Memory: Associating the old with the new

New memories can strengthen old memories if the recent and past experience contain elements that are semantically related.
  1. Chuqi Liu
  2. Gui Xue  Is a corresponding author
  1. State Key Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and Learning and the IDG/McGovern Institute for Brain Research, Beijing Normal University, China

Unlike the information stored in a computer disk or a digital camera, human memories are tightly connected and interact strongly with one another. For example, seeing a snake in the zoo may remind you of a previous encounter with a snake in the wild, or other dangerous experiences such as being chased by a dog (Figure 1A). But what are the consequences of spontaneously reviving these old memories? Does the new memory interfere and weaken the old memory (also known as retroactive interference), or does remembering the previous experience make the old memory stronger (retroactive facilitation)?

Testing the interaction between semantically related memories.

(A) Seeing a snake in the zoo may cause you to recall an old related memory, such as the time you saw a snake in the wild or another dangerous experience, like being chased by a dog. (B) To test what happens to old memories when they are re-activated and associated with new experiences, Antony et al. carried out a series of associative learning experiments. First, participants were asked to memorize 45 pairs of words (base learning), comprised of a cue (sick) and target (push). They were then given a list of 36 secondary word pairs to learn which either had the same cues and targets (No Δ condition), a new target (Δ Target), a new cue (Δ Cue), or a new cue and a new target (Δ Both), which were semantically related to the words in the base pair. For example, the base pair ‘sick-push’ could lead to the secondary word pairs ‘sick-shove’ or ‘ill-push’. No secondary pairs were created for the control group. (C) The schematic depicts how each condition impacted the participants’ memory of the base pairs five minutes or 48 hours after the learning period. All three conditions in which the words in the secondary list were changed to semantically related words improved participants’ memory of the original list compared to the control (dark blue bar), with the Δ Cue condition (orange bar) causing the largest effect and the Δ Both (yellow bar) the smallest.

Image credit: Panel A created by Saeoy; Panel C is based on data from Antony et al., 2022.

To help answer these questions, researchers have traditionally carried out associative learning experiments. In these studies, participants are asked to memorize a list of cues that are each associated with a target, such as pairs of words. They then learn a second list which contains the same cues paired with new targets, or new cues paired with the original targets. It has been proposed that if the cues or targets in the second list are semantically related to the ones initially provided (meaning they have a similar definition or share a common theme), this can influence the participants’ memory of the original list (Osgood, 1949).

Many studies have tested aspects of this theory, but the results are mixed. Some found that having identical cues led to retroactive interference, but this could be reduced by changing the targets to semantically related words (Dallett, 1962). In contrast, other studies found that having highly related cues (but not targets) led to retroactive facilitation and strengthened the participants’ ability to recall the original list (Hamilton, 1943).

Now, in eLife, James Antony and co-workers (from University of California, Davis, California Polytechnic State University and Princeton University) report a series of experiments that help to address these discrepancies (Antony et al., 2022). Unlike previous studies, the team investigated all the possible outcomes of having semantically related old and new target/cue pairs (ranging from identical to weakly related) using the same experimental framework. The impressive sample size and range of experimental conditions studied holds great potential to clarify the influence semantic relatedness has on old memories.

Participants initially learned 45 unrelated word pairs (known as the base pairs) and then an additional 36 pairs (secondary pairs). The secondary pairs were either identical to the original words (No Δ condition), contained new targets (Δ Target condition), new cues (Δ Cue condition), or new cues and new targets (Δ Both condition; Figure 1B). New cues and targets in the secondary lists either came from a set of human-generated words that have a narrow range of semantic relatedness to the base pairs (narrow set), or a computer-generated set that had a broader range of semantic relatedness (wider set). Participants (800 in total) were then tested on their ability to recall the original base pairs and the relevant secondary list five minutes or 48 hours after the learning period. The memory dependence of each base-second pair duo was subsequently calculated by quantifying the proportion of participants who simultaneously remembered or forgot the corresponding word pairs during the test.

This revealed that having secondary words that are narrowly or widely related to the original base pairs led to strong retroactive facilitation of the old memory, especially for the 48 hour delay condition (Figure 1C). In addition, the Δ Cue condition (where the target was held constant) had an overall more robust effect on facilitating old memories than the Δ Target condition. This demonstrates the asymmetric nature of semantic relatedness, with forward associations often being stronger than backward associations.

Antony et al. found that increasing the semantic relatedness between target words in the base and secondary pair improved retroactive facilitation and memory dependence in the majority of conditions. However, increasing the association between cue words did not have the same effect. Notably, when participants were asked to learn secondary Δ Target pairs made using the wider set, this interfered with their memory of the original list after a short delay, but strengthened it following a long delay. This suggests that retroactive interference is only temporary.

So, why does information that is learned later (i.e. the secondary list) benefit prior memories? One likely mechanism is the ‘recursive reminder hypothesis’ which posits that related old information is spontaneously reactivated and strengthened during new learning (Jacoby et al., 2015; Schlichting et al., 2015). This can be tested by comparing two learning methods: practicing retrieving the target after being provided with its matching cue vs. repeatedly studying the cue-target pairs.

