Intracerebral mechanisms explaining the impact of incidental feedback on mood state and risky choice

  1. Romane Cecchi
  2. Fabien Vinckier
  3. Jiri Hammer
  4. Petr Marusic
  5. Anca Nica
  6. Sylvain Rheims
  7. Agnès Trebuchon
  8. Emmanuel J Barbeau
  9. Marie Denuelle
  10. Louis Maillard
  11. Lorella Minotti
  12. Philippe Kahane
  13. Mathias Pessiglione
  14. Julien Bastin  Is a corresponding author
  1. Grenoble Alpes University, France
  2. Université de Paris, France
  3. University Hospital in Motol, Czech Republic
  4. Centre Hospitalier Universitaire de Rennes, France
  5. Hospices Civils de Lyon, France
  6. Hôpital de la Timone, France
  7. CNRS, University of Toulouse Paul Sabatier, France
  8. Centre Hospitalier Universitaire de Toulouse, France
  9. University Hospital of Nancy, France
  10. Centre Hospitalier Universitaire de Grenoble, France
  11. Pitié-Salpêtrière Hospital, France

Abstract

Identifying factors whose fluctuations are associated with choice inconsistency is a major issue for rational decision theory. Here, we investigated the neuro-computational mechanisms through which mood fluctuations may bias human choice behavior. Intracerebral EEG data were collected in a large group of subjects (n = 30), while they were performing interleaved quiz and choice tasks that were designed to examine how a series of unrelated feedbacks affects decisions between safe and risky options. Neural baseline activity preceding choice onset was confronted first to mood level, estimated by a computational model integrating the feedbacks received in the quiz task, and then to the weighting of option attributes, in a computational model predicting risk attitude in the choice task. Results showed that 1) elevated broadband gamma activity (BGA) in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) and dorsal anterior insula (daIns) was respectively signaling periods of high and low mood, 2) increased vmPFC and daIns BGA respectively promoted and tempered risk taking by overweighting gain versus loss prospects. Thus, incidental feedbacks induce brain states that correspond to different moods and bias the evaluation of risky options. More generally, these findings might explain why people experiencing positive (or negative) outcome in some part of their life tend to expect success (or failure) in any other.

Data availability

Due to ethical restrictions on data sharing, we are unable to share raw data for this manuscript to preserve participant anonymity. However, anonymized iEEG data in BIDS format can be made available upon request to the corresponding author (JB) and source data files with anonymized regression estimates are available for download.The custom codes used to (i) extract the different frequency envelopes, and in particular the broadband gamma activity (BGA), from the raw intracranial data, (ii) perform the regression analyses at recording site level, and (iii) compute the second level statistics (across all recording sites of a ROI) are available at: https://gitlab.com/romane-cecchi/publications-code/2022-ieeg-mood-and-risky-choice (Cecchi, 2022; copy archived at https://archive.softwareheritage.org/swh:1:dir:ec43d71a97bf2e3c56b38688eaea2dfde19ad573).

Article and author information

Author details

  1. Romane Cecchi

    Grenoble Alpes University, Grenoble, France
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0002-6149-939X
  2. Fabien Vinckier

    Université de Paris, Paris, France
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  3. Jiri Hammer

    University Hospital in Motol, Prague, Czech Republic
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  4. Petr Marusic

    University Hospital in Motol, Prague, Czech Republic
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  5. Anca Nica

    Centre Hospitalier Universitaire de Rennes, Rennes, France
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  6. Sylvain Rheims

    Hospices Civils de Lyon, Lyon, France
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  7. Agnès Trebuchon

    Hôpital de la Timone, Marseille, France
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0002-8632-3454
  8. Emmanuel J Barbeau

    Brain and Cognition Research Centre (CerCo), CNRS, University of Toulouse Paul Sabatier, Toulouse, France
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0003-0836-3538
  9. Marie Denuelle

    Centre Hospitalier Universitaire de Toulouse, Toulouse, France
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  10. Louis Maillard

    University Hospital of Nancy, Nancy, France
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  11. Lorella Minotti

    Centre Hospitalier Universitaire de Grenoble, Grenoble, France
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  12. Philippe Kahane

    Centre Hospitalier Universitaire de Grenoble, Grenoble, France
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  13. Mathias Pessiglione

    Pitié-Salpêtrière Hospital, Paris, France
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  14. Julien Bastin

    Grenoble Alpes University, Grenoble, France
    For correspondence
    julien.bastin@univ-grenoble-alpes.fr
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0002-0533-7564

Funding

Université Grenoble Alpes (ANR-17-CE37-0018)

  • Julien Bastin

Université Grenoble Alpes (ANR-18-CE28-0016)

  • Julien Bastin

Université Grenoble Alpes (ANR-13-TECS-0013)

  • Philippe Kahane
  • Julien Bastin

The Czech Science Foundation (20-21339S)

  • Jiri Hammer

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Ethics

Human subjects: All patients gave written, informed consent before their inclusion in the present study, which received approval from the local ethics committees (CPP 09-CHUG-12, study 0907; CPP18-001b / 2017-A03248-45; IRB00003888; CER No. 47-0913).

Copyright

© 2022, Cecchi et al.

This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License permitting unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.

Metrics

  • 1,147
    views
  • 253
    downloads
  • 9
    citations

Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.

Download links

A two-part list of links to download the article, or parts of the article, in various formats.

Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)

Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)

Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)

  1. Romane Cecchi
  2. Fabien Vinckier
  3. Jiri Hammer
  4. Petr Marusic
  5. Anca Nica
  6. Sylvain Rheims
  7. Agnès Trebuchon
  8. Emmanuel J Barbeau
  9. Marie Denuelle
  10. Louis Maillard
  11. Lorella Minotti
  12. Philippe Kahane
  13. Mathias Pessiglione
  14. Julien Bastin
(2022)
Intracerebral mechanisms explaining the impact of incidental feedback on mood state and risky choice
eLife 11:e72440.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.72440

Share this article

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.72440

Further reading

    1. Computational and Systems Biology
    Dylan C Sarver, Muzna Saqib ... G William Wong
    Research Article

    Organ function declines with age, and large-scale transcriptomic analyses have highlighted differential aging trajectories across tissues. The mechanism underlying shared and organ-selective functional changes across the lifespan, however, still remains poorly understood. Given the central role of mitochondria in powering cellular processes needed to maintain tissue health, we therefore undertook a systematic assessment of respiratory activity across 33 different tissues in young (2.5 months) and old (20 months) mice of both sexes. Our high-resolution mitochondrial respiration atlas reveals: (1) within any group of mice, mitochondrial activity varies widely across tissues, with the highest values consistently seen in heart, brown fat, and kidney; (2) biological sex is a significant but minor contributor to mitochondrial respiration, and its contributions are tissue-specific, with major differences seen in the pancreas, stomach, and white adipose tissue; (3) age is a dominant factor affecting mitochondrial activity, especially across most brain regions, different fat depots, skeletal muscle groups, eyes, and different regions of the gastrointestinal tract; (4) age effects can be sex- and tissue-specific, with some of the largest effects seen in pancreas, heart, adipose tissue, and skeletal muscle; and (5) while aging alters the functional trajectories of mitochondria in a majority of tissues, some are remarkably resilient to age-induced changes. Altogether, our data provide the most comprehensive compendium of mitochondrial respiration and illuminate functional signatures of aging across diverse tissues and organ systems.

    1. Computational and Systems Biology
    Alessandro Bitto
    Insight

    Measuring mitochondrial respiration in frozen tissue samples provides the first comprehensive atlas of how aging affects mitochondrial function in mice.