorganization of brain circuits needed for learning and other cognitive functions involves not only principal cells but also local inhibitory interneurons (Buzsáki, 2010). A large body of work has demonstrated that excitatory connections onto inhibitory interneurons, as well as inhibitory connections on principal cells, are often plastic (Kullmann and Lamsa, 2007; McBain and Kauer, 2009). However, the rules governing the plasticity of excitatory synapses on inhibitory interneurons are not always similar to that targeting other principal cells (Bartos et al., 2011; Lamsa et al., 2007; Pelkey et al., 2017). Even in the CA1 region of the hippocampus, plasticity rules can be different, depending on the experimental conditions and fibers stimulated. Although in the majority of cases some pyramidal-interneuron cell connections show anti-Hebbian non-associative plasticity, others show weight changes that are governed by Hebbian rules (Le Roux et al., 2013; Nissen et al., 2010; Topolnik et al., 2009). In addition, some inhibitory interneuron types in the hippocampus do not seem to possess plastic synapses with their excitatory inputs, such as the CCK cells (Nissen et al., 2010).
In vivo work, primarily during anesthesia, has also demonstrated that plastic alterations can occur between afferent excitatory fibers and CA1 interneurons (Buzsáki and Eidelberg, 1982; Lau et al., 2017). This work showed that stimulation of these fibers could either up- or downregulate evoked spike responses, depending on the interneuron subtype. However, our knowledge about the precise reorganization of pyramidal-interneuron connections during behavior is limited because of the technical challenge of directly performing patch-clamp recordings from monosynaptically connected pyramidal cell-interneuron pairs during such conditions. It is, however, possible to study these connections indirectly by identifying monosynaptically connected pyramidal cell-interneuron pairs by using cross-correlation analysis of the spike timing and measuring spike transmission probability between them (Csicsvari et al., 1998; Csicsvari et al., 2003; Marshall et al., 2002). Early work demonstrated the behavioral state-dependent modulation of such connections (Csicsvari et al., 1998). Moreover, changes in spike transmission probability have been seen in the prefrontal cortex during behavioral tasks (Fujisawa et al., 2008). In the hippocampus, spatial learning can cause lasting changes in these connections (Dupret et al., 2013). While such studies provide strong evidence of plasticity at excitatory-interneuron synapses, so far, no data has established a causal link between pre- and postsynaptic firing to changes in connection strength during behavior.
In this study, we optogenetically interfered with the circuit function by activating Halorhodopsin or Archaerhodopsin in a subpopulation of pyramidal cells and interneurons. This manipulation led to the inhibition of a subset of pyramidal cells and interneurons and also light-triggered disinhibition of many pyramidal cells (Gridchyn et al., 2020; Schoenenberger et al., 2016). Here, we examined whether these light-induced rate changes and the associated network effects could lead to the lasting reorganization of pyramidal-interneuron connection weights, as assessed by monosynaptic spike transmission probabilities.
Results
We recorded multiple unit and field potential activities from the dorsal hippocampus in five rats, during exploration and quiet immobility sessions. In these rats, Halorhodopsin (NpHR-YFP, n = 4 rats) or Archaerhodopsin (ArchT, n = 1 rat) was expressed in the dorsal CA1 region of the hippocampus under the control of the CaMKIIα promoter using an adeno-associated virus (AAV2/1). In four rats, fifteen independently-movable tetrodes and one 200 µm optic fiber centered in the middle of the tetrode bundle targeted the dorsal CA1 region while, in the remaining animal, 24 tetrodes and four optic fibers were used (see Materials and methods). We recorded during four 25 min exploration sessions in which first a familiar environment (FAM1) was explored followed by a novel environment (NOV), and finally, the animals were returned to the familiar environment for the remaining two sessions. During the second familiar exploration session laser stimulation was applied (FAML) in a fixed part of the environment but not in the last exploration (FAM2, see Figure 1A). We tested whether the light application affected the behavior of the animals (Figure 1—figure supplement 1). In all sessions, neither the average speed nor the occupancy within the light stimulation sector were significantly different, compared to the part of the environment where no light was triggered (all p>0.5607). We identified monosynaptically-connected pyramidal cell-interneuron pairs by calculating the cross-correlation of their spike firing times and testing for the presence of a short-latency (1–2 ms) sharp (1–2 ms wide) peak. This peak indicates the presence of a monosynaptic connection in which the presynaptic pyramidal cell can discharge the postsynaptic interneuron within a short latency. In turn, the magnitude of the peak, that is, its transmission probability, reflects the connection weight between a given cell pair. Changes in firing rate across sessions by either or both cells in the pair would result in an alteration of the magnitude of the peak that does not reflect a change in this connection weight, but rather a general change in the probability of joint firing. To account for this, throughout all the analysis, we measured the chance probability that the pair fires together, by averaging the correlation probabilities over the 10–50 ms time bins and subtracting this from the peak. Altogether, we identified 78 pyramidal cell-interneuron pairs in these recordings (see Materials and methods).
