Cerebellum encodes and influences the initiation, performance, and termination of discontinuous movements in mice
Abstract
The cerebellum is hypothesized to represent timing information important for organizing salient motor events during periodically performed discontinuous movements. To provide functional evidence validating this idea, we measured and manipulated Purkinje cell (PC) activity in the lateral cerebellum of mice trained to volitionally perform periodic bouts of licking for regularly allocated water rewards. Overall, PC simple spiking modulated during task performance, mapping phasic tongue protrusions and retractions, as well as ramping prior to both lick-bout initiation and termination, two important motor events delimiting movement cycles. The ramping onset occurred earlier for the initiation of uncued exploratory licking that anticipated water availability relative to licking that was reactive to water allocation, suggesting that the cerebellum is engaged differently depending on the movement context. In a subpopulation of PCs, climbing-fiber-evoked responses also increased during lick-bout initiation, but not termination, highlighting differences in how cerebellar input pathways represent task-related information. Optogenetic perturbation of PC activity disrupted the behavior by degrading lick-bout rhythmicity in addition to initiating and terminating licking bouts confirming a causative role in movement organization. Together, these results substantiate that the cerebellum contributes to the initiation and timing of repeated motor actions.
Editor's evaluation
This study conducts physiological recordings in awake mice to reveal how cerebellar Purkinje cells convey temporal information about the onset and offset of ongoing movements. There is a growing appreciation for the cerebellum in planned behavior, but how it contributes to a spontaneously initiated volitional behavior remained unclear. This work provides important insights into the roles of cerebellar Purkinje cells in regulating rhythmic movements under volitional control.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.71464.sa0Introduction
Voluntary movement often encompasses repeated starts and stops of the same deliberate motor action enabling animals to achieve their goals. Because these discontinuous movements require a temporal structure, the brain is thought to generate a timing representation of salient motor events, such as the transition to movement initiation and/or termination, that improves the consistency of behaviors across repetitions (Ivry et al., 2002). This activity may assist in the preparation for voluntary movement, as goal-directed actions must be planned prior to initiation (Ghez et al., 1991). Interconnected brain regions, including the motor cortex, thalamus, basal ganglia, and cerebellum, play a role in planning and executing deliberate movements (Gao et al., 2018; Guo et al., 2015; Kunimatsu et al., 2018). Human studies have helped to elucidate the putative role of each brain structure in motor control. For example, patients with cerebellar damage often have difficulty in accurately timing the initiation and termination of periodically performed discontinuous movements but can otherwise execute continuous rhythmic patterns of motor output in a relatively unimpaired manner (Bo et al., 2008; Schlerf et al., 2007; Spencer et al., 2003). These findings lend support to the idea that the cerebellum processes predictive information related to impending transitions to motor action and inaction such that planned movements are finely timed and thus well executed (Bareš et al., 2019; Ivry et al., 2002; Tanaka et al., 2021). Yet, experimental validation of this hypothesis at the neurophysiological level is lacking.
The cerebellar contribution to movement timing in the domain of sensorimotor prediction is frequently studied in animal models based on simple cue-evoked reflexive behaviors. For delay eyeblink conditioning, cerebellar activity begins ramping in response to sensory cues that predict an impending conditioned response (Giovannucci et al., 2017). Similar ramping of cerebellar activity occurs during other learned behaviors in which sensory cues provide a trigger for movement initiation (Bina et al., 2020; Tsutsumi et al., 2020; Yamada et al., 2019). Neural activity also ramps in the cerebellum during motor planning tasks in which a delay period precedes an impending deliberate movement (Chabrol et al., 2019; Gao et al., 2018; Wagner et al., 2019). As the delay time increases, the onset time of ramping activity shifts, with ramping activity commencing immediately prior to initiation, rather than throughout the entire delay (Kunimatsu et al., 2018; Ohmae et al., 2017). This ramping activity is influential in shaping behavior because its disruption can affect motor timing (Ohmae et al., 2017). Importantly, in many of these sensory-cue-driven tasks, animals must actively avoid executing the motor plan during the delay period because false starts are punished. Overall, the representation of both sensory cues and negative valence signals in cerebellar activity presents challenges in directly assessing how this brain region encodes and directly influences the timing of salient motor-event transitions.
To isolate motor-event-related neural activity in the mouse cerebellum, we trained mice to perform a periodic, discontinuous movement task requiring them to conduct a stereotyped behavior at a regular interval to acquire a reward. Movement-timing activity was apparent in Purkinje cells (PCs) located in the Crus I and II lobules of the lateral cerebellar cortices. At the population level, PC simple spike firing was related to movement kinematics and ramped immediately before the initiation of each cycle of motor action. The ramping onset times were earlier when the movement was internally triggered compared with cases in which the same action was elicited by a sensory cue. PC simple spiking also ramped immediately before the termination of each action cycle. By contrast, climbing-fiber-driven PC activity increased only prior to movement initiation. Optogenetic perturbation of PC activity disrupted movement rhythmicity, terminated ongoing movement, and could initiate movement when the optogenetic stimulus ended. Thus, the cerebellum plays an active role in the temporal organization of periodically performed discontinuous movements under volitional control.
Results
Mice learn to perform a discontinuous movement task based on internal timing
To understand how cerebellar activity relates to the organization of well-timed transitions to motor action and inaction during periodically performed movements, we trained head-fixed mice to self-initiate bouts of licking at regular intervals, where each bout consisted of rhythmic protractions and retractions of the tongue at 6–8 Hz (Horowitz et al., 1977). For this purpose, we used an interval timing task in which thirsty mice consumed water droplets dispensed at a fixed time interval (Toda et al., 2017). In the task structure (Figure 1A), we randomly withheld water allocation in 20% of the trials. Because the mice voluntarily initiated licking bouts without any sensory cues during these unrewarded trials, this step allowed us to assess their ability to elicit an internally planned, periodic motor behavior that anticipated the regular timing of water-reward availability.

An interval timing task to assess the role of the cerebellum in organizing periodic, discontinuous movement.
(A) Schematic diagram of the task. Mice were trained to lick for water rewards delivered at a regular time interval (t). Water was allocated in most trials and withheld in the others. (B) Lick patterns of a beginner mouse during an early training session. Licks are indicated by pink tic marks; water was allocated at the time indicated by the droplet (t = 10 s). (C) Trial-averaged lick rates for the beginner mouse with session trials (159 total) separated based on whether water was allocated (left; rewarded) or withheld (right; unrewarded). (D) Same as panel C but after the mouse received additional sessions of training (216 total trials). (E) Lick patterns over the course of a session after full training (same mouse as in panels B and C). (F) Trial-averaged lick rates of the fully trained animal during an individual session (300 total trials).
In the first few sessions of task performance (fixed time interval of 10 s), beginner mice typically licked sporadically throughout each trial without regard to the timing of water allocation (Figure 1B, C). With experience, the mice learned to alter their strategy to concentrate licking bouts in response to water delivery (Figure 1D). Trained mice eventually initiated exploratory licking bouts prior to water-reward delivery and terminated licking shortly after consuming the dispensed water droplet resulting in, generally, just one bout per trial (Figure 1E, F). This behavioral change occurred without an overt punishment to actively suppress licking during the delay period. In trained mice, the licking behavior was essentially unchanged during water-omission trials, demonstrating their ability to anticipate reward timing and withhold their behavior until the next trial (Figure 1F). These results show that the mice reliably self-initiate regular bouts of voluntary licking based solely on internal timing, presumably referenced by the amount of elapsed time since the previous water reward, and abruptly stop licking after consuming the water reward for each trial (Rossi et al., 2016; Toda et al., 2017).
Engagement of the cerebellum during internally timed discontinuous movements
To identify neural correlates of task-related behavioral events in the cerebellar cortex, we used extracellular electrophysiology to record from cells in the lobules of the left Crus I and II (Figure 2A), regions of the lateral cerebellum implicated in orofacial behaviors (Bryant et al., 2010; Welsh et al., 1995). Although neuronal population activity was densely sampled using multielectrode silicon probes, we restricted our analysis to PCs because they form the sole channel of output from the cerebellar cortex. We identified putative PCs based on their location, firing characteristics, and size (see Materials and methods; Figure 2—figure supplement 1; Tsutsumi et al., 2020).

Modulation of Purkinje cell (PC) simple spiking during the performance of discontinuous movement.
(A) Left: electrophysiological activity was recorded from PCs using silicon probes targeting either the left Crus I or II through a large craniotomy. Right: changes in simple spiking firing, relative to non-licking baseline, for all PCs during water-rewarded trials. Data are separated by lobule and sorted based on average activity-level changes within ±200 ms of water allocation (n = 47 Crus I PCs from 6 mice; n = 42 Crus II PCs from 5 mice). (B) The mean change in simple spike rate (black), relative to baseline, for Crus I PCs during water-rewarded trials. The corresponding trial-averaged lick rate is also shown (pink). (C) Same as panel B but for trials in which water was withheld. (D, E) Same as panels B and C but for Crus II PCs.
In trained mice, PCs in both lobules displayed a heterogeneous range of activity changes in simple spiking patterns during task performance (Figure 2A). In water-rewarded trials, the average activity of Crus I PCs increased as mice began exploratory licking in anticipation of water delivery. The mean simple spike rate increased further once the mice detected water and began consummatory licking (Figure 2B). In unrewarded trials, the PC population activity showed a similar increase during exploratory licking (Figure 2C). However, in these trials, PC simple spiking lacked a prominent second peak in activity. The average activity pattern of Crus II PCs closely resembled that of Crus I PCs. The simple spiking rate increased during exploratory licking and evolved with a further sharp uptick as consummatory licking commenced (Figure 2D, E).
The uptick in PC simple spiking during consummatory licking, relative to exploratory licking, may be attributable to the encoding of reward acquisition, which has been shown to be represented in the activity of granule cells (Wagner et al., 2017). Yet, the overall simple spiking rate showed a linear correspondence to the licking rate when assessed across all contexts of the task in either Crus I or II PCs (Figure 2—figure supplement 2). Therefore, the elevation of PC simple spike firing in response to reward allocation may instead reflect the abrupt increase in licking rate during water consumption (Figure 2B, D). To further explore the possibility that PC activity was related to the context of reward, we separated licking trials dependent on whether water was allocated at the expected interval or at an unexpectedly prolonged interval resulting from reward omission on the immediately preceding trial (Figure 2—figure supplement 3). The pattern of PC activity in the Crus I PC ensemble appeared essentially the same in these two trial types (Figure 2—figure supplement 3) indicating that the PC spike representation of putative motor-related information was not disrupted because of the prior reward-expectation error. In addition, we did not observe persistent alterations in PC activity on water allocation trials during which the mice did not choose to lick (Figure 2—figure supplement 4). Based on these results, we conclude that PCs in both Crus I and II are similarly engaged during the performance of internally timed bouts of voluntary licking and that their ensemble activity largely form a representation of movement.
Given that PCs appeared to encode motor parameters, we next evaluated whether some of the PC activity reflected encoding of individual licks that comprise licking bouts by aligning simple spikes of each cell to cycles of tongue protrusion and retraction (see Materials and methods). Many PCs showed a clear phasic modulation of their average firing during the lick cycle (Figure 3A). The timing of the peak in spiking activity, relative to the lick cycle, varied among PCs (Figure 3B, C). Thus, there is a near continuum of lick-phase representation in the collective responses of Crus I and II PCs. Although nearly all PCs were significantly entrained to the lick cycle (92%), the depth of lick-phase-modulated simple spike activity varied widely across the population (Figure 3D). Interestingly, there was no obvious relationship between the entrainment strength of PC firing to the lick cycle and the average change in firing rate of PCs around the time of water allocation when the licking rate was greatest (Figure 3E). In conclusion, Crus I and II PCs form a fast-timescale representation of the licking rhythm that is nested in a broader representation of additional motor variables related to the overall movement. To ascertain whether some of the broader PC activity in these regions specifically encodes transitions that delimit discontinuous periodic movements, we refined our analysis to examine simple spike firing around two salient motor events: lick-bout initiation and lick-bout termination.

Entrainment of Purkinje cell (PC) simple spiking to the licking rhythm.
(A) In spike histograms from five example PCs, lick phases sampled at spike times show varying levels of modulation. Contact between the tongue and the water port is defined to be 0 = 2π. The upper insets show the smoothed firing rate profiles of the same PC over the entire trial epoch, aligned to the time point of expected water allocation. (B) Probability density for spikes over the lick cycle (left) and the change in firing rate over the entire trial (right; reordered from Figure 2B). Each row corresponds to a single PC and is sorted by the strength of entrainment to licking, defined as the mean resultant length. (C) Thresholded probability density for spikes over the lick cycle. Black regions indicate phases at which the density exceeds 1.02/2π. Rows are sorted by the entrainment phase, defined as the angle of the mean resultant. (D) Entrainment strength for all PCs, sorted as in C. (E) The relationship between firing rate modulation following water delivery and the strength of entrainment to the licking rhythm for individual PCs. The change in simple spike firing rate was averaged over the 2 s following water delivery. Dashed line is the linear correlation. Note, one outlier PC is off scale in panels D and E.
PC simple spiking modulates during the transition to movement initiation
For voluntary movements, motor plans are converted into motor actions at the time point of initiation. In our task, this transition occurred at lick-bout onset, when mice began rhythmic licking. To examine the simple spiking pattern at the time point of movement initiation, we aligned PC activity to the first lick of well-separated bouts (i.e., bouts with ≥2 s of preceding nonlicking) in individual trials across animals (Figure 4A), thus sharpening our ability to discern the temporal correspondence of PC firing and the start of the behavior. Following this event-triggered averaging, we observed that the simple spiking rate generally began to increase several hundred milliseconds before the detection of the first lick in a bout (Figure 4B) with the onset time of the average PC population response leading the first lick by 240 ± 40 ms (Figure 4—figure supplement 1). Because lick initiation is rapid – the time between any visible mouth movement to full tongue protrusion is 30–60 ms (Bollu et al., 2021; Gaffield and Christie, 2017) – this ramping is more suitable for reflecting preparatory activity rather than an online representation of movement execution.

Modulation of Purkinje cell (PC) simple spiking during the initiation of discontinuous movement.
(A) Top: plot of the mean lick rate aligned to the time point of the first lick in each water-rewarded trial bout (n = 3164 trials of well-isolated licking bouts from 11 mice). The lick patterns for example trials of an individual mouse are also shown with licks indicated by pink tic marks and the time of water allocation indicated in blue. Bottom: histogram of lick-bout initiation times relative to water allocation. For most trials, mice began exploratory licking prior to water delivery (pre water). However, in the remaining trials, mice refrained from licking until after water allocation (post water), which immediately triggered a rapid increase in lick-bout initiations to consume the dispensed droplet. (B) The change in simple spiking activity relative to baseline for individual PCs sorted based on their average activity levels within ±200 ms of the first lick in each water-rewarded trial bout. (C) Trial-averaged change in simple spike activity for PCs, separated depending on whether mice initiated lick bouts before or after water allocation (pre and post water, respectively). Note the ramps in activity prior to licking. (D) Licking rates for bouts separated whether licking began before or after water allocation. (E, F) Same as panel C expect for Crus I (panel E) or Crus II (panel F) PCs. (G) Fraction of individual Crus I and II PCs with activity profiles that were either positively (+) or negatively (−) modulated around the time of lick-bout initiation, separated depending on whether licking began before or after water allocation (pre and post water, respectively). (H) Comparison of the onset times of activity ramping, relative to the first lick in trial bouts, for PCs whose activity positively modulated around lick-bout initiation. Data from each lobule were grouped together for statistical comparison (pre water: n = 42 PCs from 10 mice; post water: n = 11 PCs from 7 mice). Asterisk indicates significance (p = 0.0129, Student’s t-test). See also Figure 4—source data 1.
