Navigation: Where is that smell coming from?

Computational model reveals why pausing to sniff the air helps animals track a scent when they are far away from the source.
  1. Samuel Brudner
  2. Thierry Emonet  Is a corresponding author
  1. Department of Molecular, Cellular and Developmental Biology, and the Quantitative Biology Institute, Yale University, United States
  2. Department of Molecular, Cellular and Developmental Biology, the Quantitative Biology Institute, and the Department of Physics, Yale University, United States

Dogs, rodents and many other animals with a strong sense of smell often track a scent by keeping their nose to the ground, occasionally pausing to raise their heads and sniff something mysterious in the air (Jinn et al., 2020; Gire et al., 2016). However, exactly how alternating between these two behaviors helps animals navigate to the source of an odor remains unclear.

Airborne odors are transported by the wind, making them subject to the twisting and stretching of turbulent air motions. This results in animals downwind from an odor source being more likely to smell the odor intermittently, as air pockets containing the scent are interspersed with periods of clean air (Celani et al., 2014; Connor et al., 2018). Studies in insects suggest that animals surge upwind when they detect a smell in order to keep in contact with these turbulent odor plumes; when no odor is detected, they cast crosswind instead (Álvarez-Salvado et al., 2018; Demir et al., 2020; Kennedy, 1983; Flügge, 1934). Now, in eLife, Nicola Rigolli, Gautam Reddy, Agnese Seminara and Massimo Vergassola report how pausing to sniff the air when casting crosswind helps animals navigate towards the source of an odor (Rigolli et al., 2022).

To investigate how alternating behaviors impacts odor navigation, the team (who are based at institutes in France, Italy and the United States) designed a virtual search environment by simulating an odor dispersing downwind over a large area. The set-up created a challenging search scenario, including a low probability of encountering an odor pocket far from the source. Using machine learning, computer programs trained ‘artificial navigating agents’ to find the origin of the smell as quickly as possible (Sutton and Barto, 2019). During their search, these artificial agents were allowed to alternate between ‘walking’ while sniffing close to the ground and stopping to smell the air. Information gathered from these behaviors allowed agents to decide where to go next. After each attempt, the agents could use feedback about their previous search times to modify their strategy in the next trial. Although the researchers did not impose any explicit strategy or solution, agents reliably learnt that stopping to sniff the air sped up their search, even though it required them to pause.

Notably, trained agents mostly stopped to smell the air during the initial phase of their search before they had detected any odor. This suggests that alternating between ground and air sniffing helps agents to explore areas with dispersed levels of odor more efficiently (Figure 1A). Once agents successfully encounter an airborne cue, this signals that they have entered an odor rich zone and their rate of alternation drastically decreases.

The optimal strategy for finding the source of a smell.

(A) When tracking the source of an odor, animals alternate between walking while sniffing the ground (brown) and pausing to sniff the air (blue). Animals sniff the air more frequently when they are further away from the source and airborne cues are more dispersed (blue dashed line). As they get nearer and the density of the airborne cues increases (brown line), animals alternate less frequently and track the scent by sniffing close to the ground. (B) Rigolli et al. used a machine learning algorithm to identify the optimal strategy for tracking odors in the wind. They simulated an odor dispersing in the air (blue plume) and close to the ground (brown plume) and then trained artificial agents to find the source of the smell: the brown line indicates the trajectory agents took whilst sniffing the floor, and the blue circles represent where they paused to sniff the air. The algorithm revealed that the best way for agents to find the source of the odor was for them to alternate to sniffing the air when moving crosswind, and intersperse this with occasional surges forward until an odor was detected (blue star). (C) The simulation showed that agents displayed this alternating behavior less frequently as they moved closer to the odor source.

Image credit: Samuel Brudner; odor plumes in panel B are based on images by Nirag Kadakia and Mahmut Demir.

But how exactly does alternation speed up getting that first hint of a scent? Failing to detect an odor in a region indicates that the source of the smell is unlikely to be upwind of this area, eliminating the need to search there in the future. Odors disperse slower near the ground, and as a result do not reach as far as odors travelling in the air. Sniffing above their heads therefore allows agents to rule out larger upwind areas (if no smell is present), while also increasing the likelihood of detecting faint signals that are absent at ground level.