The light stimulation inhibited not only a selected population of pyramidal cells but also many interneurons, while a further group of pyramidal cells increased their firing due to disinhibition, as assessed directly by their light responses to brief light pulses in the rest session at the end of the recordings (Figure 1B). In our previous work, we showed that these disinhibited pyramidal cells only increased their firing after the maximum light-mediated suppression on interneurons (Gridchyn et al., 2020; Schoenenberger et al., 2016). We also showed before (Schoenenberger et al., 2016) that both somatostatin- and parvalbumin immunopositive interneurons can express transgenes following AAV-mediated transduction in agreement with earlier work (Nathanson et al., 2009). It is possible, however, that other adeno-associated virus serotypes or the usage of the same virus in other brain regions may yield principal cell-specific expression. As a result, many pyramidal cell - interneuron pairs with monosynaptic connections showed changes in firing rate during the FAML session, when compared to FAM1 (Figure 1C). The altered network activity during the light session is demonstrated by significantly lower correlation of the FAM1 vs FAML rates as compared to rates measured in alternating 5 s time windows within FAM1, both for pyramidal cells and interneurons (all p<0.0001, Z-test). However, no significant differences were found in the median of FAM1 and FAML firing rates (interneuron p=0.3213 pyramidal cell p=0.1448, Mann-Whitney test). The spike transmission probability of these cell pairs was also changed (Figure 1D). As for firing rates, the correlation FAM1 vs FAML spike transmission values was lower than that measured in alternating 5 s time windows within FAM1 (p<0.0001, Z-test). Moreover, there was a significant reduction in the median of the spike transmission probabilities from FAM1 to FAML (p<0.01. Mann-Whitney test). Because in the spike transmission measurements the chance probability that cells randomly fire together was compensated for, light-induced network modifications altered the ability of the pyramidal cell to drive the postsynaptic interneuron, beyond that of the firing rate alterations-mediated changes. Changes in connection weight between cell pairs during the FAML session could be either transient or reflect longer-term plasticity that outlasts optogenetic stimulation. Moreover, connection strength could change as place cells remap their place fields during exploration of a different environment (Wilson and McNaughton, 1993). We, therefore, tested whether significant changes in the spike transmission of monosynaptic pairs can be seen across sessions, relative to the baseline identified in FAM1 (Figure 2A–B). To do this, we generated a score that represented the absolute value of normalized spike transmission differences between sessions (difference divided by the sum, see Materials and methods). Overall, this score was the largest between NOV-FAM1 and FAML-FAM1 sessions. However, while the changes between FAM2-FAM1 sessions were about 30% weaker relative to the others, they were still significantly larger than zero (all p<0.001; ANOVA). In addition, changes between FAM2-FAM1 sessions were significantly larger than changes within FAM1 sessions as assessed by correlations measured in alternating 5 s time windows (p<0.0268, F-test), independent of the variability across animals (p=0. 0562, Likelihood-ratio test).
These population changes suggest that pyramidal cell-interneuron synaptic weights were, to some degree, reorganized by both exposure to a new environment and artificially by light stimulation. However, connection strengths were different between the first and last exposure to the same familiar environment (FAM1 and FAM2), indicating that a more lasting change had also occurred. We set out to determine the factors that accounted for the connection strength observed in FAM2 by testing at the population level whether spike transmission in FAM2 was predicted by that observed in the previous sessions. In addition, we controlled for the possible variability across animals by including animal identity as a variable into the analysis (Figure 2C–D). Spike transmissions in FAM2 were strongly predicted by the observed connection weights in all the previous sessions (all p<0.0001, F-test, Figure 2C), indicating that connection strength between the pairs was only partially reorganized. The variability across animals did not significantly account for variability in the spike transmission (all p>0.5656, Likelihood-ratio test). We then used a linear model comparison to reveal which behavioral sessions best explained the monosynaptic connection strengths in FAM2 (Figure 2D). We found that both FAM1 and FAML predicted FAM2 spike transmission, independent of the other (all p<0.0105, F-test). However, the model comparison showed that the NOV no longer predicted FAM2 when the effect of FAM1 was taken into consideration (p=0.4453, F-test) nor the variation across animals contributed (all p>0.5656, Likelihood-ratio test). Thus, the changes in connection weights during NOV did not influence the subsequent weights observed in FAM2, which instead was explained by weights in both FAM1 and FAML, independently. Therefore, while spike transmission values were more similar within the same environmental context, light application significantly biased spike transmission and caused lasting changes in FAM2 relative to FAM1.
The significant influence of the FAML session on FAM2 suggests that the light-induced alterations of the network activity led to lasting changes in the spike transmission probabilities even in the absence of light in the same environmental context. Direct optogenetic inhibition of some cells and the indirect firing increase of others led to either an increase or a decrease of spike transmission probabilities during the light application. Therefore, next, we tested whether the direction of change in spike transmission in FAML relative to FAM1 predicted similar change from FAM1 to FAM2 (Figure 2E–F). Indeed, those cell pairs in which light application led to a decrease in spike transmission relative to FAM1 also maintained weaker spike transmission values in FAM2, while cell pairs with light-enhanced transmission showed a persistent increase in transmission probability in FAM2 (all p<0.0001, F-test). Moreover, the relative change of spike transmission (difference divided by the sum) between FAML-FAM1 predicted the score change from FAM1 to FAM2 (p<0.0001, F-test), indicating that larger relative changes in spike transmission between FAML-FAM1 were accompanied by similarly larger changes between FAM2-FAM1. Thus, light-induced changes in neuronal firing during FAML may lead to lasting modifications of spike transmission. The variability across animals did not significantly account for variability in the transmission probability change (p>0.3046, Likelihood-ratio test).