-
Figure 4—source data 1
Source data for Figure 1H.
- https://cdn.elifesciences.org/articles/71464/elife-71464-fig4-data1-v2.xlsx
As noted above, the precise timing of lick-bout initiations, relative to water availability, varied from trial to trial (Figure 4A). As expected for trained mice that accurately anticipate impending rewards, most licking bouts were exploratory, beginning prior to water allocation (Figure 4A). However, in some trials, the mice did not perform any exploratory licking and, instead, waited until water became available before immediately commencing a bout of consummatory licking (Figure 4A). It is unclear why the mice withheld their licking until after water delivery during these trials. However, the tight distribution of lick-bout initiations around the time of water allocation in these trials (median response time: 360 ± 110 ms) indicates that the ensuing consummatory movements were reactive and were likely triggered by sensory evidence indicating water availability (i.e., the mice performed a licking bout after they detected the presence of water).
Separating trials of PC activity based on whether licking bouts were initiated before or after water allocation led to an unexpected result. The average simple spiking rate of PCs began to ramp earlier for exploratory licking bouts, when the movements were initiated prior to water allocation, compared to that of bouts in which consummatory licking commenced immediately after water became available (Figure 4C). Although mice exhibited elevated licking rates for reactive licking bouts relative to exploratory licking bouts (Figure 4D; peak rates: 6.5 ± 0.3 and 8.5 ± 0.2, pre- and post-water licking bouts, respectively; p < 0.001, Student’s t-test), there was no difference between the peak rates of PC simple spike firing for the two licking contexts (Figure 4C; peak Δspike rate within 500 ms of lick onset: 14.8 ± 2.1 and 10.3 ± 2.1, pre- and post-water licking bouts, respectively; p = 0.13, Student’s t-test). We observed a consistent temporal advance in simple spike ramping activity for exploratory licking bouts compared to water-reactive licking bouts for both Crus I and II PCs (Figure 4E, F, Figure 4—figure supplement 2). There were no differences in the peak change in simple spike firing rate for either Crus I PCs (peak Δspike rate: 15.7 ± 3.8 and 8.4 ± 3.3, pre- and post-water licking bouts, respectively; p = 0.15; Student’s t-test) or Crus II PCs (peak Δspike rate: 14.3 ± 2.6 and 12.9 ± 2.8 pre- and post-water licking bouts, respectively; p = 0.72; Student’s t-test) around the time of peak licking (Figure 4E, F).
These results indicate a robust activation of the PC ensemble in the lateral cerebellum prior to lick-bout initiation. However, there was heterogeneity in the simple spike response patterns of individual PCs. Some PCs positively modulated their firing during lick-bout initiations relative to their baseline firing rate (Figure 4—figure supplement 3), whereas other PCs negatively modulated their firing (Figure 4—figure supplement 3); the few remaining PCs were unresponsive during this motor transition. As expected from the population average, PCs that positively modulated their simple spike activity were more common than PCs that negatively modulated their activity (Figure 4G). Additionally, PCs were more likely to positively modulate their activity prior to exploratory licking bouts (i.e., ramp) that preceded water allocation than for bouts of consummatory licking that were reactive to water allocation (Figure 4G).
In the subset of PCs with positively modulated activity, a comparison of ramping onset times across conditions revealed that activity began ~200 ms earlier for bouts of exploratory licking compared with bouts of purely reactive consummatory licking (Figure 4H). However, among the PCs that positively responded to both licking contexts (Figure 4—figure supplement 3), the onset time of activity ramping for anticipatory versus reactive licking was not different (−300 ± 114 and −113 ± 242 ms, respectively; n = 8; p = 0.07; Student’s t-test). This implies that the timing difference in ramping activity in positively modulating PCs must also involve the participation of cells with different types of tuning (e.g., to either exploratory or reactive licking alone; Figure 4—figure supplement 3). We conclude that PC simple spike rates change prior to the initiation of licking bouts, with the timing of ramping activity depending on whether the ensuing movement was initiated by internal motivation or was triggered by a sensory cue indicating water availability.
PC simple spiking modulates during the transition to movement termination
The completion of a motor action is also a salient event. Therefore, to investigate whether PCs encode information pertaining to movement termination, we aligned PC simple spike activity to the last lick of a lick bout (Figure 5A, B). PCs displayed heterogeneous output patterns during this motor transition, with some cells positively modulating their activity and others negatively modulating their activity (Figure 5B). On average, the simple spiking rate in the PC population abruptly increased just prior to the last lick for cells in both Crus I and II during water-rewarded trials (Figure 5C; Figure 5—figure supplement 1). Because the last lick in a bout occurred well after the time point of water delivery (median time interval: 1.37 ± 0.21 s; Figure 5D), most of this ramping activity occurred after the dispensed water droplet had been consumed. In addition to encoding the impending termination of a lick bout, this simple spiking increase may also represent information pertaining to swallowing and/or reward signaling by satiation in the gastrointestinal system (Augustine et al., 2019; Zimmerman et al., 2019). However, simple spiking also ramped prior to the last lick in bouts elicited during water-omission trials (Figure 5E; Figure 5—figure supplement 1), albeit at a much-reduced level compared to the end of water consumption trials. Thus, the ramping activity in PCs at the end of licking bouts most likely corresponds to the anticipation of motor-action termination. Because the level of licking diminished from a greater maximal rate during consummatory licking relative to anticipatory licking in the unrewarded condition (see Figure 2B–E), the differences in ramping PC activity at lick-bout termination between the two licking contexts could reflect differences in the level of overall movement prior to the cessation of action which is reflected in low levels of task-related PC activity at that time point (Figure 5—figure supplement 1). Overall, lick-bout termination was well represented in the PC activity in both lobules (36.2% and 23.8% of PCs in Crus I and II, respectively, modulated their simple spiking [see Materials and methods]).

Modulation of Purkinje cell (PC) simple spiking during the termination of discontinuous movement.
(A) Plot of mean lick rate aligned to the last lick in water-rewarded trial bouts (n = 2846 trials from 11 mice). Also shown are lick patterns of example trials for an individual mouse with licks indicated by pink tick marks and the timing of water allocation in blue. (B) Change in simple spike activity for individual PCs sorted based on their average activity levels within ±200 ms of the last lick in each water-rewarded trial bout. Responses were baselined to the lick-related activity in the seconds prior to the last lick. (C) Trial-averaged change in simple spike activity for Crus I (n = 47 from 6 mice) and Crus II (n = 42 from 5 mice) PCs aligned to the time of the last lick in water-rewarded trials. (D) Distribution of the timing of water allocation aligned to the point of the last lick in rewarded trial bouts (same trials as in panel A). (E) Same as panel C but for unrewarded trials (n = 44 Crus I PCs from 6 mice; n = 38 Crus II PCs from 5 mice).
To examine whether individual PCs tune their activity to specific task features, we sorted all cells based on how their simple spiking modulated during motor-event transitions. PCs were categorized depending on whether they exhibited ramp firing to exploratory licking initiated prior to water allocation, ramp firing to consummatory licking initiated after water allocation, and/or ramp firing at the end of either type of licking bout. Because negatively modulated responses were relatively rare, we pooled PCs that exhibited decreased firing during any motor transition into a single group. Plots of mean spiking rates from several of these groupings, including PCs that positively modulated their firing around either the first or last lick in a bout or that negatively modulated their firing (Figure 5—figure supplement 2), confirmed that our sorting differentiated PCs based on their response profiles. Overall, individual PCs in both Crus I and II were heterogeneous in their representation of task-related attributes (Figure 5—figure supplement 2). Although specialist PCs were common, for example, showing a preference for ramp firing at the initiation of exploratory licking, many PCs were engaged by multiple types of motor transitions, for example by increasing their firing to both licking initiation and termination. In summary, the simple spiking activity of PCs in the lateral cerebellar cortex modulates in response to salient motor events during discontinuous bouts of periodic movements, with only modest tuning for a specific type of motor transition.
Climbing-fiber-induced PC activity increases during movement initiation
In addition to simple spikes, PCs also fire complex spikes and simultaneous bursts of dendrite-wide calcium action potentials in response to excitation provided by climbing fibers, the axonal projections of inferior olive neurons (Llinás and Sugimori, 1980; Ozden et al., 2009). Therefore, to determine the representation of climbing-fiber-induced PC activity during periodically performed discontinuous movements, we used two-photon imaging to measure climbing-fiber-evoked dendritic calcium events in PCs expressing the calcium sensor GCaMP6f (Figure 6A). This imaging-based approach is more sensitive in detecting climbing-fiber-evoked activity because it is challenging to reliably distinguish all complex spike waveforms for individual PCs in extracellular electrophysiological unit recordings (Sedaghat-Nejad et al., 2021; Tsutsumi et al., 2020). In quiescent mice, individual calcium events were readily apparent in the dendrites of left Crus I and II PCs (Figure 6B), reflecting that climbing fibers continuously bombard PCs at 1–2 Hz (Gaffield et al., 2016; Mukamel et al., 2009; Ozden et al., 2012). However, during task performance, only a subset of the PCs showed a behavior-induced change in activity. This evoked response appeared to be spatially organized and dependent on the behavioral context. In water-rewarded trials, the average rate of climbing-fiber-evoked dendritic calcium events increased in PCs in some imaged regions of Crus I and II around the time of water allocation. However, in other imaged regions, there was no change in average PC calcium activity (Figure 6C). Interestingly, the increase in climbing-fiber-evoked activity during water consumption was absent in the same PCs during water-omission trials, when mice performed licking bouts but did not receive water rewards (Figure 6D). Thus, climbing fibers appear to signal reward-acquisition-related information to PCs in specific regions of Crus I and II (Heffley and Hull, 2019; Heffley et al., 2018; Kostadinov et al., 2019).

Climbing-fiber-evoked Purkinje cell (PC) activity increases at the initiation of discontinuous movement.
(A) Left: AAV containing GCaMP6f under control of the Pcp2 promoter was injected into Crus I and II to specifically transduce PCs; a cranial window provided optical access to the infected region. Right: a two-photon image with identified PC dendrites outlined in red. (B) Example fluorescence trace from a PC dendrite showing spontaneous calcium activity during quiescence. Individual climbing-fiber-evoked calcium events are indicated by gray tic marks. (C) Average climbing-fiber-evoked calcium event rates aligned to the time point of water delivery for water-rewarded trials. PC dendrites in some regions of Crus I and II showed clear increases in activity when mice elicited bouts of licking to water allocation (black line, n = 377 PCs in 8 ROIs from 4 mice), whereas in other regions there was very little to no change in activity (dashed red line, n = 239 PCs in 5 ROIs from 4 mice). (D) Same as panel C but for licking during unrewarded, water-omission trials. (E) Trial-averaged calcium event rates in PC dendrites aligned to the timing of the first lick in exploratory bouts initiated prior to water allocation (same data as panel C). (F) Same as panel E but aligned to the first lick for bouts of consummatory licking initiated after water allocation. (G) Top: overlay of the change in trial-averaged calcium event rates, relative to nonlicking baseline, for PCs in task-responsive regions of Crus I and II, aligned to the first lick of bouts initiated before or after water allocation (pre and post water, respectively). (H) Comparison of onset times for climbing-fiber-evoked calcium event ramping for individual PCs in trials where licking was initiated before (pre water; n = 52 PCs) or after (post water; n = 49 PCs) water allocation (see Materials and methods). Black line shows the mean (not significant, NS; p = 0.36, Student’s t-test). (I) Trial-averaged calcium event rate aligned to the timing of the last lick in trial bouts (n = 616 PCs, n = 12 sessions, 5 mice). See also Figure 6—source data 1.
-
Figure 6—source data 1
Source data for Figure 6H.
- https://cdn.elifesciences.org/articles/71464/elife-71464-fig6-data1-v2.xlsx
To evaluate the correspondence of climbing-fiber-evoked activity in PCs more carefully around motor-event transitions, we aligned dendritic calcium activity to the first licks in bouts of either exploratory licking initiated prior to water allocation or reactive licking initiated after water delivery. Average calcium event rates ramped prior to movement initiation for both licking contexts. These increases in climbing-fiber-evoked activity were prominent in the reward-responsive regions of Crus I and II (Figure 6E, F). Although the mice licked at a higher peak rate for reactive bouts than for exploratory bouts, the peak change in dendritic calcium event rates was not different between these licking contexts (Figure 6G; peak Δcalcium event rate: 1.12 ± 0.27 and 2.26 ± 0.60, pre- and post-water bouts, respectively; p = 0.113, Student’s t-test). The onset times of calcium event ramping, relative to the detection of the first lick, were similar for the initiation of both exploratory and reactive bouts of licking (Figure 6H). Aligning PC dendritic calcium events to the last lick of lick bouts did not reveal any clear change in climbing-fiber-evoked activity around the transition to action completion (Figure 6I; calcium event rate 0.92 ± 0.09 and 1.28 ± 0.21 Hz prior to and immediately after the last lick; p = 0.2334, Wilcoxon signed rank test). Together, these results indicate that climbing-fiber-induced activity in a specific population of PCs ramps prior to the initiation, but not the termination, of both internally timed and sensory-cued bouts of goal-directed motor behavior.
Optogenetic PC stimulation disrupts movement rhythmicity and both initiates and terminates action bouts
Having established that PCs in the lateral cerebellar cortex modulate their activity during the performance of discontinuous periodic movements, we applied an optogenetic approach to examine the causal role of PC activity in coordinating cycles of tongue protrusion and retraction, as well as motor-event transitions between action and inaction. We obtained conditional expression of channelrhodopsin-2 (ChR2) in all PCs by crossing transgenic Ai27 mice with the Pcp2Cre driver line (Madisen et al., 2012; Zhang et al., 2004). In these animals, PC photostimulation drove robust simple spike firing, as measured in vivo using extracellular unit recording under anesthesia (Figure 7A, B). To optogenetically perturb PC activity during task performance, we bilaterally implanted optical fibers above the left and right Crus II lobule. Light stimuli were introduced during the period of peak anticipatory licking in a randomized subset of water-omission trials when trained mice were robustly engaged in performing internally timed, exploratory movements without any sensory evidence indicating water availability (Figure 7C, D). In response to PC photostimulation, the licking rate slowed and became erratic, showing a sharp degradation in rhythmicity (Figure 7D, E). Eventually, most mice ceased licking altogether (Figure 7D). After the PC photostimulation period ended, the mice resumed licking on some trials (51.4% ± 7.7%), albeit at a diminished rate compared with control trials (Figure 7D). Unilateral photostimulation of PCs in either the left Crus I or II lobule led to less dramatic effects on licking rhythmicity and rate (Figure 7E, Figure 7—figure supplement 1). The light stimulus had no effect on licking in mice that did not express ChR2 (Figure 7E). Therefore, optogenetically disrupting PC activity during internally timed licking severely disrupts behavioral performance. This includes the parameters related to how PC activity in this region represents the movement, such as the timing of individual cycles of licking, which is important for establishing rhythmicity of closely spaced licks during a lick bout.