In all the experiments conducted so far, Antony et al. had asked participants to learn the base and secondary pairs using the retrieval method. So, they repeated one of the conditions (narrow set of words, 48 hour delay) with 200 additional participants who were told to study the cues and targets instead. This condition was chosen because it had led to strong retroactive facilitation when both the cue and target were changed, a surprising result given that the base and secondary pairs did not share any overlapping words. In theory, retrieval practice should lead to more retroactive facilitation than re-study as participants are more likely to reactivate the old related memories when practicing retrieving words on the new list (Chan et al., 2006; Ye et al., 2020). However, Antony et al. found that re-study also led to retroactive facilitation but no memory dependence, and this retroactive facilitation effect was increased by amplifying the semantic relatedness of the target words.

Antony et al. set an excellent example of reconciling longstanding debates through comprehensive conditions, rigorous controls, and large sample sizes. Future studies could use neuroimaging to uncover the neural processes underlying the effects detected by Antony et al. and examine how learning changes the way old and new memories are represented in the brain. These findings will help scientists to better understand how different memories interact when they are semantically related to one another.

References

Article and author information

Author details

  1. Chuqi Liu

    Chuqi Liu is in the State Key Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and Learning and the IDG/McGovern Institute for Brain Research, Beijing Normal University, Beijing, China

    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0002-6578-7110
  2. Gui Xue

    Gui Xue is in the State Key Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and Learning and the IDG/McGovern Institute for Brain Research, Beijing Normal University, Beijing, China

    For correspondence
    gxue@bnu.edu.cn
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0001-7891-8151

Publication history

  1. Version of Record published: June 15, 2022 (version 1)

Copyright

© 2022, Liu and Xue

This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.

Metrics

  • 860
    views
  • 144
    downloads
  • 0
    citations

Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.

Download links

A two-part list of links to download the article, or parts of the article, in various formats.

Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)

Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)

Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)

  1. Chuqi Liu
  2. Gui Xue
(2022)
Memory: Associating the old with the new
eLife 11:e80030.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.80030

Further reading

    1. Biochemistry and Chemical Biology
    2. Neuroscience
    Katarzyna Marta Zoltowska, Utpal Das ... Lucía Chávez-Gutiérrez
    Research Article

    Amyloid β (Aβ) peptides accumulating in the brain are proposed to trigger Alzheimer’s disease (AD). However, molecular cascades underlying their toxicity are poorly defined. Here, we explored a novel hypothesis for Aβ42 toxicity that arises from its proven affinity for γ-secretases. We hypothesized that the reported increases in Aβ42, particularly in the endolysosomal compartment, promote the establishment of a product feedback inhibitory mechanism on γ-secretases, and thereby impair downstream signaling events. We conducted kinetic analyses of γ-secretase activity in cell-free systems in the presence of Aβ, as well as cell-based and ex vivo assays in neuronal cell lines, neurons, and brain synaptosomes to assess the impact of Aβ on γ-secretases. We show that human Aβ42 peptides, but neither murine Aβ42 nor human Aβ17–42 (p3), inhibit γ-secretases and trigger accumulation of unprocessed substrates in neurons, including C-terminal fragments (CTFs) of APP, p75, and pan-cadherin. Moreover, Aβ42 treatment dysregulated cellular homeostasis, as shown by the induction of p75-dependent neuronal death in two distinct cellular systems. Our findings raise the possibility that pathological elevations in Aβ42 contribute to cellular toxicity via the γ-secretase inhibition, and provide a novel conceptual framework to address Aβ toxicity in the context of γ-secretase-dependent homeostatic signaling.

    1. Computational and Systems Biology
    2. Neuroscience
    Sara Ibañez, Nilapratim Sengupta ... Christina M Weaver
    Research Article

    Normal aging leads to myelin alterations in the rhesus monkey dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC), which are positively correlated with degree of cognitive impairment. It is hypothesized that remyelination with shorter and thinner myelin sheaths partially compensates for myelin degradation, but computational modeling has not yet explored these two phenomena together systematically. Here, we used a two-pronged modeling approach to determine how age-related myelin changes affect a core cognitive function: spatial working memory. First, we built a multicompartment pyramidal neuron model fit to monkey dlPFC empirical data, with an axon including myelinated segments having paranodes, juxtaparanodes, internodes, and tight junctions. This model was used to quantify conduction velocity (CV) changes and action potential (AP) failures after demyelination and subsequent remyelination. Next, we incorporated the single neuron results into a spiking neural network model of working memory. While complete remyelination nearly recovered axonal transmission and network function to unperturbed levels, our models predict that biologically plausible levels of myelin dystrophy, if uncompensated by other factors, can account for substantial working memory impairment with aging. The present computational study unites empirical data from ultrastructure up to behavior during normal aging, and has broader implications for many demyelinating conditions, such as multiple sclerosis or schizophrenia.