The light application can change the firing rate of the presynaptic pyramidal cell or the postsynaptic interneuron, which may, in turn, account for the observed plastic changes in transmission probability. Therefore, to address whether our effects reflected a pre- or postsynaptic mechanism, we assessed the relationship between firing rate changes within connected pairs and the modification of their monosynaptic connection. To do this, we calculated the relative firing rate changes between FAML-FAM1 for both pyramidal cells and interneurons, a measure that reflects the influence of light on the baseline firing of these cells. We then analyzed whether this measure predicted changes in transmission probabilities between FAM1 and FAM2, which reflects the longer-lasting change in synaptic strength. As a control, we also measured relative firing rate changes between FAM2-FAM1 because rate alterations reflecting the alterations in the average excitatory inputs cells received. Such changes in excitability may have influenced spike transmission beyond the rate alteration-mediated changes of the chance joint firing probability, which later were already compensated for by normalizing the histograms (Figure 3A–B). We found that interneuron rate changes of both FAML-FAM1 and FAM2-FAM1 influenced FAM2-FAM1 spike transmission changes, even when each other’s influence was taken into account (all p<0.0064, Likelihood-ratio test). However, pyramidal cell rate changes during FAML no longer significantly influenced FAM2-FAM1 spike transmission changes when FAM2-FAM1 rate change was taken into account (p=0.0999, Likelihood-ratio test). This suggests that changes in the excitability of interneurons during the light application, as assessed by rate changes, influenced the spike transmission strength subsequently in FAM2 even when the FAM2-FAM1 rate alterations of the interneuron were compensated for. In these models, the variability across animals did not significantly account for variability in the transmission probability change (all p>0.0907, Likelihood-ratio test). In addition to the changing firing rate, the light application can cause remapping in a subpopulation of cells (Schoenenberger et al., 2016). However, a change in spike transmission in FAM2 did not predict the degree of pyramidal place field remapping (p=0.1612, F-test).
The finding that interneuron rate change between FAM2-FAM1 itself independently influenced FAM2-FAM1 spike transmission changes suggests that the excitability of the postsynaptic interneuron has a strong influence on the strength of spike transmission. Spike transmission changes we detected in the NOV session may, in part, have been caused by the excitability change of the interneurons. To test whether rate changes of pyramidal cells or interneurons in NOV influenced spike transmission in FAM2, we calculated normalized (difference divided by the sum) spike transmission and rate changes, in order to predict weight change separately for both cell types (Figure 4). As before, only interneuron rate changes predicted spike transmission changes, even when the pyramidal rate change was taken into consideration (interneuron: p<0.0001; pyramidal: p=0.1672, Likelihood-ratio test). A similar analysis was performed for the FAML session itself, where again, we found that only interneuron rate alterations predicted spike transmission alterations during the light application, even when each-others’ contribution was considered (interneuron: p<0.0001; pyramidal: p=0.5885, Likelihood-ratio test). In these models, the variability across animals did not significantly account for variability in the transmission probability changes in NOV-FAM1 and FAML-FAM1 (all p>0.382, Likelihood-ratio test).
Next, we examined whether the direction of firing rate change during light application influenced the spike transmission change between FAM1 and FAM2 (Figure 3C–D). As before, we considered FAM1 as a baseline and generated a normalized score for the rate and weight change. Interneurons that exhibited elevated or reduced firing rate during FAML session exhibited significantly different changes in monosynaptic spike transmission probability across FAM2 and FAM1 (p<0.0001, F-test) independent of the variability across animals (p=0.3372, Likelihood-ratio test). Pyramidal cells did not show such a relationship (p=0.4499, F-test) and the variability across animals did not influence this result (p=0.1359, Likelihood-ratio test). Therefore, suppressed interneurons tended to weaken their monosynaptic weights with presynaptic pyramidal cells, whereas those that increased their rate exhibited increased weights. These effects lasted after the light application in the same environmental context. To confirm that the observed changes in monosynaptic weight were indeed mediated by a postsynaptic change in the interneuron firing rate, we differentiated monosynaptic cell pairs into four groups, according to whether the pair exhibited a rate increase or decrease, of both pyramidal cells and interneurons (Figure 5). A two-way ANOVA analysis showed that the interneuron increase and decrease groups were significantly different, independent of the pyramidal increase and decrease as a factor (p<0.0004, F-test), but not the pyramidal group (p=0.4067, F-test), and the variability across animals did not influence the result (p=0.2749, Likelihood-ratio test) and no significant interactions were seen between cell pair groups (p=0.6109, F-test).