Optogenetic perturbation of Purkinje cell (PC) activity degrades the performance of discontinuous movements.
(A) Left: extracellular electrophysiological measurements were obtained from ChR2-expressing PCs in response to photostimulation. Right: optogenetically induced simple spiking in a PC (light pulses indicated in blue). Red lines show the means for each epoch. (B) Summary plot of mean simple spike rate across PCs (n = 6) before, during, and after the optogenetic stimulus. Asterisk indicates significance (p < 0.0001, ANOVA with Tukey’s post-test). (C) In mice with ChR2-expressing PCs, Crus II was bilaterally photostimulated during the interval task. (D) The effect of bilateral optogenetic PC activity perturbation on lick rate (blue) in unrewarded trials (n = 9 sessions, 3 mice). The photostimulus was timed to the period of peak licking, as referenced by interleaved control trials (black). Only well-isolated bouts were included in the analysis. (E) Summary of lick rate variability during optogenetic perturbation of PC activity. The y-axis is scaled logarithmically. Data include trials with bilateral photostimulation of Crus II (n = 9 sessions, 3 mice), trials with unilateral photostimulation of Crus I or II (n = 12 sessions, 4 mice), and trials in control mice where blue light was delivered bilaterally to the cerebellum but PCs did not express ChR2 (n = 6 sessions, 2 mice). Asterisks indicate significant differences during photostimulation trials (p = 0.0228 and 0.0199 for the unilateral and bilateral photostimulation conditions, respectively; ANOVA with Tukey’s post-test). (F) Same as panel D but with the photostimulus timed to the period of earliest lick-bout initiations. Note the absence of licking during photostimulation and the large increase in licking immediately after photostimulation ended (n = 11 sessions, 3 mice). (G) Histogram of lick-bout initiation times for control and optogenetic stimulation trials (same data as panel F). A clear increase in licking probability is apparent after photostimulus ended. Only well-separated lick bouts were included (>2 s of prior nonlicking). (H) Summary of the effect of optogenetic PC activity perturbation on licking behavior during task performance. Asterisks indicate a significant reduction in licking during photostimulation (On: p = 0.0079 and p = 0.0486 for unilateral and bilateral stimuli, respectively; ANOVA with Tukey’s correction for multiple comparisons) and a significant increase during the rebound period for bilateral stimulation (Rb: p = 0.0034, ANOVA with Tukey’s correction for multiple comparisons). In the control condition, light stimuli were delivered to the cerebellum of non-ChR2-expressing animals (n = 12 sessions, 2 mice). See also Figure 7—source data 1.
-
Figure 7—source data 1
Source data for Figure 7B, E and H.
- https://cdn.elifesciences.org/articles/71464/elife-71464-fig7-data1-v2.xlsx
To further explore the ramifications of PC activity perturbation on motor-event transitions, we delivered optogenetic stimuli well before the time point of expected water allocation, when mice first began to elicit bouts of exploratory licking. During the PC photostimulation period, exploratory licking again largely abated (Figure 7F, Figure 7—figure supplement 1). However, immediately after the photostimulation, the mice initiated a barrage of licking at a probability much greater than that observed at the same time point in control trials (Figure 7G, Figure 7—figure supplement 1). Such optogenetically induced rebound licking was more prominent for bilateral photostimulation of Crus II PCs than for unilateral photostimulation of Crus I or II PCs and was not observed for light stimuli delivered to non-ChR2-expressing mice (Figure 7H). Together, these results indicate that in addition to perturbing cycles of tongue protraction and retraction, PC photostimulation can also both initiate and terminate bouts of licking, depending on the timing of the activity perturbation relative to the licking context, indicating a role for PC activity in coordinating the regularity of ongoing motor performance as well as motor-event transitions.
Discussion
The temporal consistency of periodically performed discontinuous movement is believed to be aided by timing representations of salient motor events by the cerebellum (Ivry et al., 2002). In support of this idea, we observed changes in murine PC activity immediately prior to both the initiation and termination of licking bouts timed to regular intervals of water-reward allocation. Moreover, perturbation of this activity influenced the behavioral performance. Although the cerebellum has been implicated in learning temporal associations of predictive sensory cues and impending motor actions, the changes we observed in PC activity occurred while the animals performed well-timed, goal-directed licking independent of any overt sensory evidence indicating reward availability. Thus, our results indicate that this activity is internally driven and related to the timing of motor events that are pertinent to organizing the temporal consistency of volitional behavioral performance.
In rodents, consummatory licking is performed as a continuous rhythmic movement composed of repeated cycling of tongue protractions and retractions which is commanded by a brainstem central pattern generator (Horowitz et al., 1977; Wiesenfeld et al., 1977). Motor plans for volitional lick-bout initiation and the choice of directional licking are composed in the cerebral cortex, which depends on cerebellar input for accurate planning (Gao et al., 2018; Li et al., 2015). We found that intermingled PCs in the Crus I and II lobules displayed heterogeneous coding of licking-related attributes in their pattern of simple spiking suggesting that the cerebellum helps prepare and execute the resulting movement. PCs encoded parameters related to the phasic timing of individual licks that comprise bouts, similar to that observed in a prior study examining nonperiodically performed consummatory licking (Bryant et al., 2010), collectively mapping the entire spectrum of the lick cycle in their simple spiking activity. Climbing-fiber-induced activity in PCs has previously been shown to be aligned to licks in consummatory bouts, including in mouse Crus II (Gaffield et al., 2016; Welsh et al., 1995). The overall licking rate was also encoded in the ensemble PC simple spiking response. However, this effect was only observed across the entire spectrum of licking and was not apparent for subtle differences in lick rate behavior, for example, between the peak rate of exploratory and reactive licking bouts. These results indicate a rich, nested representation of multiple motor attributes occurring at different timescales, reminiscent of PC activity during other rhythmic motor behaviors (Sauerbrei et al., 2015). In our recordings, we also readily observed activity ramping in individual PCs in advance of both lick-bout initiation and termination, suggesting a representation of impending motor-event transitions in this population. This activity appeared to be involved with the preparation of the change in vigor of the ensuing start and end of lick bouts; the former parameter informing the timing of the peak lick rate which, for our task, was temporally aligned to the predicted time of water-reward allocation.
Activity ramping in PCs has also been observed during other volitional motor behaviors. For example, in monkeys, PCs fire elevated barrages of simple spikes immediately prior to changes in eye-movement speed and/or direction during object tracking (Herzfeld et al., 2015) or arm movements during reaching behaviors (reviewed in Ebner et al., 2011). Increased simple spiking also briefly precedes spontaneous and sensory evoked whisking in some PCs of mice (Brown and Raman, 2018; Chen et al., 2016). However, compared to the close temporal correspondence between PC activity ramping and motor action in these earlier reports, lasting just a few tens of milliseconds, we found that the onset time of simple spike ramping preceded lick-bout initiation and termination by hundreds of milliseconds. This time frame is consistent with the ramping of cerebellar activity in advance of internally timed, volitional eye movements of monkeys that followed a delay interval of several seconds (Ohmae et al., 2017). Thus, the onset of PC activity ramping, relative to the ensuing movement, may undertake different dynamics dependent on the timing needs of the underlying behavior.
PC activity did not map the entire epoch of the delay period in our interval timing task, but rather only the end point immediately prior to lick-bout initiation. This result is consistent with the idea that multiple brain regions form timing representations of movements at different scales, with the cerebellum contributing largely to the subsecond range (Tanaka et al., 2021). Interestingly, we found that the onset time of PC activity ramping occurred earlier for licking that was preemptive, rather than reactive, to water-reward availability. Because reactive licking was likely triggered by an external cue, it may be that the shorter onset time of ramping in this context of licking promotes a more reflex-like response, perhaps emergent from sensorimotor associations formed in the cerebellum, ensuring that ensuing consummatory movements are executed with little delay. By contrast, when the timing representation of the impending motor event is internally rather than externally signaled, such as for exploratory licking elicited prior to water allocation, PC activity may ramp earlier due to input (either direct or indirect) from a separate brain region that also participates in organizing the behavior. Contributing brain regions could include the basal ganglia, which also forms a time representation of discontinuous movement, but with a longer timescale than the cerebellum (Kunimatsu et al., 2018; Ohmae et al., 2017). In mice, optogenetic stimulation of an inhibitory basal ganglia pathway catastrophically disrupts licking during the same interval timing task, indicating that this region also plays a role in organizing the behavior (Toda et al., 2017). The cerebellum is also recurrently connected with the cerebral cortex, forming a loop whose activity helps maintain motor plans in working memory until initiation (Gao et al., 2018; Svoboda and Li, 2018). Because mice must carefully track the passage of time to correctly anticipate their next planned cycle of licking around water-reward allocation, earlier PC activity ramping during exploratory licking bouts may also reflect the engagement of cortical circuitry.
The termination of motor action is also a salient event that delimits discontinuous movement. However, in comparison to movement initiation, less is known about how cerebellar activity at the end of each movement cycle encodes and influences periodically performed behaviors. We observed widespread ramping of simple spiking in individual Crus I and II PCs at lick-bout termination, although the population response was relatively weak, especially for unrewarded trials. During task training, the mice learned to adjust their licking rate, ultimately stopping after consuming the dispensed water droplet on rewarded trials. Thus, the diminishing quantity of water during consumption may signal an impending need to end the action based on prior sensorimotor associations formed during task training. Mice also stopped licking on most unrewarded trials, presumably because the estimated time window for the expected water reward had passed. For unrewarded trials, there was no sensory information to cue an impending end to action. After giving up, the mice began waiting for the next period of reward allocation to elicit their planned behavior. Thus, we speculate that the uptick in PC simple spiking toward the end of licking bouts helps prepare an impending stop to motor action, similar to the modulation of cerebellar activity at the end of dexterous reaching behaviors that influences kinematics and endpoint precision of grasps (Becker and Person, 2019; Low et al., 2018). We propose that this activity may be necessary to coordinate precise temporal control of the subsequent cycle of movement.
Although we observed abundant positively modulating PCs in Crus I and II during periodically performed bouts of licking, we recorded relatively few PCs with negatively modulating simple spike responses. This result is surprising because cerebellar-dependent motor control has been attributed to increases in output from the cerebellar nuclei, which is expected to result from the absence of PC-mediated inhibition (Ten Brinke et al., 2017). However, the influence of PCs on movement is believed to manifest at the population level, resulting from the cerebellar nuclei integrating activity from both positively and negatively modulating PCs (Calame et al., 2021; Herzfeld et al., 2015). It also remains possible that the activity we observed is predominately from PCs that play a supporting role in movement organization, participating as an antagonistic module that is recurrently connected with a direct, motor-driving module whose PCs oppositely modulate their simple spike firing pattern (Ohmae et al., 2021). Further work, using anatomical and functional tools to delineate the targets of both positively and negatively modulating PCs, as well as the ability to independently toggle their activity, will be necessary to address this question.
In monkey Crus I and II, PCs encode movement-related activity, but not reward or reward-expectation activity (at least in the absence of learning), in their simple spiking during the performance of a reward-driven motor task (Sendhilnathan et al., 2020). While our results point to a likewise representation of motor-timing events in Crus I and II PC simple spiking, we cannot fully discount the possibility that expectation information is also encoded in the same PC population, as has been shown for cerebellar granule cells (Wagner et al., 2017). Because preparatory neural activity is inexplicably linked to expectation when volitional movements are performed to acquire rewards, it may be that the predictively timed licking behavior we observed may have benefited from such information. For example, the cerebellum may have harnessed the sensory feedback of water availability to guide temporal learning so that licking was periodically elicited around reward availability and ended when the dispensed water was fully consumed.
Climbing fibers provide instructive signals to guide supervised or reinforcement learning (Hull, 2020; Raymond and Medina, 2018). Interestingly, in addition to the ramping of PC simple spiking, we observed an increase in climbing-fiber-evoked activity in a PC subpopulation, which preceded lick-bout initiation by ~100 ms. Although climbing fibers are responsive to sensory stimuli (Gaffield et al., 2019; Ohmae and Medina, 2015), the activity increase occurred in advance of exploratory licking bouts that were initiated without an overt sensory cue, ruling out the possibility that this activity represented an external stimulus. Climbing-fiber-evoked activity did not change at lick-bout termination, suggesting a specific role for this input only at the start of each movement cycle. Reward-related climbing fiber signaling is prevalent in the cerebellar cortex (Heffley and Hull, 2019; Heffley et al., 2018; Kostadinov et al., 2019); thus, the dramatic increase in climbing-fiber-evoked activity in PCs during behavior could reflect a prospective response to an anticipated reward. However, the same PCs were unresponsive in water-omission trials, indicating a lack of apparent reward prediction errors. Therefore, in our view, this movement-aligned activity is more likely motor related.
Because the synchronization of climbing-fiber-evoked complex spiking within parasagittal-aligned clusters of PCs can evoke and/or invigorate motor action, including bouts of sensory-triggered licking (Apps and Hawkes, 2009; Ten Brinke et al., 2017; Tsutsumi et al., 2020; Welsh, 2002), the activity increase we observed in PCs due to climbing fiber input could influence the kinematics of lick-bout initiation if this activity were temporally correlated in the PC population. However, as we used small fields of view during our optical recordings, there were generally too few simultaneously active PC dendrites to accurately quantify the level of their synchrony. For this reason, we were unable to determine whether the responsive and unresponsive regions of Crus I and II during task performance corresponded to distinct, functional clusters of PCs that have been shown to be either engaged or not engaged during sensory-driven licking (Tsutsumi et al., 2020).
Our optogenetic experiment provided causal evidence that Crus I and II PC activity influences movement performance. For example, licking rhythmicity was degraded by PC photostimulation suggesting that the cerebellum is capable of modulating the central pattern generator. PC photostimulation also elicited motor-event transitions resembling those occurring during planned, periodically performed licking. Perturbating PC activity terminated ongoing licking, even when the stimulus was timed to the peak output rate around expected water rewards. This perturbation could also trigger lick-bout initiations when the photostimulation ended. However, optogenetically evoked licking was apparent only when the perturbation was timed to the period when the animals were beginning to elicit exploratory bouts of licking in anticipation of water rewards. Therefore, like the susceptibility of the cerebellum to instantiate learning (Albergaria et al., 2018), our results indicate that there may be a conditional state during which the cerebellum is more effective at triggering movement initiation, in particular when planned movements are first being converted into motivated actions, which may require a differential engagement of the cerebellar–thalamocortical pathway compared to online control of the ensuing movement (Nashef et al., 2021). These behavioral effects are consistent with prior reports. For example, a study found that optogenetic perturbation of cerebellar activity reduces voluntary whisker movements during the photostimulation period and leads to a subsequent rebound in whisking behavior (Proville et al., 2014). Because PCs form a nested representation of multiple behavioral attributes related to periodically performed discontinuous motor behavior, it may that the effects of the PC optogenetic perturbation during licking, or at lick initiation, may stem from disruption of signals related to either individual tongue protrusions and retractions and/or lick-bout initiation or termination. One way to fully disambiguate these effects in the future would be to separately perturb CPG-targeting and non-CPG-targeting PCs, if such pathways exist. Furthermore, we did not assess the circuit effect of autonomous PC photostimulation during behavior. Therefore, we cannot draw any conclusions between the precise pattern of induced activity and motor-event outcomes. However, PC photostimulation produces both direct increases and indirect decreases in simple spiking in the PC ensemble and can drive complex-spike-like bursts of activity (Bonnan et al., 2021; Tsutsumi et al., 2020) which all may be important for motor preparation and execution.
