Prairie Voles: Recovering from a broken heart

A molecular signature found in the brains of monogamous prairie voles begins to decay after prolonged separation from their partner.
  1. Alison M Bell  Is a corresponding author
  1. Department of Evolution, Ecology and Behavior, University of Illinois, United States

Despite the best efforts of my hole-digging dog, my backyard is home to rodents who are in love. Furry prairie voles spend lots of time huddled underground, usually in pairs that work together to rear their pups. If the couple is separated, even for as long as two weeks, they still remain bonded and will reunite as tight as they were before (Getz et al., 1981). These humble rodents are famous for their monogamous behavior, and have been the subject of many eye-catching headlines (Tucker, 2014; Armitage, 2015). Part of their appeal is the possibility to understand the biological basis of love, devotion, and life-long attachment – topics that seem more suited to humanities than biology departments.

Most scientific research on prairie voles has focused on how monogamous pairs are formed, particularly the role of oxytocin, the famous ‘love hormone’ (Insel and Shapiro, 1992). However, less is known about what happens when these bonds get broken; what happens when one member of the pair outlives the other? Previous behavioral studies have shown that voles exhibit signs of distress when they are separated from their partner, but eventually adapt to this loss and seek out a new connection (Harbert et al., 2020). Now, in eLife, Zoe Donaldson and colleagues from the University of Colorado Boulder and Oregon Health and Science University – including Julie Sadino as first author – report what happens to voles as they recover from a broken heart (Sadino et al., 2023).

Sadino et al. studied male voles in same-sex and opposite sex pairs that had been housed together for two weeks and then separated for 48 hours or four weeks. Using a technique called vTRAP (short for translating ribosome affinity purification), they investigated which genes are expressed in the nucleus accumbens, a part of the brain that is engaged during pair formation in mammals, including humans (Walum and Young, 2018). The pattern of genes expressed (also known as the transcriptional signature) was measured before and after voles were separated, along with their behavior to see if these two factors are coordinated together. The female voles were also prevented from becoming pregnant to control for the potentially confounding influence of co-parenting together.

The experiments revealed that male voles stably express hundreds of genes in their nucleus accumbens when living with a partner, which remained unchanged even after the pair were separated for a few days. However, while the male voles continued to prefer the company of their partner even after long periods apart, the transcriptional signature in the nucleus accumbens started to decay as the voles spent more time away from each other.

These findings suggest that genes in the brains of the voles start to alter their expression before the voles lose preference for their old mate. This means that although voles may behaviorally resume their relationship after long periods of separation, elements of their transcriptome may be less optimistic that this reunion will happen. It is tempting to speculate that the genes that begin to change expression may somehow be involved in recovering from the loss of a partner.

Furthermore, while behavioral and genomic changes in the brain occurring over different timescales is not an entirely new finding (White et al., 2002), the results of Sadino et al. do offer some clues as to how this may work at a mechanistic level. For example, genes that were upregulated in bonded males, but downregulated after a few weeks of separation, follow a pattern that suggests they may have a role in coping with loss. In contrast, another set of genes, which was initially downregulated and then upregulated after weeks of separation, may be involved in helping to prime the vole to form a new bond.

Understanding the neural basis of complex social behaviors is a hard problem. However, the work by Sadino et al. – which uses a savvy experimental design to connect genes, the brain and animal behavior – shows how it can be done. Moreover, and somewhat surprisingly, other researchers recently reported that prairie voles do not require a receptor for oxytocin in order to form social attachments (Berendzen et al., 2023). It seems that there are actually many players coordinating this complex social behavior.

An immediate task for future work is to look at brain areas other than the nucleus accumbens, which is closely associated with reward. While pair bonding is rewarding, this may be independent from the cognitive aspects of separation, such as the memory of a lost companion or the willingness to find a new partner. Another priority is to explicitly connect the transcriptional signatures identified with the formation or degradation of the pair bond using functional tests, a challenge the scientific community studying these furry lovers is no doubt going after next.

References

  1. Book
    1. Tucker A
    (2014)
    What Can Rodents Tell Us about Why Humans Love?
    Smithsonian Magazine.

Article and author information

Author details

  1. Alison M Bell

    Alison M Bell is in the Department of Evolution, Ecology and Behavior, University of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign, United States

    For correspondence
    alisonmb@illinois.edu
    Competing interests
    No competing interests declared
    ORCID icon "This ORCID iD identifies the author of this article:" 0000-0001-8933-8494

Publication history

  1. Version of Record published:

Copyright

© 2023, Bell

This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.

Metrics

  • 1,364
    views
  • 54
    downloads
  • 1
    citations

Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.

Download links

A two-part list of links to download the article, or parts of the article, in various formats.

Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)

Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)

Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)

  1. Alison M Bell
(2023)
Prairie Voles: Recovering from a broken heart
eLife 12:e87550.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.87550

Further reading

    1. Neuroscience
    Sharon Inberg, Yael Iosilevskii ... Benjamin Podbilewicz
    Research Article

    Dendrites are crucial for receiving information into neurons. Sensory experience affects the structure of these tree-like neurites, which, it is assumed, modifies neuronal function, yet the evidence is scarce, and the mechanisms are unknown. To study whether sensory experience affects dendritic morphology, we use the Caenorhabditis elegans' arborized nociceptor PVD neurons, under natural mechanical stimulation induced by physical contacts between individuals. We found that mechanosensory signals induced by conspecifics and by glass beads affect the dendritic structure of the PVD. Moreover, developmentally isolated animals show a decrease in their ability to respond to harsh touch. The structural and behavioral plasticity following sensory deprivation are functionally independent of each other and are mediated by an array of evolutionarily conserved mechanosensory amiloride-sensitive epithelial sodium channels (degenerins). Calcium imaging of the PVD neurons in a micromechanical device revealed that controlled mechanical stimulation of the body wall produces similar calcium dynamics in both isolated and crowded animals. Our genetic results, supported by optogenetic, behavioral, and pharmacological evidence, suggest an activity-dependent homeostatic mechanism for dendritic structural plasticity, that in parallel controls escape response to noxious mechanosensory stimuli.

    1. Neuroscience
    Mi-Seon Kong, Ethan Ancell ... Larry S Zweifel
    Research Article

    The central amygdala (CeA) has emerged as an important brain region for regulating both negative (fear and anxiety) and positive (reward) affective behaviors. The CeA has been proposed to encode affective information in the form of valence (whether the stimulus is good or bad) or salience (how significant is the stimulus), but the extent to which these two types of stimulus representation occur in the CeA is not known. Here, we used single cell calcium imaging in mice during appetitive and aversive conditioning and found that majority of CeA neurons (~65%) encode the valence of the unconditioned stimulus (US) with a smaller subset of cells (~15%) encoding the salience of the US. Valence and salience encoding of the conditioned stimulus (CS) was also observed, albeit to a lesser extent. These findings show that the CeA is a site of convergence for encoding oppositely valenced US information.