We found that the modulation of interneuron activity during light stimulation or exposure to a novel environment directly influenced changes in transmission probabilities between putative monosynaptic connections of pyramidal-interneuron cell pairs. This raises the possibility that such changes are activity-dependent. To examine this, we measured the number of instances in which pyramidal cell firing occurred in 10 ms, 20 ms, 50 ms, and 100 ms time windows before or after the interneuron spike as well as their sum for all spike-pairing events (Figure 6). The relative change (difference/sum) of spike pairing events was calculated between FAML and FAM1 and these spike pairing changes predicted significantly FAM2-FAM1 spike transmission change in all three cases for 50 ms window (p<0.00099, F-test) even when multiple testing correction was performed while the variability across animals did not influence the result (all p>0.3913, Likelihood-ratio test). In the other tested time intervals, the correlations were not significant (all p>0.0517, F-test). Moreover, spike pairing event number no longer predicted the FAM2-FAM1 spike transmission changes when interneuron and pyramidal changes were together taken into account, (all p>0.417, F-test). However, spike pairing itself was strongly predicted (all R2>0.859) by pyramidal and interneuron firing rates, explaining why spike transmission changes could not be predicted independently from the combined interneuron and pyramidal rates by spike pairing.
Discussion
Here we showed that light-induced, optogenetic alterations of the CA1 network activity can trigger lasting alterations of the monosynaptic spike transmission probability of pyramidal-cell interneuron pairs. During the light session, both changes in the postsynaptic interneuron rates and the pairing probability of cell firing predicted the changes in monosynaptic spike transmission within the same familiar environment, when sessions before and after the light interference were compared. In addition, we observed spike transmission changes in the novel environment relative to the familiar environment, which took place before the light application. These changes were specific to the novel environment, however, and were not maintained during the subsequent familiar sessions. This suggests that altered interneuron firing rate and the activity-dependent alterations of the pyramidal-interneuron spike pairing can modify pyramidal-interneuron connection weights during exploratory behavior. Our study did not use control animals in which only YFP was expressed. Therefore, we cannot exclude the possibility that optogenetic channel expression, or, perhaps, light application enhanced the plasticity on pyramidal-interneuron synapses. Yet, we observed similar activity-dependent changes during spatial learning before (Dupret et al., 2013). So, it is likely that the optogenetic, light-mediated rate alteration was a primary driver of the activity-dependent, lasting connection weight changes.
We used a measure of spike transmission probability that compensated for the changes in the chance probability that the two cells fire together as a result of firing rate alterations. However, the average depolarization level of the cell, as reflected by its mean firing rate, can lead to more efficient spike transmission, even without changes in the synaptic weight. Indeed, the spike transmission changes from FAM1 to FAM2 were influenced by the rate changes of the postsynaptic interneuron between these sessions. In turn, this suggests that the postsynaptic interneuron’s general level of depolarization can influence spike transmission. However, in a similar manner, changes in FAML-FAM1 interneuron rate also predicted FAM2-FAM1 spike transmission changes. Indeed, when FAML interneuron rate increased, a stronger spike transmission was seen subsequently in FAM2, while reduced spike transmission was associated with reduced interneuron rate. Considering that the interneuron rate increase in FAML was not directly mediated by the light, we cannot exclude the possibility that stronger pyramidal inputs caused the interneuron rate increase in FAML that is caused by the plastic strengthening of these connections. Nevertheless, the excitability of interneurons in FAM2 alone, which may be indicative of plasticity-mediated input to interneurons, did not explain the spike transmission changes. Indeed, rate changes in FAML could predict spike transmission changes in FAM2-FAM1 independent of FAM2-FAM1 rate changes. That is, FAML-FAM1 rate changes predicted FAM2-FAM1 spike transmission changes even when the FAM2-FAM1 interneuron rate changes were accounted for. Therefore, FAML-FAM1 interneuron rate changes had further predictive value beyond those observed by FAM2-FAM1 rates changes and consequently excitability/depolarization alterations in FAML that were no longer present in FAM2 still predicted FAM2-FAM1 spike transmission changes. This finding indicates that interneuronal excitability changes during light application session caused lasting changes of the pyramidal interneuron connections, beyond any lasting nonspecific excitability changes that occurred between FAM2-FAM1.
We observed changes in spike transmission between FAM1 and the novel environment, which were larger in amplitude than those across the familiar environment before and after the light application. Exposure to the novel environment nevertheless did not influence changes in the familiar environment. Can we expect that pyramidal-interneuron weights change from one environment to another but they revert to the previous configuration when the animal is returned to the first environment? Although we cannot exclude this possibility, it is more likely that the average depolarization level of each interneuron is different across different environments, which in turn reveals different monosynaptic connections and connection strengths. Interneuron rates reorganize across the familiar and novel environment, which may reflect the influence of different nonspecific neurotransmitters (Nitz and McNaughton, 2004; Wilson and McNaughton, 1993). Moreover, spike transmission changes across the familiar and novel environments were predicted by the rate changes of interneurons but not pyramidal cells. This suggests that postsynaptic effects such as differences in interneuron depolarization levels contributed to the changes in spike transmission between different environments. In addition to nonspecific neuromodulator transmitters, presynaptic place cells that were specifically active in the novel environment may have caused changes in the interneuron depolarization as well. Although novel environments may not entirely reorganize pyramidal connections, spatial learning is able to do so (Dupret et al., 2013). During the course of spatial learning, some interneurons increase their rates while others decrease, which are accompanied by changes in spike transmission probabilities of monosynaptic pairs. However, these spike transmission changes depended on both pyramidal and interneuron rate changes. One common aspect of the light and the spatial learning-mediated monosynaptic spike transmission changes is that, in both cases, it took place in a familiar environment in which some of the place cells remapped their place fields. In Dupret et al., 2013 paradigm, some cells altered their place fields to represent the changed goal locations while in our paradigm, some of the place cells whose in-field firing was inhibited by the light remapped their place fields (Schoenenberger et al., 2016).