The simulation also revealed that during the early search phase, trained agents combined alternation with specific patterns of locomotion (Figure 1B). Agents moving crosswind sniffed the air more frequently than when they surged upwind. Rigolli et al. observed that this behavior helps the agents to rule out cross-sections of the simulated arena before moving upwind to gather evidence about a new region.

This cast-sniff-surge strategy involves many tradeoffs: casting over a wider distance takes longer but also eliminates a wider cross-section; sniffing in the air requires stopping and therefore losing time. Using a mathematical framework, Rigolli et al. show that optimally balancing these tradeoffs requires exploring the arena back and forth in an expanding crosswind zigzag, gradually casting across larger areas as the search progresses. Remarkably, these characteristics also appeared in trained agents which were not constrained to use a cast-sniff-surge approach.

Overall, Rigolli et al. demonstrate how, in theory, alternating between sniffing the ground and the air allows animals to efficiently search large areas for an odor source. Future studies should now test these predictions, for example examining if real animals do tend to alternate behaviors mostly in odor-poor regions. This work could also be applied to robotics, in particular to improve the exploratory behavior of drones used in difficult search-and-rescue operations.

References

  1. Book
    1. Sutton RS
    2. Barto AG
    (2019)
    Reinforcement Learning: An Introduction
    MIT Press.

Article and author information

Author details

  1. Samuel Brudner

    Samuel Brudner is in the Department of Molecular, Cellular and Developmental Biology, and the Quantitative Biology Institute, Yale University, New Haven, United States

    Contributed equally with
    Thierry Emonet
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0002-6043-9328
  2. Thierry Emonet

    Thierry Emonet is in the Department of Molecular, Cellular and Developmental Biology, the Quantitative Biology Institute, and Department of Physics, Yale University, New Haven, United States

    Contributed equally with
    Samuel Brudner
    For correspondence
    thierry.emonet@yale.edu
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0002-6746-6564

Publication history

  1. Version of Record published:

Copyright

© 2022, Brudner and Emonet

This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.

Metrics

  • 683
    views
  • 66
    downloads
  • 0
    citations

Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.

Download links

A two-part list of links to download the article, or parts of the article, in various formats.

Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)

Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)

Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)

  1. Samuel Brudner
  2. Thierry Emonet
(2022)
Navigation: Where is that smell coming from?
eLife 11:e82635.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.82635
  1. Further reading

Further reading

    1. Computational and Systems Biology
    2. Physics of Living Systems
    Natanael Spisak, Gabriel Athènes ... Aleksandra M Walczak
    Tools and Resources Updated

    B-cell repertoires are characterized by a diverse set of receptors of distinct specificities generated through two processes of somatic diversification: V(D)J recombination and somatic hypermutations. B-cell clonal families stem from the same V(D)J recombination event, but differ in their hypermutations. Clonal families identification is key to understanding B-cell repertoire function, evolution, and dynamics. We present HILARy (high-precision inference of lineages in antibody repertoires), an efficient, fast, and precise method to identify clonal families from single- or paired-chain repertoire sequencing datasets. HILARy combines probabilistic models that capture the receptor generation and selection statistics with adapted clustering methods to achieve consistently high inference accuracy. It automatically leverages the phylogenetic signal of shared mutations in difficult repertoire subsets. Exploiting the high sensitivity of the method, we find the statistics of evolutionary properties such as the site frequency spectrum and dN/dS ratio do not depend on the junction length. We also identify a broad range of selection pressures spanning two orders of magnitude.

    1. Physics of Living Systems
    Marcelo A Carignano, Martin Kroeger ... Igal Szleifer
    Research Article

    We propose the Self Returning Excluded Volume (SR-EV) model for the structure of chromatin based on stochastic rules and physical interactions. The SR-EV rules of return generate conformationally defined domains observed by single-cell imaging techniques. From nucleosome to chromosome scales, the model captures the overall chromatin organization as a corrugated system, with dense and dilute regions alternating in a manner that resembles the mixing of two disordered bi-continuous phases. This particular organizational topology is a consequence of the multiplicity of interactions and processes occurring in the nuclei, and mimicked by the proposed return rules. Single configuration properties and ensemble averages show a robust agreement between theoretical and experimental results including chromatin volume concentration, contact probability, packing domain identification and size characterization, and packing scaling behavior. Model and experimental results suggest that there is an inherent chromatin organization regardless of the cell character and resistant to an external forcing such as RAD21 degradation.