In summary, PC activity encodes and influences cycles of repeat actions and both represents and causes motor-event transitions. Thus, the coordination of explicitly timed, volitional movements is improved by the cerebellum, consequently altering their temporal consistency across repeat cycles of goal-directed action.
Materials and methods
Animals
All animal procedures were performed following protocols approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee at the Max Planck Florida Institute for Neuroscience. Adult animals (>10 weeks) from the following strains of mice were used in this study: C57/Bl6 (5 f, 3 m; Jackson Lab stock: 000664), Pcp2Cre (1 f, 2 m; Jackson Lab stock: 010536), Pcp2Cre crossed with Ai27 Gt(ROSA)26Sortm27.1(CAG-COP4*H134R/tdTomato) (5 f, 8 m; Jackson Lab stock: 012567), and nNOS-ChR2 (1 f, 2 m) in which ChR2(H134R)-YFP expression is controlled by the Nos1 promoter (Kim et al., 2014; Madisen et al., 2012; Zhang et al., 2004). These mice were maintained on a 12-hr light–dark cycle with ad libitum access to food and were provided a running wheel for enrichment.
Surgical procedures
Request a detailed protocolFor all surgeries, we used isoflurane for anesthesia (1.5–2%). A heating pad with biofeedback control provided body temperature maintenance. Buprenorphine (0.35 mg/kg subcutaneous), carprofen (5 mg/kg subcutaneous), and a lidocaine/bupivacaine cocktail (topical) were used for pain control. To restrain the head during behavioral experiments, a small stainless-steel post was attached to the skull. To facilitate neural activity recording, we performed a craniotomy (~2 mm square) over the left lateral cerebellum, above a region that included portions of the Crus I and II lobules (Gaffield et al., 2016). A glass coverslip was cemented over this area to protect the brain postsurgery while the animals were trained for the behavior. For some optogenetic experiments, optical fibers (MFC_400/430–0.48_MF1.25_FLT, Doric Lenses, Quebec, Canada) were instead installed bilaterally over Crus II (3.5 mm lateral, 2.2 mm caudal from lambda). All implants were fixed in position using Metabond (Parkell, Edgewood, NY). For calcium imaging experiments, a subset of mice were injected with adeno-associated virus (AAV)1 containing the genetically encoded calcium indicator GCaMP6f (Chen et al., 2013) under control of the Pcp2 promoter (Nitta et al., 2017) at the brain area under the craniotomy to transduce Crus I and II PCs. All mice were given at least 7 days to recover from surgery before beginning behavioral training and/or experimentation.
Behavioral procedures
Request a detailed protocolFor the interval timing task, mice were held under head-fixation in a custom-built apparatus consisting of a metal tube (25.4 mm diameter) in which the mice rested comfortably and a metal water port that was placed in front of their mouths. This port was calibrated to allocate 4 µl of water per dispensed droplet as determined by the open time of a solenoid valve (INKA2424212H, Lee Company, Westbrook, CT). This apparatus was housed inside a sound-insulated and light-protected enclosure. The water valve was located outside of the enclosure to prevent the mice from hearing it open and close. Licks were detected using a simple transistor-based lick circuit connecting the metal tube to the metal water port. This circuit closed when the tongues of the mice contacted the water port. The apparatus was controlled by a BPod state machine (Sanworks, Rochester, NY) installed on a Teensy 3.6 microcontroller (SparkFun Electronics, Boulder, CO) combined with custom-written codes (Matlab, MathWorks, Natick, MA).
Thirst was used to motivate behavioral performance. To achieve this, mice underwent water restriction with daily water intake limited to 1 ml with frequent monitoring to confirm the lack of any adverse health consequences (Guo et al., 2014). Initial sessions of behavioral training consisted of a block of 50 consecutive water-rewarded trials to reinforce the target time interval. Thereafter, sessions consisted of a trial structure compromising 80% rewarded and 20% unrewarded trials that were randomly distributed (although consecutive unrewarded trials were prevented). Mice typically completed 250–300 trials per session, with only a single session per day, and were considered well trained when they consistently initiated licking bouts prior to water delivery, and they accurately anticipated the water delivery time on unrewarded trials (peak lick rate within 0.5 s of the expected time of water allocation). Most mice reached this criterion level after 10–15 training sessions.
Electrophysiology
Request a detailed protocolThe cerebellum was accessed for electrophysiology recording through the previously prepared craniotomy that exposed large portions of both the Crus I and II lobules including zebrin bands 7+, 6−, and 6+. On the day of recording, and under light isoflurane anesthesia, the coverslip was removed by removing the Metabond that secured it in place. A silver wire was then place into the craniotomy site to provide a ground signal. At least 45 min after recovery from anesthesia, the mouse was transferred to the behavioral apparatus and the brain and ground wire were covered with a saline solution. A silicon probe (A1 × 32-Poly3-5mm-25s-177, Neuronexus, Ann Arbor, MI) was then slowly inserted into the exposed cerebellar cortex, under view of a high-magnification mini-camera, at a few microns per second using a motorized micromanipulator (Mini, Luigs and Neumann, Ratingen, Germany). Because of the large size of the craniotomy, both Crus I and II could be visually distinguished in the video image, allowing the targeted localization of the probe recording site to either lobule of interest by investigator choice. The target depth was approximately <500 μm. The silicon probe was connected to an amplifier (RHD2132, Intan Technologies, Los Angeles, CA) and read out by a controller interface (RHD200, Intan Technologies) with a sampling rate of 20 kHz. Commercial software (Intan Technologies) was used for data acquisition. For electrophysiology experiments combining optogenetics, laser light was delivered directly onto the exposed brain using a patch cable that was carefully positioned to minimize any induced artifacts in the recordings while still targeting the targeted region of the cerebellar cortex. A copy of the electrical signal driving the light stimulus was recorded to ensure proper registration to the electrophysiological and behavioral recordings.
Calcium imaging
Request a detailed protocolAll two-photon imaging experiments were performed as previously described (Gaffield et al., 2016; Gaffield et al., 2019). Briefly, a custom-built, movable-objective microscope acquired continuous images at ~30 frame/s using an 8 kHz resonant scan mirror in combination with a galvanometer mirror (Cambridge Technologies, Bedford, MA). A ×16, 0.8 NA water immersion objective (Olympus, Tokyo, Japan) was used for light focusing and collection. The lens was dipped in an immersion media of diluted ultrasound gel (1:10 with distilled water) that was applied to the cranial window the day of recording. The microscope was controlled using ScanImage software (Vidrio Technologies, Ashburn, VA). GCaMP6f was excited with pulsed infrared (900 nm) light from a Chameleon Vision S laser (Coherent, Santa Clara, CA) with an output power of <30 mW from the objective.
Optogenetics
Request a detailed protocolIn optogenetic experiments, stimulation of ChR2 was driven by a 473 nm continuous-wave laser (MBL-F-473-200 mW; CNI Optoelectronics, Changchun, China). An acousto-optical modulator (MTS110-A3-VIS controlled by a MODA110 Fixed Frequency Driver; AA Opto-Electronic, Orsay, France) modulated the laser power to produce brief pulses of light during experimental procedures (40 Hz, 5 ms). For this, the laser light was directed into a patch cable (BFYL2F01; Thorlabs, Newton, NJ) that was either directly placed over the installed cranial window (unilateral stimulation) or connected to the implanted optical fibers (bilateral stimulation). For unilateral stimulation of either Crus I or II, the output of the patch cable was set to 15 mW and the surrounding area was covered with black foam to limit visibility to the mouse. For bilateral stimulation experiments, the light output was ~2.75 mW; the ceramic connectors (ADAL1; Thorlabs) were covered in black heat shrink tubing, and then covered again with small black tubes to limit light visibility to the mouse. All optogenetic experiments included an overhead 470 nm light emitting diode (M470L4; Thorlabs) that was continuously directed at the mice to mask the light flashes used for optogenetic stimulation.
Data analysis
Request a detailed protocolLicking behavior was analyzed by calculating the lick rate as the inverse of the interlick interval. To quantify correlations between lick rate and the simple spiking rate in PCs, the mean lick rate for each trial was sorted into 1 s bins. Only trials in which the first lick occurred at least 2 s into the trial were included to ensure enough prelick electrophysiological data for analysis. Similarly, only trials in which the last lick had at least 1 s of postlick electrophysiological data were included in the analysis. In many analyses, we only focused on well-isolated licking, defined as bouts without preceding licking for 2 s. For optogenetic perturbation experiments, the lick variance was normalized to the prestimulus control condition for each session.
Silicon probe data were sorted automatically by the Kilosort algorithm (Pachitariu et al., 2016), followed by manual curation using Phy2 software (https://github.com/cortex-lab/phy, Rossant, 2021) into 202 unique clusters. In a first-pass analysis, PC units (n = 48) were unambiguously identified by some accompanying complex spikes in the recordings (Figure 2—figure supplement 1A). We also confirmed that these units had the simple spiking properties expected for PCs (i.e., firing rate and regularity) (Van Dijck et al., 2013). There were an additional 56 units with similar simple spiking responses, but complex spikes could not be clearly discerned in their activity recordings. Noticeably, spiking in unambiguously identified PC units was clearly represented in many nearby channels of the silicon probes (Figure 2—figure supplement 1C). Therefore, we calculated the mean peak spike size in each channel and counted the number of channels with a peak significantly above the noise (~30 µV). This approach determined that the channel count for unambiguously identified PCs tended to be quite high (i.e., ≥7), likely due to the large size of PCs relative to the spacing of the electrode pads on the silicon probes (Figure 2—figure supplement 1C,D).
To test whether channel count representation could be used to classify PCs, we compared the spike distribution of unambiguously identified PCs in each channel with those of another abundant cerebellar cell type, molecular layer interneurons (MLIs). We did not choose to examine granule cells, another abundant cell type, in this comparison because granule cells typically have very different behavior-evoked firing properties than PCs (Powell et al., 2015). In addition, due to the low impedance of the electrode pads used on silicon probes, the activity of granule cells is generally not detectable in extracellular electrophysiological recordings. To positively identify MLI units in our recordings, we used an opto-tag strategy whereby ChR2-expressing MLIs in nNOS-ChR2 mice (Kim et al., 2014) had short-latency responses to light pulses (Figure 2—figure supplement 1B; recordings were not obtained from these mice during behavior). In comparison to unambiguously identified PC units, spiking in identified MLI units was detected in only a few channels (Figure 2—figure supplement 1C-E). Therefore, a high representation of spiking activity in many channels (≥7) could successfully classify PCs. Using this criterion to identify additional putative PCs, we included another 41 units, from the 56 showing similar spiking response characteristics, for a total of 89 in our analysis.
When quantifying the entrainment of PC spiking to the lick cycle, we only included licks that occurred in sequences of at least three consecutive licks with interlick intervals of 100–175 ms. For each PC, the phase of the lick cycle at which each spike occurred was obtained by linear interpolation between consecutive licks. Tongue contact with the sensor was defined to be 0 = 2π radians. To determine whether each PC was entrained to the licking rhythm, a Rayleigh test was performed. To control for multiple comparisons across neurons, a Benjamini–Hochberg correction was applied with a false discovery rate of 0.05. The distribution of lick phases at spike times was visualized using histograms with a bin width of π/10. The circular density of lick phases was computed using a kernel density estimator with a kernel width of 0.3. The phase and magnitude of entrainment were characterized using the angle and length of the mean resultant, respectively.
We manually examined the data to classify PCs into groups based on how their simple spiking activity was tuned to different types of motor-event transitions during the behavioral task. PCs were classified as activated during the first lick in a bout if their simple spiking rate showed three consecutive increases in 100ms time bins that began 1 s before that first lick and ended 0.5 s after that same first lick. The ramp onset time was the time bin of the first of those rate increases. PCs were classified as activated during the last lick in a bout based on the mean simple spike rate during the 300 ms preceding the last lick. Negatively modulated PCs were classified using the same criterion except we used negative values instead. In some cases, individual PC recordings were noisy enough that our rigid classification may have missed some responding cells, so we expect our results to represent a lower-bound estimate of tuning specificity. To quantify population activity, we used the change in simple spike rate (Δspike rate). For this metric, we determined the average firing rate for all nonlicking periods for each PC during the recording and used this level of activity as the baseline from which movement-related firing was computed for that cell. For the last lick analysis, Δspike rate was alternatively calculated relative to PC activity during the 1–5 s preceding the last lick.
Calcium imaging data were analyzed using a standard procedure (Gaffield et al., 2016). In brief, individual PC dendrites were identified using independent component analysis (Hyvärinen, 1999). Calcium events were then extracted from the raw fluorescence traces using an inference algorithm (Vogelstein et al., 2010). Regions of interest (ROIs) in the imaged areas of Crus I and II were considered water responsive if a peak in activity was observed in the total PC average for that region at the time of water delivery. The ramp onset time was determined when the peak event rate reached >3 standard deviations above the mean, in 100-ms time bins, beginning 1 s before and up to 0.5 s after the first lick in a lick bout. To quantify calcium event activity prior to the last lick, we used a similar criterion, but examined event rate activity at least 2-s post water delivery in rewarded trials as well as at the end of licking in water-omission trials.
In the figures, shaded areas in activity plots indicate standard error of the mean (SEM range); error bars are also represented as SEM. Statistical values were calculated using GraphPad (Prism, San Diego, CA) with significance indicated by p values below 0.05.
Data availability
All data generated or analyzed during this study are included in the manuscript and supporting files. Source data files have been provided for Figures 4, 6 and 7.
