Competing neural representations of choice shape evidence accumulation in humans

  1. Krista Alexandria Marie Bond  Is a corresponding author
  2. Javier Rasero Daparte
  3. Raghav Madan
  4. Jyotika Bahuguna
  5. Jonathan E Rubin
  6. Timothy Verstynen  Is a corresponding author
  1. Carnegie Mellon University, United States
  2. University of Washington, United States
  3. University of Pittsburgh, United States

Abstract

Making adaptive choices in dynamic environments requires flexible decision policies. Previously, we showed how the evidence accumulation process that drives decisions shifts when outcome contingencies change (1). Using in silico experiments, here we show how the cortico-basal ganglia-thalamic (CBGT) circuits can feasibly implement shifts in the evidence accumulation process. When action contingencies change, dopaminergic plasticity redirects the balance of power, both within and between action representations, to divert the flow of evidence from one option to another. This model predicts that when competition between action representations is highest, the rate of evidence accumulation is lowest. This prediction was validated in in vivo experiments on human subjects, using fMRI, which showed that 1) evoked hemodynamic responses can reliably predict trialwise choices and 2) competition between action representations, measured using a classifier model, tracked with changes in the rate of evidence accumulation. These results paint a holistic picture of how CBGT circuits manage and adapt the evidence accumulation process in mammals.

Data availability

Behavioral data and computational derivatives are publically available here: https://github.com/kalexandriabond/competing-representations-shape-evidence-accumulation. Raw and preprocessed hemodynamic data, in addition to physiological measurements collected for quality control, are available here: https://openneuro.org/datasets/ds004283/versions/1.0.3.

The following data sets were generated

Article and author information

Author details

  1. Krista Alexandria Marie Bond

    Department of Psychology, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, United States
    For correspondence
    kbond@andrew.cmu.edu
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0003-1492-6798
  2. Javier Rasero Daparte

    Department of Psychology, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, United States
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
  3. Raghav Madan

    Department of Biomedical and Health Informatics, University of Washington, Seattle, United States
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0002-9790-393X
  4. Jyotika Bahuguna

    Department of Psychology, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, United States
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0002-2858-5325
  5. Jonathan E Rubin

    Department of Mathematics, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, United States
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0002-1513-1551
  6. Timothy Verstynen

    Department of Psychology, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, United States
    For correspondence
    timothyv@andrew.cmu.edu
    Competing interests
    The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0003-4720-0336

Funding

Air Force Research Laboratory (180119)

  • Timothy Verstynen

National Institutes of Health (CRCNS: R01DA053014)

  • Timothy Verstynen

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Ethics

Human subjects: All procedures were approved by the Carnegie Mellon University Institutional Review Board (Approval Code: 2018_00000195).All research participants provided informed consent to participate in the study and consent to publish any research findings based on their provided data.

Copyright

© 2023, Bond et al.

This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License permitting unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.

Metrics

  • 712
    views
  • 95
    downloads
  • 2
    citations

Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.

Download links

A two-part list of links to download the article, or parts of the article, in various formats.

Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)

Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)

Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)

  1. Krista Alexandria Marie Bond
  2. Javier Rasero Daparte
  3. Raghav Madan
  4. Jyotika Bahuguna
  5. Jonathan E Rubin
  6. Timothy Verstynen
(2023)
Competing neural representations of choice shape evidence accumulation in humans
eLife 12:e85223.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.85223

Share this article

https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.85223

Further reading

    1. Developmental Biology
    2. Neuroscience
    Xingsen Zhao, Qihang Sun ... Xuekun Li
    Research Article

    Williams syndrome (WS; OMIM#194050) is a rare disorder, which is caused by the microdeletion of one copy of 25–27 genes, and WS patients display diverse neuronal deficits. Although remarkable progresses have been achieved, the mechanisms for these distinct deficits are still largely unknown. Here, we have shown that neural progenitor cells (NPCs) in WS forebrain organoids display abnormal proliferation and differentiation capabilities, and synapse formation. Genes with altered expression are related to neuronal development and neurogenesis. Single cell RNA-seq (scRNA-seq) data analysis revealed 13 clusters in healthy control and WS organoids. WS organoids show an aberrant generation of excitatory neurons. Mechanistically, the expression of transthyretin (TTR) are remarkably decreased in WS forebrain organoids. We have found that GTF2IRD1 encoded by one WS associated gene GTF2IRD1 binds to TTR promoter regions and regulates the expression of TTR. In addition, exogenous TTR can activate ERK signaling and rescue neurogenic deficits of WS forebrain organoids. Gtf2ird1-deficient mice display similar neurodevelopmental deficits as observed in WS organoids. Collectively, our study reveals critical function of GTF2IRD1 in regulating neurodevelopment of WS forebrain organoids and mice through regulating TTR-ERK pathway.

    1. Developmental Biology
    2. Neuroscience
    Jayanarayanan Sadanandan, Sithara Thomas ... Peeyush Kumar T
    Research Article

    The blood-brain barrier (BBB) controls the movement of molecules into and out of the central nervous system (CNS). Since a functional BBB forms by mouse embryonic day E15.5, we reasoned that gene cohorts expressed in CNS endothelial cells (EC) at E13.5 contribute to BBB formation. In contrast, adult gene signatures reflect BBB maintenance mechanisms. Supporting this hypothesis, transcriptomic analysis revealed distinct cohorts of EC genes involved in BBB formation and maintenance. Here, we demonstrate that epigenetic regulator’s histone deacetylase 2 (HDAC2) and polycomb repressive complex 2 (PRC2) control EC gene expression for BBB development and prevent Wnt/β-catenin (Wnt) target genes from being expressed in adult CNS ECs. Low Wnt activity during development modifies BBB genes epigenetically for the formation of functional BBB. As a Class-I HDAC inhibitor induces adult CNS ECs to regain Wnt activity and BBB genetic signatures that support BBB formation, our results inform strategies to promote BBB repair.