Can rules derived from in vitro observations, or those seen in vivo during anesthesia through afferent stimulations explain our findings during behavior? Our interneurons were recorded in the CA1 pyramidal layer where Ca++ permeable AMPA receptors mediate the primary form of LTP and LTD. This form of plasticity requires the stimulation of their synaptic inputs and the hyperpolarization or a non-depolarized state of the interneuron (Le Roux et al., 2013; Nissen et al., 2010). In addition, metabotropic glutamate receptors can further regulate dendritic Ca++ levels and the direction of synaptic plasticity (Camiré and Topolnik, 2014). In our case, the firing rate alteration of the interneurons was the strongest predictor of spike transmission change. The firing rate of the interneuron during FAML may reflect both the afferent excitation level of the cell as well as the light-induced inhibition; both of which could contribute to plastic changes. Excitability changes caused by the light stimulation are also expected to contribute to our observed results because both FAML-FAM1 and FAM2-FAM1 rate changes independently predicted FAM2-FAM1 spike transmission changes. It has been shown that Schaffer collateral stimulation enhances the excitability of CA1 PV cells following the stimulation, mediated via mGluR5 receptors (Campanac et al., 2013). In our dataset, the majority of interneurons showed a reduction in firing rate. Therefore, interneurons may be able to undergo both up- and downregulation of their excitation levels, which are not exclusively controlled by Schaffer collateral inputs.
We also saw that spike pairing of the pyramidal cells and interneuron in 50 ms time windows weakly influenced spike transmission, independent of the light-induced rate changes of these cells. Indeed, parvalbumin-positive CA1 interneurons exhibit NMDA-dependent associative plasticity as well (Le Roux et al., 2013) on their feedback connections from CA1 pyramidal cells. This may explain our spike pairing results. In vivo work suggested that theta-frequency afferent stimulation was optimal to induce LTP or LTD-like changes (Lau et al., 2017). Our light application occurred during theta oscillations, where such afferents would indeed provide theta-rhythmic stimulation of the CA1 interneurons. However, such a pairing relationship was observed only for a 50 ms time window. Moreover, it was no longer significant when pyramidal and interneuron rate changes were together taken into account. Because our optogenetic manipulation altered rates of individual cells without specifically influencing pyramidal-interneuron spike pairings, the combination of pyramidal and interneuron rates strongly predicted spike pairing probability. This can explain why spike pairing no longer predicted spike transmission changes when these rates were taken into account. After all, this result shows that rate-predicted spike pairing numbers are as good as the real ones to predict spike transmission changes. Nevertheless, we cannot exclude the possibility that the independent rate alterations of pyramidal cells and interneurons in FAML governed spike transmission probability changes, without spike pairing itself directly influencing it. Future work in which interneuron (or a certain genetic type) firing rate is selectively altered by optogenetics may provide further evidence for the independent contribution of postsynaptic interneuron depolarization in plasticity. Nevertheless, even in such manipulations, indirect alteration of pyramidal rates (e.g., because of disinhibition) is expected to occur.
Overall, our data indicate that during active behavior, changes in interneuron excitability that is coupled with spike pairing or altered presynaptic pyramidal spiking during theta epochs may trigger plasticity at the excitatory inputs to CA1 interneurons. Learning and the associated reorganization of the CA1 network may be a condition where such changes occur naturally.
Materials and methods
This study used previously published electrophysiological spike data (Schoenenberger et al., 2016). Accordingly, the experimental and spike clustering work has been described in this previous work in detail. Data from one additional rat recorded in the same paradigm and analyzed using the same methods was included in the data set.