References
-
Locomotor activity modulates associative learning in mouse cerebellumNature Neuroscience 21:725–735.https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-018-0129-x
-
Cerebellar cortical organization: a one-map hypothesisNature Reviews. Neuroscience 10:670–681.https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2698
-
A cerebellar deficit in sensorimotor prediction explains movement timing variabilityJournal of Neurophysiology 100:2825–2832.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.90221.2008
-
Cerebellar cortical output encodes temporal aspects of rhythmic licking movements and is necessary for normal licking frequencyThe European Journal of Neuroscience 32:41–52.https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07244.x
-
What features of limb movements are encoded in the discharge of cerebellar neurons?Cerebellum (London, England) 10:683–693.https://doi.org/10.1007/s12311-010-0243-0
-
Chronic imaging of movement-related Purkinje cell calcium activity in awake behaving miceJournal of Neurophysiology 115:413–422.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00834.2015
-
Organization of voluntary movementCurrent Opinion in Neurobiology 1:664–671.https://doi.org/10.1016/s0959-4388(05)80046-7
-
Coordinated cerebellar climbing fiber activity signals learned sensorimotor predictionsNature Neuroscience 21:1431–1441.https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-018-0228-8
-
Genetic and environmental variability in lick rates of micePhysiology & Behavior 19:493–496.https://doi.org/10.1016/0031-9384(77)90224-4
-
Fast and robust fixed-point algorithms for independent component analysisIEEE Transactions on Neural Networks 10:626–634.https://doi.org/10.1109/72.761722
-
The cerebellum and event timingAnnals of the New York Academy of Sciences 978:302–317.https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.2002.tb07576.x
-
Electrophysiological properties of in vitro Purkinje cell dendrites in mammalian cerebellar slicesThe Journal of Physiology 305:197–213.https://doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.1980.sp013358
-
Minimal Purkinje Cell-Specific PCP2/L7 Promoter Virally Available for Rodents and Non-human PrimatesMolecular Therapy. Methods & Clinical Development 6:159–170.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.omtm.2017.07.006
-
Climbing fibers encode a temporal-difference prediction error during cerebellar learning in miceNature Neuroscience 18:1798–1803.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4167
-
Cerebellar Roles in Self-Timing for Sub- and Supra-Second IntervalsThe Journal of Neuroscience 37:3511–3522.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2221-16.2017
-
Reliable coding emerges from coactivation of climbing fibers in microbands of cerebellar Purkinje neuronsThe Journal of Neuroscience 29:10463–10473.https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0967-09.2009
-
Cerebellum involvement in cortical sensorimotor circuits for the control of voluntary movementsNature Neuroscience 17:1233–1239.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3773
-
Computational Principles of Supervised Learning in the CerebellumAnnual Review of Neuroscience 41:233–253.https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-neuro-080317-061948
-
A GABAergic nigrotectal pathway for coordination of drinking behaviorNature Neuroscience 19:742–748.https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4285
-
Timing of rhythmic movements in patients with cerebellar degenerationCerebellum (London, England) 6:221–231.https://doi.org/10.1080/14734220701370643
-
P-sort: an open-source software for cerebellar neurophysiologyJournal of Neurophysiology 126:1055–1075.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00172.2021
-
Disrupted timing of discontinuous but not continuous movements by cerebellar lesionsScience (New York, N.Y.) 300:1437–1439.https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1083661
-
Neural mechanisms of movement planning: motor cortex and beyondCurrent Opinion in Neurobiology 49:33–41.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2017.10.023
-
Nigrotectal Stimulation Stops Interval Timing in MiceCurrent Biology 27:3763–3770.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2017.11.003
-
Fast nonnegative deconvolution for spike train inference from population calcium imagingJournal of Neurophysiology 104:3691–3704.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.01073.2009
-
Functional significance of climbing-fiber synchrony: a population coding and behavioral analysisAnnals of the New York Academy of Sciences 978:188–204.https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.2002.tb07567.x
-
Licking behavior: evidence of hypoglossal oscillatorScience (New York, N.Y.) 196:1122–1124.https://doi.org/10.1126/science.558653
-
Highly restricted expression of Cre recombinase in cerebellar Purkinje cellsGenesis (New York, N.Y 40:45–51.https://doi.org/10.1002/gene.20062
Decision letter
-
Aya Ito-IshidaReviewing Editor; Keio University School of Medicine, Japan
-
Tirin MooreSenior Editor; Stanford University, United States
-
Shogo OhmaeReviewer; Hokkaido University School of Medicine, Japan
Our editorial process produces two outputs: i) public reviews designed to be posted alongside the preprint for the benefit of readers; ii) feedback on the manuscript for the authors, including requests for revisions, shown below. We also include an acceptance summary that explains what the editors found interesting or important about the work.
Decision letter after peer review:
Thank you for submitting your article "The cerebellum encodes and influences the initiation and termination of discontinuous movements" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by 3 peer reviewers, one of whom is a member of our Board of Reviewing Editors, and the evaluation has been overseen by Tirin Moore as the Senior Editor. The following individual involved in review of your submission has agreed to reveal their identity: Shogo Ohmae (Reviewer #2).
The reviewers have discussed their reviews with one another and agreed on the potential importance of the study. However, the reviewers have raised some critical concerns which need to be clarified by additional experiments and/or analysis. The Reviewing Editor has highlighted these concerns, and the comments from three reviews are listed below.
Essential revisions:
1) The authors show that the simple spikes but not complex spikes ramp up before the initiation and termination of lick bouts. While this finding is important, the causality between the simple spikes and motor initiation/termination is unclear. In general, simple spikes fire more when the lick rate increases and confounds the interpretation of ramping activity (Reviewer 2). Furthermore, the trained licking behavior may not be well-timed enough to obtain a clear correlation between neuronal activity and behavior (Reviewer 1). These issues need to be clarified either by additional experiments or analysis.
2) The results from the optogenetic experiments did not sufficiently support the main conclusion. Because the light stimuli by itself caused significant changes in licking, the shift in the lick–peak may have been induced indirectly by this abnormal licking behavior and not directly by the changes in simple spike activity (Reviewer 1-1, Reviewer 2-4). In addition, because optogenetic stimuli did not specifically change simple spikes, the involvement of complex spikes cannot be ruled out (Reviewer 3).
Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):
Specific concerns:
1) It is difficult to interpret the optogenetic perturbation experiments in absence of corresponding neural data. While recording during such experiments certainly imposes an additional experimental burden, it is necessary to understand what essential features of PC activity dictate behavioral performance. In particular, it is striking that the same stimulation only produces a delay in peak licking when licks are initiated less than one second from the stimulus. Corresponding neural data revealing what differs between trials with short and long latency lick initiations would enable a much stronger interpretation of this result.
Related to this concern, Figure 7 demonstrates that optogenetic stimulation initiates licks on some (but not all) trials immediately following stimulation. The spike above baseline in lick initiation probability (Figure 7G) occurs < 1 s following optogenetic stimulation. This may suggest that the delay in peak lick rate only occurs when non–voluntary licks are initiated too close to the target time (i.e. on the trials where cessation of optogenetic stimulation drives immediate rebound licking)? The sub vs supra–second timing result could therefore relate to 1) the animals' voluntary initiation of licking too close to the optogenetic perturbation on a subset of trials, or 2) the animals' immediate involuntary response to cessation of optogenetic stimulation on a subset of trials. These two possibilities may suggest different interpretations for how the cerebellum contributes to the behavior.
2) It is not clear that the observed responses relate to motor initiation and termination. For example, considering figure 4C vs 4E, the Sspk ramping at lick offset is quite different for omission trials as compared to rewarded trials. If these signals primarily reflect a timing response for stopping the motor program, they should not depend so heavily on reward context. Alternatively, the initiation signals could also be interpreted in the context of expectation. This is very challenging to disentangle, however. In this behavior, the omission trials do not serve well to do so, as it is not possible to align the data appropriately. Figure 2C shows that the animals have a poor temporal estimation of reward delivery on average, and maintain near–peak licking across an approximate 5 second window in absence of reward. Thus, because animals do not have a strong estimation of when the reward will occur, alignment to 'expected reward time' on omission trials is unlikely to capture any underlying responses. Related to this, given that Wagner et al. showed that granule cells also have reward omission responses in Crus I – it is important to reconcile this difference, even if it is simply because omission responses may not be identifiable given this task design.
Also related to this concern, the discussion argues that "our results reinforce the idea that the cerebellum influences well–timed, regularly performed actions that are generated solely by motivation...". This may not be accurate, and illustrates a major confound in interpreting signals during this behavior. Certainly, naïve animals are driven solely by motivation. They do not, however, exhibit any predictively timed licking behavior. In contrast, trained animals have harnessed the reward–related sensory stimulus to generate a new predictive behavior. This difference between naïve and trained animals shows that reward does double duty in this task – it is both a sensory stimulus that has intrinsic positive valence (and is thus sought solely by motivation in naïve animals), and a sensory cue that can guide temporal learning. Because naïve animals exhibit the former but not the latter, these two roles are separable. However, this dual role of the sensory input in this task also makes the interpretation of the neural responses in the learned condition challenging. In other words, because the cue and reward are one in the same in this task, it is extremely difficult to disambiguate sensory, expectation/prediction, and motor related signals. I am concerned that the task design may preclude causal relationships between the neural activity and behavior as a result.
Additional questions:
1) What is the depth of silicon probe recordings (what is the range of depths of the primary contacts for recorded PCs?). Does this overlap with imaging depths, or does the imaging data come from different regions than the electrophysiology? Do the Cspks recorded with electrophysiology exhibit the same behavior as those captured with calcium imaging?
2) What is the definition of a lick bout? Why is the average absolute lick rate (in s–1) pinned at 0 with no error for 2 seconds prior to lick bout initiation (e.g. Figure 3A)? It seems that bout initiations begin about 2.5 seconds prior to reward (Figure 3A bottom). Figures 1 and 2 show continuous licking on average for 5 seconds prior to reward. Doesn't this mean that there should be some non–zero lick rate prior to bout initiation? Alternatively, if these are licks defined by 2 seconds of preceding quiescence, shouldn't the initiations in the bottom of figure 2A bottom be further from the reward time?
3) It looks like there is a Cspk response to the last lick in figure 6I – is this significant? Is this response different between rewarded and reward–omission trials?
4) How are lick initiations defined surrounding optogenetic stimulation? Optogenetic stimulation suppresses licking, and results in rebound responses afterwards. When these occur in the pre–water period, they are defined as initiations. Why aren't the post water licks that follow the quiescence period of optogenetic stimulation considered initiations? Would not the same analysis show in 7G (related to 7F) yield a similar spike in initiation probability if performed on the data from 7D?
5) It's a bit odd to argue that imaging was necessary because it is "challenging to reliably distinguish complex spike waveforms" (line 337), while at the same time making the case that cell sorting was validated based on the property that PC units were 'unambiguously identified by accompanying complex spikes in the recordings" (line 774).
Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):
I recommend the following analysis, presentation, and discussion to improve the paper.
For the first major comment, the authors need to evaluate the encoding of each bout of licking by cross–correlation or event triggered averaging, and separate it from the encoding of initiation and termination (e.g. using linear model).
For the second comment, they should further analyze the negatively modulating Purkinje cells, shown in Figure 5C. They also need to display the histology of their recording sites (Schematic drawing in Figure 2A is not enough). Also, they should discuss about the potential bias in recording of Purkinje cells.
For the third comment, I recommend the spike rete–based display for the data presentations, which should include the spontaneous spike rates.
For the fourth and fifth comments, related to the first comment, if the Purkinje cells contribute to controlling each of licking bout, the observation in the optogenetic experiments could be explained. The authors need to consider the possibility and improve the interpretation and conclusion.
Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):
– Figure 3 E shows the difference in simple spike onset between pre and post licking. It seems both Crus I and II were pooled together, but this analysis should be conducted separately in different lobules.
– Figure 3 F shows representative PC activity relative to the first lick. Is this trace from pre–water lick or post–water lick? Representative traces from both pre–water and post–water lick should be presented to demonstrate the difference in the ramp onset.
– Figure 4C shows spike rate changes before the termination of the lick. As the firing rate in Figure 3C–D is plotted in (s–1), this data should be also presented using the same unit for comparison.
– Figure 5D, please revise color coding for easier interpretation.
– Figure 5D shows a significant number of cells in Crus II ramped up for both pre–water and post-water licking. It would be informative to compare the ramp onset between pre-water and post–water licking for these cells. The result will show whether the same cells responded differently to the self–initiated and externally triggered licking.
– In Figures 7 and 8, optogenetic stimulation of PCs was conducted with ChR2. While this experiment was performed to test the involvement of simple spikes, it could be mediated by alteration in complex spikes. Were complex spikes affected by the stimuli? Representative traces from raw data for Figure 7A should be presented to address this point.
– Figures 8B and 8F show the delayed peak in lick rate with PC activation. This finding should be supported with additional statistical analysis.
[Editors' note: further revisions were suggested prior to acceptance, as described below.]
Thank you for resubmitting your work entitled "The cerebellum encodes and influences the initiation and termination of discontinuous movements" for further consideration by eLife.
We have consulted the original reviewers and have agreed that the revised version improved significantly. However, several issues remain and need to be clarified. Please read the reviewers' comments below and try to address them fully. Please note that points #1 and #2 in Reviewer 1's comment are critical for a successful revision: both reviewers agreed that these are the major weaknesses of the paper.
Reviewer 1:
I have several comments below directly related to our initial reviews. I think the paper is interesting, and that they could revise one more time with no experiments to address these issues.
The main concerns about the initial submission focused largely on the question of whether the authors' data and analysis support the central claim that the measured neural activity patterns are causally related to motor initiation and termination. In response to these concerns, the authors have provided several important clarifications and some new analyses. While I remain positive about the potential impact of the findings in this study, and would support this publication for eLife, there are some important remaining issues directly related to those central concerns.
1. Now that the authors have clarified the analysis related to figure 4, I am concerned about its main conclusions. Specifically, the authors state that "the average simple spiking rate began to ramp earlier for exploratory licking trials, when the movements were initiated prior to water allocation, compared with that for trials in which consummatory licking commenced immediately after water became available" (lines 205–208).
This is a key point meant to distinguish neural responses on planned vs unplanned movements, but I am not convinced by the underlying analysis. The authors have subtracted z–scored neural data for pre and post water licks, and used the difference to suggest that pre water licks have an associated neural responses that "starts earlier". However, this analysis conflates response amplitude and timing, as a subtraction only reveals when responses diverge (the negative latency in the difference trace cannot be taken to mean that the larger response began earlier). To specifically address timing (and not amplitude), the responses must first be scaled, and then subtracted. Because a smaller response takes longer to cross the same amplitude threshold, a subtraction will show a difference right away, even if the responses start at the same time. In addition, the quantification as performed in Figure 4F will necessarily indicate shorter latencies for larger responses.
It seems very likely that a scaled subtraction will show no latency difference for the data in figure 4C and D, and this would contradict the conclusion of a timing difference in neural responses for planned and unplanned movement.
The data in Figure 4 do, however, show a much smaller response on unplanned movements, which is interesting. However, it is unclear whether or not the licking is different in this case? This needs to be shown for the associated neural traces in figure 4. The overall data suggest that lick bouts are relatively homogenous at their initiation. Thus, these data may indicate that the amplitude of the neural response does not reflect lick rate, and the timing of the response does not relate to prepared or unprepared movement. As reviewer 2 notes, PC activity also does not seem to represent peak lick rate timing.
Together with the above, I therefore remain unclear on the authors model linking neural activity and behavior, and whether the recorded activity relates to motor preparation / planning / execution of some kind.
Other important issues:
2. In reading the other reviewers concerns about z–scoring and the authors responses, I realized that the z–scoring is performed to different baselines for different analyses, and not to the mean spike rate across the trial or a common reference period across analyses (the specifics of the z–scoring are not in the methods). This can be seen in difference between figures 4C and 5C, for example. I am concerned about this practice for a couple of reasons:
2.1) it means that the amplitude of neural signals cannot be compared across different figures and conditions. This makes it challenging to interpret the relationship between firing changes and behavior.
2.2) to disambiguate other explanations for the neural data in this paper, it would be helpful to further leverage conditions where behavior differs. For example, in Figure 4C, there are trials where no licks occur until after presentation of water. This affords the opportunity to ask whether or not there was any increase in spike rate before water allocation in absence of licking (and thus test whether changes in spike rate might reflect something other than motor initiation). However, the authors have z–scored to the mean immediately preceding post–water licks, which would obscure any such changes (Figure 4 supp. 2 may show such an increase pre–water?). While I am sympathetic to the authors' arguments that z–scoring is commonplace for neural recording data, the implementation here is not ideal for evaluating the relationship between spiking and behavior.
3. In the first round of reviews, the question of how to appropriately disambiguate expectation based on omission trials was raised, given the animals imprecise expectation of the time of water delivery. This concern necessitates a more convincing analysis in order to support the authors statements in the discussion regarding expectation signals. For example, with complex spiking on omission trials, alignment to the first lick after the time of water expectation would provide a more appropriate timepoint to indicate the animals' expectation. Even better would be to look at the moment when lick bouts start to decrease / are terminated on omission trials, as this is the timepoint when the animals' behavior indicates recognition that the expected reward is not present (and this time has previously been shown to reveal such expectation signals, at least in some conditions). There may indeed be no evidence of expectation signals in this behavior, but in absence of such analysis to evaluate the question appropriately, it seems premature to make such conclusions.
4. This point is only a suggestion, but I think others may have the same confusion regarding figure 8. The difference between 5 and 10 second trials is not that licking ramps up more slowly on 10 second trials – rather, the mean lick plots shown here speak to the probability of lick bout initiation across trials. On single trials, licking just goes from nothing to the patterned bout rate. On average, however, this manifests as a ramp due to variability in the onset of lick bout times, and the increase in probability of initiation as the trial progresses. The essential feature of 10 second trials is that lick bouts start on average later than they do for 5 second trials. This necessarily means that lick bouts are more likely to be outside of the 1.25 second window from optogenetic stimulation defined by the analysis in 8F. However, if the same analysis from figure 8E and F were performed for 10 second trials (<1 vs > 1.25 second initiations from stimulation), it should yield the same result. It is likely that the average licking for 10 second trials following stimulation only looks different because it is weighted so much more heavily toward bouts initiated >1.25 seconds from stimulation.
This analysis would greatly enhance the authors point that these effects are all about the duration from optogenetic stimulation when the animal tries to lick, and not about the absolute duration of the trial (as many will likely assume based on the figure and its description). Perhaps this seems obvious, but such clarification could provide strong support for the authors arguments related to the final figure.Reviewer 2:
The paper contains very interesting topics and findings and has a potential impact worthy of eLife, but I think it is still too immature to be published with minor revisions. I think the opinion of revising it again without experiments is very valid.
I also agree with the concerns of Reviewer #1. In particular, I have exactly the same concerns about the main concern and #2 issue, and I think they are very important points. So below is a summary of my other opinions.
The authors focus on lick–bout initiation and termination, and make the central claim that the lick cycle is controlled by the CPG in the brainstem, and that the cerebellum contributes to its initiation and termination.
This is a great improvement over the previous version. In particular, the detailed analysis shows that the cerebellar Crus I and II encode individual Licks, Lick rates, and information about lick–bout initiation and termination, which is a very interesting and exciting finding. The authors focus on lick–bout initiation and termination, and make the central claim that the lick cycle is controlled by CPG in the brainstem, and that the cerebellum contributes to its initiation and termination. But the experiments of optogenetics do not fully demonstrate the causality of this central claim. The story focusing only on the current central claim may be somewhat unreasonable.
Again, considering that their central claim is that the Lick cycle is controlled by the CPG in the brainstem, and that the cerebellum contributes to its initiation and termination, the optogenetic experiments performed to demonstrate causality do not clearly support this. First, regarding the initiation, considering that the neural activity in Figure 4 is a rising ramping activity, one would expect that the PC stimulation in Figure 7F would help the ramping and accelerate the initiation time. However, the result was rather the opposite. During stimulation, no initiation occurs, and in fact, it appears to strongly inhibit the initiation of licking (also in Figure 8B). Rather, initiation occurred as a rebound after stimulation. It needs to be properly explained why this is the opposite.
Next, regarding termination, considering that Figure 5 also shows ramp–up activity before the termination, PC stimulation at the peak of lick rate in Figure 7D would be expected to accelerate the termination time. Although this terminated the lick cycle as expected, licking started again after the stimulus offset. Then, the possibility remains that the stimulus only temporarily suppressed individual licking bouts, but did not terminate the lick cycle.
Figure 8 seems to have nothing to do with their central claim. I think this is a different story: The initiation time of the Lick cycle influences the subsequent motor plan (Licking peak time), as they stated (L557). This is interesting in itself, as it is related to Figure 2E, but I feel that the change in story here is abrupt, as the story has been following the central claim up to this point. I think some readers may find it hard to follow. I think it would be better if the story were more consistent.
L557 "a delay or discoordination in the transition to movement initiation, which is normally signaled by ramping PC activity, disrupts and/or delays the remaining motor plan, leading to a mistimed action."
As for the CF signal in Figure 6, it does not encode individual licks, which seems inconsistent with the Nature paper by Welsh et al. 1995. It would be good to have a discussion on why the different results were obtained.
Related to a concern of Reviewer 1, when calculating the firing rate, the firing rate for the period immediately before the event of interest is set to 0. Therefore, the 0 value is different in each plot, and the activity immediately before the event is missing. It is preferable to display the plots with the spontaneous firing rate set to 0 (at least in Supplementary Figures).
[Editors' note: further revisions were suggested prior to acceptance, as described below.]
Thank you very much for revising your article "The cerebellum encodes and influences the initiation and termination of discontinuous movements". We have discussed the revised manuscript with the original reviewers. All three agreed that the manuscript has improved significantly and contains important information which should merit those studying cerebellum and motor control.
However, during the consultation, it was brought to our attention that some statements regarding the optogenetic experiments are inappropriate. Purkinje cells' activity could be classified into two types:
1. PC activity for individual licking movements (as shown in Figure 3).
2. PC activity for the transition (initiation and termination) of licking cycles.
Therefore, the optogenetic stimulation of PCs should influence both individual licking movements and the transition of licking bouts. In the current manuscript, most statements focus on #2 without mentioning the influence of #1. It gives an impression of an over–interpretation or over–simplification and may lead to misunderstanding. Below are some examples:
– L38 (Abstract)
Optogenetic perturbation of PC activity disrupted the behavior in both initiating and terminating licking bouts, confirming a causative role in movement organization.
– L536 (Results)
Together, these results indicate that PC photostimulation can both initiate and terminate bouts of licking, depending on the timing of the activity perturbation relative to the licking context, indicating a role for PC activity in coordinating motor event transitions.
– L717 (Discussion)
Our optogenetic experiment provided causal evidence that Crus I and II PC activity influences movement performance.
– L741 (Conclusion of Discussion)
In summary, PC activity both represents and causes motor–event transitions, influencing the coordination of explicitly timed, volitional movements to improve the temporal consistency across repeat cycles of goal–directed action.
The above examples can be read as such that the causality between PC activation and behavior transition is clear (#2). However, this is not the case because activity #1 is also involved. Please take a moment to consider this point and clarify the statements to avoid confusion. Below are some recommendations from one of the reviewers.
1) Describe activity #1 clearly in the abstract and others. Activity #1 should be written with respect to previous works (e.g., Bryant et al.). Novel aspects of this study, such as analysis, etc., should be described more specifically.
2) The optogenetics experiment should not be discussed by solely focusing on the causality between activity #2 and behavior but should consider the causality between activities #1/#2 and behavior (rewrite L505).
3) Describe the conclusion of the optogenetics experiment, as "the experiment indicates that activities #1 and #2 contribute to the behavioral performance of Licking."
4) As a Discussion of the optogenetics experiment, it should be stated that the impairment may be mediated by changes in either Activity #2 (especially the experiment with photostimulation during Licking) or Activity #1 (especially experiments with photostimulation during the licking initiation) or both. The authors may discuss a possible approach to separate the two: it may be necessary to develop new paradigms such as pathway–specific photostimulation (individual stimulation of CPG–projecting PCs and non-CPG-projecting PCs).
When submitting your revised manuscript, please reinstate the previous Figure 4 —figure supplement 1. It is important to include this figure as a supplementary material because it is the only plot that shows the firing rates of the populations before subtracting the baselines.
We would also be grateful if you could give the title and abstract careful consideration. Please include in the title, a clear indication of the biological system under investigation. The abstract should not contain specialist abbreviations and acronyms where possible.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.71464.sa1Author response
Essential revisions:
1) The authors show that the simple spikes but not complex spikes ramp up before the initiation and termination of lick bouts. While this finding is important, the causality between the simple spikes and motor initiation/termination is unclear. In general, simple spikes fire more when the lick rate increases and confounds the interpretation of ramping activity (Reviewer 2). Furthermore, the trained licking behavior may not be well-timed enough to obtain a clear correlation between neuronal activity and behavior (Reviewer 1). These issues need to be clarified either by additional experiments or analysis.
2) The results from the optogenetic experiments did not sufficiently support the main conclusion. Because the light stimuli by itself caused significant changes in licking, the shift in the lick–peak may have been induced indirectly by this abnormal licking behavior and not directly by the changes in simple spike activity (Reviewer 1-1, Reviewer 2-4). In addition, because optogenetic stimuli did not specifically change simple spikes, the involvement of complex spikes cannot be ruled out (Reviewer 3).
We have made extensive changes to the manuscript based on the reviewers’ recommendations. This includes an attempted to better characterize the relationship between Purkinje cell activity and behavior. We now demonstrate that Purkinje cell activity has a temporally nested structure and is modulated on the timescale both of individual lick cycles and at the initiation and termination of licking bouts. This is reported in a new main figure. We also addressed important points regarding expectation signals and Purkinje cell activity (new supplemental figures). Further analyses are used to address important concerns regarding optogenetic stimulation experiments and their effect on behavior (new supplemental figure). Last, we have also substantially altered the text to clarify our results and include important information regarding alternative possibilities to our conclusions.
Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):
Specific concerns:
1) It is difficult to interpret the optogenetic perturbation experiments in absence of corresponding neural data. While recording during such experiments certainly imposes an additional experimental burden, it is necessary to understand what essential features of PC activity dictate behavioral performance. In particular, it is striking that the same stimulation only produces a delay in peak licking when licks are initiated less than one second from the stimulus. Corresponding neural data revealing what differs between trials with short and long latency lick initiations would enable a much stronger interpretation of this result.
As the reviewer points out, we found that optogenetic PC activation just prior to lick bout initiation only influences the timing of the ensuing peak licking rate when the perturbation precedes this behavioral epoch by <1 sec. This implies a limited temporal correspondence between PC preparatory activity and the resulting behavioral adjustment. Notably, this result is congruent with the cerebellum’s role in organizing behavioral timing over short periods. For longer intervals (i.e., >1 sec), it is likely that neural activity has time to evolve/reset to its normal trajectory such that the peak licking rate was not delayed.
This may be attributable to the interceding activity of other brain regions (i.e., the basal ganglia or the cerebral cortex). We have edited the text to make this logic clearer. Making multi-site recordings and/or additional optogenetic perturbations to test this hypothesis is beyond the scope of the current project.
Related to this concern, Figure 7 demonstrates that optogenetic stimulation initiates licks on some (but not all) trials immediately following stimulation. The spike above baseline in lick initiation probability (Figure 7G) occurs < 1 s following optogenetic stimulation. This may suggest that the delay in peak lick rate only occurs when non–voluntary licks are initiated too close to the target time (i.e. on the trials where cessation of optogenetic stimulation drives immediate rebound licking)? The sub vs supra–second timing result could therefore relate to 1) the animals' voluntary initiation of licking too close to the optogenetic perturbation on a subset of trials, or 2) the animals' immediate involuntary response to cessation of optogenetic stimulation on a subset of trials. These two possibilities may suggest different interpretations for how the cerebellum contributes to the behavior.
We agree this is a concern. For this exact reason, we focused our analysis on the subset of trials without licking during the photostimulus period (note how the licking rate is pinned to zero). We apologize for not making this point clearer. That said, we were motivated to addresses the reviewer’s concern further, so we separately analyzed the subset of trials with licking during the photostimulus period. A response delay was also observed in these trials, like that for the remaining trials without licking during the photostimulus period. By ruling out the possibility that licking itself during the photostimulus period somehow disrupted subsequent licking around the time of water allocation, we conclude that the delay is attributable to the PC activity perturbation. This new analysis is included as a new supplemental figure and the text has been edited to clarify these points.
2) It is not clear that the observed responses relate to motor initiation and termination. For example, considering figure 4C vs 4E, the Sspk ramping at lick offset is quite different for omission trials as compared to rewarded trials. If these signals primarily reflect a timing response for stopping the motor program, they should not depend so heavily on reward context. Alternatively, the initiation signals could also be interpreted in the context of expectation. This is very challenging to disentangle, however. In this behavior, the omission trials do not serve well to do so, as it is not possible to align the data appropriately. Figure 2C shows that the animals have a poor temporal estimation of reward delivery on average, and maintain near–peak licking across an approximate 5 second window in absence of reward. Thus, because animals do not have a strong estimation of when the reward will occur, alignment to 'expected reward time' on omission trials is unlikely to capture any underlying responses. Related to this, given that Wagner et al. showed that granule cells also have reward omission responses in Crus I – it is important to reconcile this difference, even if it is simply because omission responses may not be identifiable given this task design.
While it is difficult to fully discount the possibility that some PC activity during the task may reflect a representation of expectation, evidence argues against that it fully accounts for the activity we observed around lick bout initiation and termination. First, the differences in ramping activity just prior to lick bout termination likely stem from differences in the licking behavior between the two contexts. Specifically, the licking rate is substantially lower on average for water omission trials than for water allocation trials. Therefore, the termination response is apt to be smaller. Second, if PC activity was mainly related to the context of expectation, then, on the next trial after water omission, PC activity would likely be disrupted by the prior expectation error. However, the average spiking activity of the Crus I PC ensemble in these trials (i.e., an unexpected 20 s interval) looks essentially identical to the pattern observed during trials of regular water allocation (i.e., the expected 10 s interval they were trained on). This result implies that PC activity likely encodes parameters related to licking performance rather than expectation. We have articulated these points in the manuscript and include an additional supplementary figure to directly address this concern.
Also related to this concern, the discussion argues that "our results reinforce the idea that the cerebellum influences well–timed, regularly performed actions that are generated solely by motivation...". This may not be accurate, and illustrates a major confound in interpreting signals during this behavior. Certainly, naïve animals are driven solely by motivation. They do not, however, exhibit any predictively timed licking behavior. In contrast, trained animals have harnessed the reward–related sensory stimulus to generate a new predictive behavior. This difference between naïve and trained animals shows that reward does double duty in this task – it is both a sensory stimulus that has intrinsic positive valence (and is thus sought solely by motivation in naïve animals), and a sensory cue that can guide temporal learning. Because naïve animals exhibit the former but not the latter, these two roles are separable. However, this dual role of the sensory input in this task also makes the interpretation of the neural responses in the learned condition challenging. In other words, because the cue and reward are one in the same in this task, it is extremely difficult to disambiguate sensory, expectation/prediction, and motor related signals. I am concerned that the task design may preclude causal relationships between the neural activity and behavior as a result.
We added to the discussion to highlight the reviewer’s points. We also removed the phrase “generated solely by motivation” in the text.
Additional questions:
1) What is the depth of silicon probe recordings (what is the range of depths of the primary contacts for recorded PCs?). Does this overlap with imaging depths, or does the imaging data come from different regions than the electrophysiology? Do the Cspks recorded with electrophysiology exhibit the same behavior as those captured with calcium imaging?