Surgery for virus injection and microdrive implantation
Request a detailed protocolFour male adult rats (Long Evans, 300–500 g) were injected with a recombinant adeno-associated virus to express Halorhodopsin-YFP in the dorsal CA1 area (AAV2/1.CaMKIIα::eNpHR3.0-YFP Zhang et al., 2007, obtained from the Penn Vector Core facility, 1.6 × 1013 genome copies/mL; Addgene 26971) and the remaining animal was injected with a recombinant adeno-associated virus to express Archaerhodopsin (ArchT) in the dorsal CA1 area (AAV2/1.CaMKII::ArchT.GFP.WPRE.SV40 (Boyden et al., 2005), obtained from the Penn Vector Core facility, 6.41*1012 genome copies per mL). The virus was injected at four sites into dorsal CA1 of the right hemisphere in four rats and bilaterally in one rat: site 1: −3.0 AP, ±2.2 L, 2.1 DV; site 2: −3.7 AP, ±2.9 L, 2.0 DV; site 3: −4.3 AP, ±3.5 L, 2.0 DV; site 4: −5.0 AP, ±4.2 L, 2.2 DV. 3.5 weeks after virus injection, animals were implanted with 15 (28 in one rat) independently movable wire-tetrodes under deep anesthesia using isoflurane (0.5–2%), oxygen (1–2 L/min) and an initial dose of buprenorphine (0.1 mg/kg). Tetrodes were attached to the 15-tetrode (24-tetrode and 4-octrode in one rat) microdrive assemblies, enabling their independent movement. The tetrodes were constructed from four individual tungsten wires, 12 µm in diameter (H-Formvar insulation with Butyral bond coat, California Fine Wire, Grover Beach CA), twisted and then heated to bind them into a single bundle. The tips were then gold plated to reduce their impedance to 200–300 kΩ.
A 200 μm/0.48 NA optic fiber stub (Doric Lenses) located in the center of the tetrode array was used to apply laser light directly to the dorsal CA1 area. During surgery, a craniotomy was prepared above the dorsal hippocampus centered at AP = −4.0; ML = ± 3.0. Two stainless steel screws inserted through the skull above the cerebellum served as ground and reference electrodes, and six additional screws were used to permanently attach the microdrive assembly to the skull. Implantation was performed such as to position the tip of the optic fiber at a depth of 1.7 mm. The paraffin-wax coated electrodes and microdrives were then daubed with bone cement to encase the electrode-microdrive assembly and anchor it to the screws in the skull. Following a recovery period of 7 days, the tetrodes were lowered to their target locations over a further period of around 7 d. Tetrode locations were identified by electrophysiological markers such as theta band power, sharp wave polarity, and the presence of ripple oscillations, and by extrapolating the location of the electrodes by tracing the distances back along each electrode tract according to the daily advancement of the recorded electrodes. Implanted animals were housed individually in a separate room under a 12 hr light/12 hr dark cycle with ad libitum access to water, and they were maintained in a food-deprived state between 85–90% (plus an incremental 5 g per week) of their post-operative weight. Experiments were performed during the light phase. All rats used in this study were naïve and not used for additional procedures before surgery.
All procedures involving experimental animals were carried out in accordance with Austrian (Austrian Federal Law for experiments with live animals) animal law under a project license (BMBWF-66.018/0015-WF/V/3b/2014, BMBWF-66.018/0018-WF/V/3b/2019) approved by the Austrian Federal Science Ministry (BMWFW).
Data acquisition
Request a detailed protocol32-channel unity-gain preamplifier panels (Axona Ltd, St Albans, Hertfordshire, UK) were used to reduce cable movement artifacts. Wide-band (0.1/1 Hz – 5 kHz) recordings were taken, and the amplified local field potential and multiple-unit activity were continuously digitized at 24 kHz using a 128-channel data acquisition system (Axona Ltd, St Albans, Hertfordshire, UK). Two red LEDs mounted on the preamplifier headstage were used to track the location of the animal.
Green/yellow laser light for NpHR activation was provided by a 561 nm DPSS laser system equipped with an acousto-optic modulator (Omicron). The light was coupled into an optic fiber (four optic fibers in one rat) connected to a fiber-optic rotary joint (Doric lenses) from where a 200 μm/0.48 NA patch cord transmitted the light to the microdrive. Laser intensity was set to reach 25 mW total power at the tip of every implanted fiber stub. Data were recorded 6–7 weeks after AAV injection to ensure sufficient NpHR-YFP/ArchT GFP expression levels.
Behavioral paradigms
Request a detailed protocolData was recorded while the animals explored different arenas or rested in a sleep box. The sleep box was small (20 cm × 27 cm) with 60 cm high walls and cushioned with a terry towel for the animal to sleep/rest comfortably. During training and electrode positioning, the animals were familiarized with a 120 cm circular environment with 20 cm high walls (minimum of 60 min of exposure per day for at least seven days) that served as the familiar arena in all experiments. Curtains were used to enclose this arena and provide a stable set of external cues. In all exploration sessions, small food pellets were dropped at random from an automated overhead system (2–3/min) to motivate the animals to explore the entire arena. For recordings in a novel environment, several other arenas with different sizes, shapes, and textures were used. In addition, curtains were opened to provide novel distal room cues.
Typical recording days consisted of 10 sessions: four exploration sessions flanked by five sleep sessions and a final test session where brief laser pulses were applied while the animal still rested in the sleep box. Typically, sleep and exploration sessions lasted 25 min, whereas the laser test session lasted 18 min. The animals first explored the familiar arena. After visiting a different novel arena, the familiar arena was explored again, but laser illumination was automatically triggered when the animal entered a specific part of the arena (light zone). Finally, the same arena was explored again. All exploration sessions were flanked by sleep. The light zone was defined by a center position and an angle between 120° and 180° such that it covered one-third to half of the arena. The initial angle defining the illumination zone was random and thus random also with respect to the hippocampal place fields. Every day, a novel illumination zone that had about 50% overlap with the previous day’s zone was defined. During the course of the project and also within individual animals, the angle defining the size of the illumination zone was increased to include more place fields in the light zone. After completion of the experiments, the rats were deeply anesthetized and perfused through the heart first with PBS followed by a 4% buffered formalin phosphate solution for the histological verification of electrode tracks and optic fiber position. Furthermore, NpHR-YFP/ArchT-GFP expression in dorsal CA1 was verified in each animal by checking the fluorescence of the YFP/GFP tag.