Our electrophysiology and imaging approached sampled similar areas of Crus I and II because we performed the same, large craniotomy for both approaches (i.e., all animals were prepared the same way, irrespective of the recoding method, with the only difference being that we removed the cranial window prior to electrophysiology recording). We targeted a depth of <500 µm for silicon probe placement, which should correspond to the surface layers though it is difficult to truly ascertain because of deformation of the brain during penetration. Numerous prior reports have indicated that dendritic calcium events in Purkinje cells are an exacting proxy for complex spikes. Therefore, we elected to measure climbing-fiber-evoked responses optically. We have added additional information to the text to address these concerns.
2) What is the definition of a lick bout? Why is the average absolute lick rate (in s–1) pinned at 0 with no error for 2 seconds prior to lick bout initiation (e.g. Figure 3A)? It seems that bout initiations begin about 2.5 seconds prior to reward (Figure 3A bottom). Figures 1 and 2 show continuous licking on average for 5 seconds prior to reward. Doesn't this mean that there should be some non–zero lick rate prior to bout initiation? Alternatively, if these are licks defined by 2 seconds of preceding quiescence, shouldn't the initiations in the bottom of figure 2A bottom be further from the reward time?
To quantify lick-bout initiations, we identified well-isolated bouts so there was no ambiguity regarding when one bout stopped and the next started. Therefore, we set an arbitrary time in which the start of licking must be separated from other licks (i.e., absent of licks). We did not state this clearly, so we apologize for this omission. As the reviewer acknowledges, this period was >2 s long for the data presented in the figure. Hence, licking was pinned to 0, without error, for 2 s before initiation in lickaligned bouts (notably, this definition meant that there were uncategorized licks which were not included in the lick-aligned analysis). Also, the reviewer should note that the in the top of figure panel A, licking is aligned to the first lick. Whereas in the bottom, it is aligned to the time of expected water delivery. To address the reviewer’s concern, we have defined what a lick bout is in the text. We also state that we only included well separated bouts, based on the criterion described above, in the analysis.
3) It looks like there is a Cspk response to the last lick in figure 6I – is this significant? Is this response different between rewarded and reward–omission trials?
A significance test is now reported in the manuscript. There is no difference between rewarded and reward-omission trials.
4) How are lick initiations defined surrounding optogenetic stimulation? Optogenetic stimulation suppresses licking, and results in rebound responses afterwards. When these occur in the pre–water period, they are defined as initiations. Why aren't the post water licks that follow the quiescence period of optogenetic stimulation considered initiations? Would not the same analysis show in 7G (related to 7F) yield a similar spike in initiation probability if performed on the data from 7D?
For the dataset in the figure panel, we used the same criteria to define lick bout initiations surrounding optogenetic stimulation as for the interleaved control trials (the blue and black lines, respectively). Again, we arbitrarily used a 2 s period of non-licking to define well-separated lick bouts. As this was the period of earliest lick-bout initiations, there were plenty of bouts for inclusion. In contrast, the rebound in licking after the optogenetically induced period of suppression in figure panel D would not qualify as well separate initiations based on our criterion because this period of suppress licking was short lived, lasting several hundred milliseconds. As noted in the text, the licking pattern during the optogenetic perturbation was highly disturbed: rhythmicity was disrupted with the licks appearing erratic and mostly ceased altogether. Shortly after, licking resumed but also appeared irregular regarding both variance and rate. Therefore, it was difficult to categorize what type of licking this was (i.e., whether it was an initiation or a continuation of licking). Due to this ambiguity, we did not perform the same analysis for this dataset as we did for figure panel F. We have added additional descriptive information in the text regarding our criterion for lick bouts to address this concern.
5) It's a bit odd to argue that imaging was necessary because it is "challenging to reliably distinguish complex spike waveforms" (line 337), while at the same time making the case that cell sorting was validated based on the property that PC units were 'unambiguously identified by accompanying complex spikes in the recordings" (line 774).
By stating that it is "…challenging to reliably distinguish complex spike waveforms…", we did not mean to imply that we could not detect any complex spikes. Rather, we were trying to relay our lack of confidence in identifying all of them. For unambiguous PC classification, we only needed to identify SOME complex spikes in our unit recordings. However, to analyze how complex spikes represent behavioral attributes, we believed we needed to identify most of them. Furthermore, because imaging allowed us to measure calcium transients in many Purkinje cells simultaneously, we were able to increase the sample size significantly beyond what would have been feasible with electrophysiology. To address the reviewer’s concern, we have edited the text to make this point.
Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):
I recommend the following analysis, presentation, and discussion to improve the paper.
For the first major comment, the authors need to evaluate the encoding of each bout of licking by cross–correlation or event triggered averaging, and separate it from the encoding of initiation and termination (e.g. using linear model).
Cross-correlation analysis of each lick bout requires some variability in the statistics of the behavior. Unfortunately, the intervals between licks, when examined across session-trials from individual animals, are extremely regular, precluding single-bout analysis of lick rate and PC activity. For this reason, we had to examine all licking across all animals to see any correlation. We considered using linear models as the reviewer suggested to address how the broader behavior (i.e., each bout) is encoded by PC activity. However, such models require distinct variables to be present and absent in different trials. Because every bout of licking includes a start and stop to the action, we could not parse trials into those that include lick-bout initiation but not termination, and vice-versa. Linear models, therefore, are not appropriate.
To address the reviewer’s concern, we performed a new analysis focused on the representation of individual licks in PC firing. Our new results (Figure 3), show remarkable heterogeneity in lick-phase encoding in the PC ensemble. Important to the reviewer’s concern, the entrainment strength of PC activity to individual licks, was not predictive of their overall firing pattern when examined on a cell-bycell basis. This is consistent with a view of a rich representation of nested behavioral variables at different timescales for periodically performed, discontinuous movements.
For the second comment, they should further analyze the negatively modulating Purkinje cells, shown in Figure 5C. They also need to display the histology of their recording sites (Schematic drawing in Figure 2A is not enough). Also, they should discuss about the potential bias in recording of Purkinje cells.
In our dataset, there are not enough negatively modulating PCs for further analysis. We were hesitant to perform additional experiments in a concerted attempt to only identify negatively modulating PCs because this makes an a priori assumption of what type of PC activity is important for task performance. Histological confirmation of our recording sites was unnecessary because we could clearly target our probes to either Crus I or Crus II under visual guidance. As mentioned above, our craniotomy site was large (atypical for in vivo electrophysiology recording), exposing the surfaces of both Crus I and Crus II which are clearly distinguishable from one another based on the sulcus dividing them, allowing targeted recordings to either lobule under investigator choice. These are the same areas that we have recording from for the past 5 years with little ambiguity (see Gaffield et al. 2016, 2017, 2018, and 2019). We have now specified this logic in the text. Last, as mentioned above, we have extensively edited our text to discuss how our results fit in with a scheme of antagonistic cerebellar modules.
For the third comment, I recommend the spike rete–based display for the data presentations, which should include the spontaneous spike rates.
As mentioned in our response above, we chose to show z-scored spike rates as this is the current standard in the neurophysiology field. That said, we changed how we displayed the activity of individual cells as spike rates.
For the fourth and fifth comments, related to the first comment, if the Purkinje cells contribute to controlling each of licking bout, the observation in the optogenetic experiments could be explained. The authors need to consider the possibility and improve the interpretation and conclusion.
We again apologize for not emphasizing how PCs encode additional behavioral variables related to an online representation of the licking bout itself. We have edited the text to make this point clearer and have elaborated on this point to improve the interpretation and conclusion of our optogenetic results with this in mind.
Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):
– Figure 3 E shows the difference in simple spike onset between pre and post licking. It seems both Crus I and II were pooled together, but this analysis should be conducted separately in different lobules.
The reviewer is correct, PC activity from both Crus I and Crus II were originally pooled together because the effect of early ramping onset times for anticipatory licking was similar. However, in response to the reviewer’s suggestion, we now present the analysis separately for each lobule.
– Figure 3 F shows representative PC activity relative to the first lick. Is this trace from pre–water lick or post–water lick? Representative traces from both pre–water and post–water lick should be presented to demonstrate the difference in the ramp onset.
Figure 3F showed activity from three representative PCs aligned to pre-water licking bouts. We now include PC activity from post-water licking bouts; this is presented in a supplemental figure. The timing difference in PC activity relative to the types of licking are starkly evident in these examples.
– Figure 4C shows spike rate changes before the termination of the lick. As the firing rate in Figure 3C–D is plotted in (s–1), this data should be also presented using the same unit for comparison.
Figures 3 and 4 have been edited for continuity as recommended by the reviewer. We chose to show z-scored spike rates as this is the standard metric for reporting activity profiles across cells.
– Figure 5D, please revise color coding for easier interpretation.
We changed the color scheme of this figure panel for easier interpretation.
– Figure 5D shows a significant number of cells in Crus II ramped up for both pre–water and post-water licking. It would be informative to compare the ramp onset between pre-water and post–water licking for these cells. The result will show whether the same cells responded differently to the self–initiated and externally triggered licking.
We identified 8 PCs that showed ramping activity for both pre- and post-water licking. Some cells had clear timing differences between the two different licking contexts, ramping earlier for anticipatory licks compared to reactive consummatory licks (see PC1 and PC2 in the new supplemental figure). Across these 8 cells, the onset time of activity ramping relative to pre- and post-water licking was -0.300 ± 0.114 ms and -0.1125 ± 0.242 ms, respectively. Although there was a clear trend in the data, the difference was insignificant (p = 0.07; Student’s t-test). This implies that PCs tuned to either pre- or postwater licking also contributed to the overall timing difference observed in the entire PC ensemble.
– In Figures 7 and 8, optogenetic stimulation of PCs was conducted with ChR2. While this experiment was performed to test the involvement of simple spikes, it could be mediated by alteration in complex spikes. Were complex spikes affected by the stimuli? Representative traces from raw data for Figure 7A should be presented to address this point.
As mentioned in the text, we have recently shown that direct optogenetic activation of PCs can elicit responses resembling complex spikes. However, this effect is dependent on the intensity of the optogenetic stimulus (Bonnan et al., 2021). Although we used a consistent light power for the photostimulus across animals, we cannot control for the inevitable light-power variability within the stimulation site (e.g., the edges of the optical fiber tip or with depth), even if we recorded PC activity simultaneously with the optogenetic stimulus. Therefore, we cannot fully discount the possibility that complex-spike-like bursts were elicited in some PCs, in addition to changes in simple spiking when ChR2expressing PCs were photostimulated. In the end, we want to be conservative regarding our interpretation of the effect of ChR2-induced stimulation of PCs. We have edited the text to make this rationale clearer.
– Figures 8B and 8F show the delayed peak in lick rate with PC activation. This finding should be supported with additional statistical analysis.
We now include statistical tests for the delay in lick rate with PC photostimulation. Note that the timing differences shown in Figure 8E and 8F are summarized in Figure 8G, with accompanying statistical tests (asterisks denote a significant timing difference relative to control).
[Editors' note: further revisions were suggested prior to acceptance, as described below.]
Reviewer 1:
I have several comments below directly related to our initial reviews. I think the paper is interesting, and that they could revise one more time with no experiments to address these issues.
The main concerns about the initial submission focused largely on the question of whether the authors' data and analysis support the central claim that the measured neural activity patterns are causally related to motor initiation and termination. In response to these concerns, the authors have provided several important clarifications and some new analyses. While I remain positive about the potential impact of the findings in this study, and would support this publication for eLife, there are some important remaining issues directly related to those central concerns.
1. Now that the authors have clarified the analysis related to figure 4, I am concerned about its main conclusions. Specifically, the authors state that "the average simple spiking rate began to ramp earlier for exploratory licking trials, when the movements were initiated prior to water allocation, compared with that for trials in which consummatory licking commenced immediately after water became available" (lines 205–208).
This is a key point meant to distinguish neural responses on planned vs unplanned movements, but I am not convinced by the underlying analysis. The authors have subtracted z–scored neural data for pre and post water licks, and used the difference to suggest that pre water licks have an associated neural responses that "starts earlier". However, this analysis conflates response amplitude and timing, as a subtraction only reveals when responses diverge (the negative latency in the difference trace cannot be taken to mean that the larger response began earlier). To specifically address timing (and not amplitude), the responses must first be scaled, and then subtracted. Because a smaller response takes longer to cross the same amplitude threshold, a subtraction will show a difference right away, even if the responses start at the same time. In addition, the quantification as performed in Figure 4F will necessarily indicate shorter latencies for larger responses.
It seems very likely that a scaled subtraction will show no latency difference for the data in figure 4C and D, and this would contradict the conclusion of a timing difference in neural responses for planned and unplanned movement.
Provided the reviewer’s concern, we have removed z-scoring from the paper. The timing difference between the two licking contexts is apparent in the plots of population activity, now represented as the change in firing rate. The differences are rigorously quantified in the positively modulating PCs (i.e., those that account for the ramping in the population response) in Figure 4G and H (the subtracted z-score panels were always for illustration purposes). Regarding Figure 4F (now 4H), this analysis is based on an assessment of the simple spiking pattern of individual PCs (deviation from baseline that was independent of amplitude), so the reviewer’s concern is not applicable.
The data in Figure 4 do, however, show a much smaller response on unplanned movements, which is interesting. However, it is unclear whether or not the licking is different in this case? This needs to be shown for the associated neural traces in figure 4. The overall data suggest that lick bouts are relatively homogenous at their initiation. Thus, these data may indicate that the amplitude of the neural response does not reflect lick rate, and the timing of the response does not relate to prepared or unprepared movement. As reviewer 2 notes, PC activity also does not seem to represent peak lick rate timing.
As we showed in Figure 6, water-reactive (post-water) licking bouts have a higher peak rate of licking compared to exploratory (pre-water) licking bouts. This result is now presented in Figure 4. We did not find a significant change in the peak simple spiking response around the time of peak licking between the different types of licking. This likely owes to the relatively small difference in peak lick rate between these two contexts (about 1 s-1). As shown in Figure 2—figure supplement 2, differences in PC simple spike firing are only apparent with large differences in licking rate (e.g., between 3 and 6 s-1). These comparisons and discussion points have been added to the manuscript.
Together with the above, I therefore remain unclear on the authors model linking neural activity and behavior, and whether the recorded activity relates to motor preparation / planning / execution of some kind.
Other important issues:
2. In reading the other reviewers concerns about z–scoring and the authors responses, I realized that the z–scoring is performed to different baselines for different analyses, and not to the mean spike rate across the trial or a common reference period across analyses (the specifics of the z–scoring are not in the methods). This can be seen in difference between figures 4C and 5C, for example. I am concerned about this practice for a couple of reasons:
2.1) it means that the amplitude of neural signals cannot be compared across different figures and conditions. This makes it challenging to interpret the relationship between firing changes and behavior.
As mentioned above, to facilitate comparisons between figures, we removed z-scoring. Instead, we use changes in PC simple spike firing rate. Of course, this comes at the cost of not having an inherent representation of the significance of the change, which is the advantage of z-scored responses.
2.2) to disambiguate other explanations for the neural data in this paper, it would be helpful to further leverage conditions where behavior differs. For example, in Figure 4C, there are trials where no licks occur until after presentation of water. This affords the opportunity to ask whether or not there was any increase in spike rate before water allocation in absence of licking (and thus test whether changes in spike rate might reflect something other than motor initiation). However, the authors have z–scored to the mean immediately preceding post–water licks, which would obscure any such changes (Figure 4 supp. 2 may show such an increase pre–water?). While I am sympathetic to the authors' arguments that z–scoring is commonplace for neural recording data, the implementation here is not ideal for evaluating the relationship between spiking and behavior.
For our new analysis, we calculated baseline from the activity level measured across all non-licking periods for each cell during recording (i.e., we did not baseline immediately before the onset of licking). We did not observe an increase in spiking activity prior to water allocation in post-water licking. For plots in Figure 5, we also show activity baselined to the activity during the preceding licking. We believe this facilitates comparisons across figures. To further address the reviewer’s point regarding nonmotor signals, we also analyzed the change in PC firing in reward-absent trials without licking. There was no response; this new data is included as a new supplemental figure. We also now cite the findings of Sendhilnathan et al. 2020 who identified widespread movement-related activity, but not reward or reward expectation activity (at least in the absence of learning), in Crus I and II PCs of monkeys performing an overtrained motor task. We think our results are congruent with this observation.
3. In the first round of reviews, the question of how to appropriately disambiguate expectation based on omission trials was raised, given the animals imprecise expectation of the time of water delivery. This concern necessitates a more convincing analysis in order to support the authors statements in the discussion regarding expectation signals. For example, with complex spiking on omission trials, alignment to the first lick after the time of water expectation would provide a more appropriate timepoint to indicate the animals' expectation. Even better would be to look at the moment when lick bouts start to decrease / are terminated on omission trials, as this is the timepoint when the animals' behavior indicates recognition that the expected reward is not present (and this time has previously been shown to reveal such expectation signals, at least in some conditions). There may indeed be no evidence of expectation signals in this behavior, but in absence of such analysis to evaluate the question appropriately, it seems premature to make such conclusions.
We previously attempted this analysis. However, for climbing-fiber-induced PC activity, there were too few omission trials to generate a clear result.
4. This point is only a suggestion, but I think others may have the same confusion regarding figure 8. The difference between 5 and 10 second trials is not that licking ramps up more slowly on 10 second trials – rather, the mean lick plots shown here speak to the probability of lick bout initiation across trials. On single trials, licking just goes from nothing to the patterned bout rate. On average, however, this manifests as a ramp due to variability in the onset of lick bout times, and the increase in probability of initiation as the trial progresses. The essential feature of 10 second trials is that lick bouts start on average later than they do for 5 second trials. This necessarily means that lick bouts are more likely to be outside of the 1.25 second window from optogenetic stimulation defined by the analysis in 8F. However, if the same analysis from figure 8E and F were performed for 10 second trials (<1 vs > 1.25 second initiations from stimulation), it should yield the same result. It is likely that the average licking for 10 second trials following stimulation only looks different because it is weighted so much more heavily toward bouts initiated >1.25 seconds from stimulation.
This analysis would greatly enhance the authors point that these effects are all about the duration from optogenetic stimulation when the animal tries to lick, and not about the absolute duration of the trial (as many will likely assume based on the figure and its description). Perhaps this seems obvious, but such clarification could provide strong support for the authors arguments related to the final figure.
At the urging of Review 2, we removed Figure 8. The reviewer’s point is now moot.
Reviewer 2:
The paper contains very interesting topics and findings and has a potential impact worthy of eLife, but I think it is still too immature to be published with minor revisions. I think the opinion of revising it again without experiments is very valid.
I also agree with the concerns of Reviewer #1. In particular, I have exactly the same concerns about the main concern and #2 issue, and I think they are very important points. So below is a summary of my other opinions.
The authors focus on lick–bout initiation and termination, and make the central claim that the lick cycle is controlled by the CPG in the brainstem, and that the cerebellum contributes to its initiation and termination.
This is a great improvement over the previous version. In particular, the detailed analysis shows that the cerebellar Crus I and II encode individual Licks, Lick rates, and information about lick–bout initiation and termination, which is a very interesting and exciting finding. The authors focus on lick–bout initiation and termination, and make the central claim that the lick cycle is controlled by CPG in the brainstem, and that the cerebellum contributes to its initiation and termination. But the experiments of optogenetics do not fully demonstrate the causality of this central claim. The story focusing only on the current central claim may be somewhat unreasonable.
Again, considering that their central claim is that the Lick cycle is controlled by the CPG in the brainstem, and that the cerebellum contributes to its initiation and termination, the optogenetic experiments performed to demonstrate causality do not clearly support this. First, regarding the initiation, considering that the neural activity in Figure 4 is a rising ramping activity, one would expect that the PC stimulation in Figure 7F would help the ramping and accelerate the initiation time. However, the result was rather the opposite. During stimulation, no initiation occurs, and in fact, it appears to strongly inhibit the initiation of licking (also in Figure 8B). Rather, initiation occurred as a rebound after stimulation. It needs to be properly explained why this is the opposite.
Next, regarding termination, considering that Figure 5 also shows ramp–up activity before the termination, PC stimulation at the peak of lick rate in Figure 7D would be expected to accelerate the termination time. Although this terminated the lick cycle as expected, licking started again after the stimulus offset. Then, the possibility remains that the stimulus only temporarily suppressed individual licking bouts, but did not terminate the lick cycle.
We did not mean to imply that the lick cycle is under the sole control of a brainstem CPG. Likely, multiple brain regions cooperate to determine the performance of this motor behavior. In fact, we show that the lick cycle is encoded by cerebellar activity and its perturbation can affect rhythmicity. We focused on lick bout initiation and termination because these parameters are poorly understood. As explained in our prior rebuttal, the licking behavior that we studied was quite regular across trial conditions. Due to the low amount of variability in the licking response, it is difficult to draw conclusions between changes in PC firing and licking rate on a trial-by-trial basis which would provide insight to the reviewer’s point. Certainly, this will be the goal in a subsequent study.
In about half the trials, the mice did not resume licking after stopping in response to the optogenetically induced perturbation of PC activity. This result is now reported in the text. We believe this is in line with a robust “stop” to action rather than a temporary pause of the behavior. In trials where the mice resumed licking, it is impossible for us to disambiguate whether this is a stop-restart action rather than a true pause in the behavior as the mice are likely to continue to be motivated to consume water rewards after the optogenetically induced cessation of licking and therefore reinitiate licking shortly after they had abruptly stopped.
Figure 8 seems to have nothing to do with their central claim. I think this is a different story: The initiation time of the Lick cycle influences the subsequent motor plan (Licking peak time), as they stated (L557). This is interesting in itself, as it is related to Figure 2E, but I feel that the change in story here is abrupt, as the story has been following the central claim up to this point. I think some readers may find it hard to follow. I think it would be better if the story were more consistent.
L557 "a delay or discoordination in the transition to movement initiation, which is normally signaled by ramping PC activity, disrupts and/or delays the remaining motor plan, leading to a mistimed action."
We eliminated this figure based on the reviewer’s urging.
As for the CF signal in Figure 6, it does not encode individual licks, which seems inconsistent with the Nature paper by Welsh et al. 1995. It would be good to have a discussion on why the different results were obtained.
We did not make this claim. In fact, we previously examined CF-induced activity in Crus II PCs aligned to individual licks in water-consummation bouts and found a result like Welsh et al. 1995 (see Gaffield et al., 2016). In our current manuscript, we focused on CF activity aligned to the initiation and termination of licking which Welsh et al. did not do. We now reference this prior work in the discussion.
Related to a concern of Reviewer 1, when calculating the firing rate, the firing rate for the period immediately before the event of interest is set to 0. Therefore, the 0 value is different in each plot, and the activity immediately before the event is missing. It is preferable to display the plots with the spontaneous firing rate set to 0 (at least in Supplementary Figures).
For our plots showing the change in PC firing rate, baseline is determined from the activity level for all non-licking periods during the recording. We believe this meets the reviewer’s criteria of displaying plots with “spontaneous” firing set to 0, though that definition of “spontaneous” is subjective in a continuously behaving animal.
[Editors' note: further revisions were suggested prior to acceptance, as described below.]
However, during the consultation, it was brought to our attention that some statements regarding the optogenetic experiments are inappropriate. Purkinje cells' activity could be classified into two types:
1. PC activity for individual licking movements (as shown in Figure 3).
2. PC activity for the transition (initiation and termination) of licking cycles.
Therefore, the optogenetic stimulation of PCs should influence both individual licking movements and the transition of licking bouts. In the current manuscript, most statements focus on #2 without mentioning the influence of #1. It gives an impression of an over–interpretation or over–simplification and may lead to misunderstanding. Below are some examples:
We thank the reviewers for their positive assessment of our revised manuscript and appreciate the importance of this last concern. We have addressed this point in our revised manuscript, as detailed below.
– L38 (Abstract)
Optogenetic perturbation of PC activity disrupted the behavior in both initiating and terminating licking bouts, confirming a causative role in movement organization.
– L536 (Results)
Together, these results indicate that PC photostimulation can both initiate and terminate bouts of licking, depending on the timing of the activity perturbation relative to the licking context, indicating a role for PC activity in coordinating motor event transitions.
We edited both sentences to indicate that we also examined the effect of perturbing PC activity on cycles of individual licking movements.
– L717 (Discussion)
Our optogenetic experiment provided causal evidence that Crus I and II PC activity influences movement performance.
This is a generic sentence that is inclusive of all our findings. The effect of PC activity perturbation on individual licking movements and motor transitions is separately elaborated on in the two sentences that follow it.
– L741 (Conclusion of Discussion)
In summary, PC activity both represents and causes motor–event transitions, influencing the coordination of explicitly timed, volitional movements to improve the temporal consistency across repeat cycles of goal–directed action.
This concluding sentence been modified to be more inclusive of all our findings.
The above examples can be read as such that the causality between PC activation and behavior transition is clear (#2). However, this is not the case because activity #1 is also involved. Please take a moment to consider this point and clarify the statements to avoid confusion. Below are some recommendations from one of the reviewers.
1) Describe activity #1 clearly in the abstract and others. Activity #1 should be written with respect to previous works (e.g., Bryant et al.). Novel aspects of this study, such as analysis, etc., should be described more specifically.
We edited the manuscript to describe PC activity related to individual licks in the Abstract and more thoroughly in the Discussion. We have previously specified our use of multiple modes of in vivo activity measurements and use of optogenetics, both in combination with analysis of a periodically performed discontinuous motor behavior, which makes our study unique. Regarding Bryant et al., we have repeatedly cited this manuscript by the Heck Lab throughout our manuscript to call attention to the importance of this work.
2) The optogenetics experiment should not be discussed by solely focusing on the causality between activity #2 and behavior but should consider the causality between activities #1/#2 and behavior (rewrite L505).
We have rewritten this sentence to be inclusive of both types of PC activity and how their perturbation could explain the optogenetic result.
3) Describe the conclusion of the optogenetics experiment, as "the experiment indicates that activities #1 and #2 contribute to the behavioral performance of Licking."
We added the statement that optogenetic perturbation of PC activity also effects individual cycles of licking.
4) As a Discussion of the optogenetics experiment, it should be stated that the impairment may be mediated by changes in either Activity #2 (especially the experiment with photostimulation during Licking) or Activity #1 (especially experiments with photostimulation during the licking initiation) or both. The authors may discuss a possible approach to separate the two: it may be necessary to develop new paradigms such as pathway–specific photostimulation (individual stimulation of CPG–projecting PCs and non-CPG-projecting PCs).
As requested, we have discussed this possibility. We also added that, in the future, targeting specific PC pathways (if they exist) may allow disambiguation of these effects.
When submitting your revised manuscript, please reinstate the previous Figure 4 —figure supplement 1. It is important to include this figure as a supplementary material because it is the only plot that shows the firing rates of the populations before subtracting the baselines.
We reinstated Figure 4—figure supplement 1 (now Figure 4—figure supplement 2).
We would also be grateful if you could give the title and abstract careful consideration. Please include in the title, a clear indication of the biological system under investigation. The abstract should not contain specialist abbreviations and acronyms where possible.
We edited the title to indicate that the experiments were performed in mice. We also updated the title to reflect the reviewers’ input outlined above.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.71464.sa2Article and author information
Author details
Funding
National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (NS1188401)
- Jason M Christie
National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (NS105958)
- Jason M Christie
National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (NS112289)
- Jason M Christie
Max Planck Florida Institute for Neuroscience (open access funding)
- Michael A Gaffield
- Jason M Christie
The funders had no role in study design, data collection, and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.
Acknowledgements
We thank Samantha Amat for laboratory assistance and the GENIE program (Janelia Research Campus, including Drs. Jayaraman, Kerr, Kim, Looger, and Svoboda) for freely providing GCaMP6f to the neuroscience community.
Ethics
All of the animals were handled according to approved Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (IACUC) protocols of the Max Planck Florida Institute for Neuroscience (Protocol Number: 18-009) . As detailed in Methods and materials, care was taken to minimize animal pain, suffering, and distress.
Senior Editor
- Tirin Moore, Stanford University, United States
Reviewing Editor
- Aya Ito-Ishida, Keio University School of Medicine, Japan
Reviewer
- Shogo Ohmae, Hokkaido University School of Medicine, Japan
Version history
- Received: June 20, 2021
- Preprint posted: June 24, 2021 (view preprint)
- Accepted: April 21, 2022
- Accepted Manuscript published: April 22, 2022 (version 1)
- Version of Record published: May 6, 2022 (version 2)
Copyright
© 2022, Gaffield et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
-
- 1,910
- Page views
-
- 329
- Downloads
-
- 4
- Citations
Article citation count generated by polling the highest count across the following sources: PubMed Central, Crossref, Scopus.
Download links
Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)
Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)
Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)
Further reading
-
- Neuroscience
How does the human brain combine information across the eyes? It has been known for many years that cortical normalization mechanisms implement ‘ocularity invariance’: equalizing neural responses to spatial patterns presented either monocularly or binocularly. Here, we used a novel combination of electrophysiology, psychophysics, pupillometry, and computational modeling to ask whether this invariance also holds for flickering luminance stimuli with no spatial contrast. We find dramatic violations of ocularity invariance for these stimuli, both in the cortex and also in the subcortical pathways that govern pupil diameter. Specifically, we find substantial binocular facilitation in both pathways with the effect being strongest in the cortex. Near-linear binocular additivity (instead of ocularity invariance) was also found using a perceptual luminance matching task. Ocularity invariance is, therefore, not a ubiquitous feature of visual processing, and the brain appears to repurpose a generic normalization algorithm for different visual functions by adjusting the amount of interocular suppression.
-
- Neuroscience
Tastes typically evoke innate behavioral responses that can be broadly categorized as acceptance or rejection. However, research in Drosophila melanogaster indicates that taste responses also exhibit plasticity through experience-dependent changes in mushroom body circuits. In this study, we develop a novel taste learning paradigm using closed-loop optogenetics. We find that appetitive and aversive taste memories can be formed by pairing gustatory stimuli with optogenetic activation of sensory neurons or dopaminergic neurons encoding reward or punishment. As with olfactory memories, distinct dopaminergic subpopulations drive the parallel formation of short- and long-term appetitive memories. Long-term memories are protein synthesis-dependent and have energetic requirements that are satisfied by a variety of caloric food sources or by direct stimulation of MB-MP1 dopaminergic neurons. Our paradigm affords new opportunities to probe plasticity mechanisms within the taste system and understand the extent to which taste responses depend on experience.