Spike sorting and unit classification
Request a detailed protocolUnit isolation and clustering procedures have been described before (Csicsvari et al., 1998). Briefly, after resampling of the raw data to 20 kHz, action potentials were extracted from the digitally high-pass filtered (0.8–5 kHz) signal. The power computed in a sliding window (12.8 ms) and action potentials with a power of >5 SD from the baseline mean were selected. The spike features were then extracted using principal components analyses. The detected action potentials were then segregated into putative multiple single units using an automatic clustering software (Harris et al., 2000) (http://klustakwik.sourceforge.net/). Finally, the generated clusters were manually refined by a graphical cluster cutting program. Only units with clear refractory periods in their autocorrelation and well-defined cluster boundaries were used for further analysis. Periods of waking spatial exploration, immobility, and sleep were clustered together. The stability of the cells was verified by plotting spike features over time. In addition, an isolation distance (based on Mahalanobis distance, [Harris et al., 2000]) was calculated to ensure the spike clusters did not overlap during the recordings. CA1 pyramidal cells and interneurons were discriminated by their autocorrelations, firing rate, and waveforms (Csicsvari et al., 1999; Henze et al., 2000). In total, we recorded 1842 pyramidal cells and 91 interneurons.
Pyramidal cell-interneuron coupling
Request a detailed protocolIsolation of monosynaptically-connected pyramidal cell-interneuron pairs was performed as described previously by identifying cross-correlograms between pyramidal cells and interneurons that exhibited a large, sharp peak in the 0.5–2.5 ms bins (after the discharge of the reference pyramidal cells) (Csicsvari et al., 1998). Because the number of action potentials used for the construction of these cross-correlograms varied from cell to cell, the histograms were normalized by dividing each bin by the number of reference pyramidal spike events (Csicsvari et al., 1998). The connection strength was thus accessed by measuring the spike transmission probability at the monosynaptic peak indicating the probability that the pyramidal cell would discharge its postsynaptic interneuron partner. However, the chance probability of the two cells firing together was subtracted in order to account for firing rate change-related fluctuations in the correlation strength. The chance firing probability was estimated by averaging the 10–50 ms bins on both sides of the histogram. The significance level for the monosynaptic peak was set at three standard deviations from the baseline (p<0.000001) (Abeles, 1982; Csicsvari et al., 1998). In addition, to filter out sparse histograms, we only considered monosynaptic pairs in which either FAM1 or FAM2 contained at least 1000 spike coincidence counts with the −50 to 50 ms intervals, and the SD of the bin values were less than one-third of the mean bin value.
Comparison of firing rate and firing field analysis
Request a detailed protocolTo compare firing rates between two sessions, we calculated the relative firing rate change by dividing the signed difference between the mean firing rates by the sum of the mean rates (i.e. c=(r2-r1)/(r2+r1)), where r1 and r2 denote the mean firing rates in the two sessions that are compared (Leutgeb et al., 2004; Leutgeb et al., 2005). This score is always between −1 and 1, and the extreme values −1 and 1 mean that a neuron is firing exclusively in one of the two sessions. A similar measure was used to measure the relative change of spike transmission and spike pairing events across different sessions.
Statistical analyses
Request a detailed protocolWe used linear mixed models and ANOVA to determine the significance of variables in predicting spike transmission probabilities and their changes. We used a mixed model comparison to test whether predictions were independent of other variables and displayed corresponding partial correlations in the figures. We added the animal variable as a random effect to all linear mixed models to account for variability across animals and used the comparison of linear mixed model and linear model without an animal variable to test whether an animal variable contributed significantly to the prediction of spike probability and their changes. We used Holm-Bonferroni multiple testing correction to account for comparisons in multiple time windows in the analysis with prediction through pairing.
Data availability
Original data and programs are available in the scientific repository of the Institute of Science and Technology Austria (https://doi.org/10.15479/AT:ISTA:8563).
-
IST AustriaOptogenetic Alteration of Hippocampal Network Activity.https://doi.org/10.15479/AT:ISTA:8563
References
-
Role of the cortical neuron: integrator or coincidence detector?Israel Journal of Medical Sciences 18:83–92.
-
Millisecond-timescale, genetically targeted optical control of neural activityNature Neuroscience 8:1263–1268.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1525
-
Direct afferent excitation and long-term potentiation of hippocampal interneuronsJournal of Neurophysiology 48:597–607.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.1982.48.3.597
-
Oscillatory coupling of hippocampal pyramidal cells and interneurons in the behaving ratThe Journal of Neuroscience 19:274–287.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-01-00274.1999
-
Behavior-dependent short-term assembly dynamics in the medial prefrontal cortexNature Neuroscience 11:823–833.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.2134
-
Intracellular features predicted by extracellular recordings in the Hippocampus in vivoJournal of Neurophysiology 84:390–400.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.2000.84.1.390
-
Long-term synaptic plasticity in hippocampal interneuronsNature Reviews Neuroscience 8:687–699.https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2207
-
Long-term plasticity in identified hippocampal GABAergic interneurons in the CA1 area in vivoBrain Structure & Function 222:1809–1827.https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-016-1309-7
-
Input-specific learning rules at excitatory synapses onto hippocampal parvalbumin-expressing interneuronsThe Journal of Physiology 591:1809–1822.https://doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.2012.245852
-
Presynaptic plasticity: targeted control of inhibitory networksCurrent Opinion in Neurobiology 19:254–262.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2009.05.008
-
Differential modulation of CA1 and dentate gyrus interneurons during exploration of novel environmentsJournal of Neurophysiology 91:863–872.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00614.2003
-
Hippocampal GABAergic inhibitory interneuronsPhysiological Reviews 97:1619–1747.https://doi.org/10.1152/physrev.00007.2017
-
Activity-dependent plasticity of hippocampal place mapsNature Communications 7:11824.https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms11824
Article and author information
Author details
Funding
Austrian Science Fund (I02072)
- Jozsef Csicsvari
Swiss National Science Foundation
- Philipp Schoenenberger
Austrian Science Fund (I03713)
- Jozsef Csicsvari
The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.
Acknowledgements
We thank Michele Nardin and Federico Stella for comments on an earlier version of the manuscript. K Deisseroth for providing the pAAV-CaMKIIα::eNpHR3.0-YFP plasmid through Addgene. E Boyden for providing AAV2/1.CaMKII::ArchT.GFP.WPRE.SV40 plasmid through Penn Vector Core. This work was supported by the Austrian Science Fund (I02072 and I03713) and a Swiss National Science Foundation grant to PS. The authors declare no conflicts of interest.
Ethics
Animal experimentation: All procedures involving experimental animals were carried out in accordance with Austrian (Austrian federal Law for experiments with live animals) animal law under a project license (BMBWF-66.018/0015-WF/V/3b/2014, BMBWF-66.018/0018-WF/V/3b/2019) approved by the Austrian Federal Science Ministry (BMWFW).
Copyright
© 2020, Gridchyn et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
-
- 1,931
- views
-
- 268
- downloads
-
- 7
- citations
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.
Download links
Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)
Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)
Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)
Further reading
-
- Neuroscience
Time estimation is an essential prerequisite underlying various cognitive functions. Previous studies identified ‘sequential firing’ and ‘activity ramps’ as the primary neuron activity patterns in the medial frontal cortex (mPFC) that could convey information regarding time. However, the relationship between these patterns and the timing behavior has not been fully understood. In this study, we utilized in vivo calcium imaging of mPFC in rats performing a timing task. We observed cells that showed selective activation at trial start, end, or during the timing interval. By aligning long-term time-lapse datasets, we discovered that sequential patterns of time coding were stable over weeks, while cells coding for trial start or end showed constant dynamism. Furthermore, with a novel behavior design that allowed the animal to determine individual trial interval, we were able to demonstrate that real-time adjustment in the sequence procession speed closely tracked the trial-to-trial interval variations. And errors in the rats’ timing behavior can be primarily attributed to the premature ending of the time sequence. Together, our data suggest that sequential activity maybe a stable neural substrate that represents time under physiological conditions. Furthermore, our results imply the existence of a unique cell type in the mPFC that participates in the time-related sequences. Future characterization of this cell type could provide important insights in the neural mechanism of timing and related cognitive functions.
-
- Neuroscience
Granule cells of the cerebellum make up to 175,000 excitatory synapses on a single Purkinje cell, encoding the wide variety of information from the mossy fibre inputs into the cerebellar cortex. The granule cell axon is made of an ascending portion and a long parallel fibre extending at right angles, an architecture suggesting that synapses formed by the two segments of the axon could encode different information. There are controversial indications that ascending axon (AA) and parallel fibre (PF) synapse properties and modalities of plasticity are different. We tested the hypothesis that AA and PF synapses encode different information, and that the association of these distinct inputs to Purkinje cells might be relevant to the circuit and trigger plasticity, similar to the coincident activation of PF and climbing fibre inputs. Here, by recording synaptic currents in Purkinje cells from either proximal or distal granule cells (mostly AA and PF synapses, respectively), we describe a new form of associative plasticity between these two distinct granule cell inputs. We show for the first time that synchronous AA and PF repetitive train stimulation, with inhibition intact, triggers long-term potentiation (LTP) at AA synapses specifically. Furthermore, the timing of the presentation of the two inputs controls the outcome of plasticity and induction requires NMDAR and mGluR1 activation. The long length of the PFs allows us to preferentially activate the two inputs independently, and despite a lack of morphological reconstruction of the connections, these observations reinforce the suggestion that AA and PF synapses have different coding capabilities and plasticity that is associative, enabling effective association of information transmitted via granule cells.