Tapered whiskers are required for active tactile sensation
Abstract
Many mammals forage and burrow in dark constrained spaces. Touch through facial whiskers is important during these activities, but the close quarters makes whisker deployment challenging. The diverse shapes of facial whiskers reflect distinct ecological niches. Rodent whiskers are conical, often with a remarkably linear taper. Here we use theoretical and experimental methods to analyze interactions of mouse whiskers with objects. When pushed into objects, conical whiskers suddenly slip at a critical angle. In contrast, cylindrical whiskers do not slip for biologically plausible movements. Conical whiskers sweep across objects and textures in characteristic sequences of brief sticks and slips, which provide information about the tactile world. In contrast, cylindrical whiskers stick and remain stuck, even when sweeping across fine textures. Thus the conical whisker structure is adaptive for sensor mobility in constrained environments and in feature extraction during active haptic exploration of objects and surfaces.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.01350.001eLife digest
When foraging in dark, confined spaces, mammals use the information gathered by their whiskers to ‘see’ the world around them. Mammalian whiskers come in a variety of shapes and sizes, most likely reflecting the way in which they are used. Rodent whiskers are conical and precisely tapered, whereas some harbor seals have flattened whiskers with wave-like undulations. Human hair is cylindrical.
Rodents sweep their whiskers back and forth over objects and surfaces without moving their head. They use this process, called whisking, to build up a three-dimensional picture of objects. Whisking allows the rodent to estimate where an object is located, how big it is, and what kind of surface texture it has. Information about surface texture can, for example, help the animal to distinguish a stone from a seed.
Hires et al. have used theoretical and experimental methods to analyze the interaction of mouse whiskers with objects. The conical shape of a mouse whisker makes the tip thousands of times more flexible than the base. Hires et al. show that this flexibility gradient allows the whiskers to slip past objects close to the face and to move freely across rough surfaces. Cylindrical whiskers, on the other hand, become stuck behind nearby objects and get caught on tiny features in an object’s surface texture.
Hires et al. conclude that conical whiskers are advantageous in the tight confines of the tunnels that mice live, forage and socialize in, because they are able to gather a more complete sensory picture of their surroundings. The maneuverability of the whiskers also allows the mouse to move their whiskers forwards or backwards when rough tunnel walls are close by. By contrast, the sticking experienced by cylindrical whiskers would lead to ‘blind spots’. In addition to providing insights into the ways that mice interact with their environment, this work could also lead to improvements in the design of the canes used by the visually impaired to navigate human environments.
https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.01350.002Introduction
Many mammals use facial whiskers for navigation (Vincent, 1912; Dehnhardt et al., 2001), object localization (Hutson and Masterton, 1986; Knutsen et al., 2006; Krupa et al., 2001; Mehta et al., 2007; O’Connor et al., 2010a; Pammer et al., 2013), texture discrimination (Carvell and Simons, 1990; Wolfe et al., 2008; Chen et al., 2013), and object recognition (Anjum et al., 2006). The shapes of mammalian whiskers are diverse. Rodent whiskers are conical (Birdwell et al., 2007; Williams and Kramer, 2010; Quist et al., 2011; Pammer et al., 2013), whereas sea lion whiskers (Hanke et al., 2010) and human hair are approximately cylindrical. Whiskers of harbor seals have elliptical cross-sections with an undulated structure (Hanke et al., 2010). Differences in whisker shapes across different species likely reflect differences in how animals use their whiskers. For example, the undulating microstructure of harbor seal whiskers suppresses vibrations triggered by vortices and enhances the seal’s ability to analyze water movements (Hanke et al., 2010).
What could be the advantages of the whisker taper seen in rodents? Rodents sense their surroundings by moving their whiskers over objects with large amplitudes (up to 50° peak–peak) in a rhythmic motion (Knutsen et al., 2006; O’Connor et al., 2010a; O’Connor et al., 2013; Voigts et al., 2008). Rodents can localize and recognize objects in three dimensions (Knutsen et al., 2006; Krupa et al., 2001; O’Connor et al., 2010a; Pammer et al., 2013; Voigts et al., 2008) and also discriminate subtle differences in surface textures (Carvell and Simons, 1990; Wolfe et al., 2008) (reviewed in Diamond, 2010). These behaviors are based on collisions between whiskers and objects, which cause time-varying forces at the whisker base and excitation of sensory neurons in the follicles (Zucker and Welker, 1969; Szwed et al., 2006). Whisker mechanics thus couples the tactile world to forces at the whisker base (Solomon and Hartmann, 2006; Birdwell et al., 2007; Bagdasarian et al., 2013; Pammer et al., 2013).
Rodent whiskers are thin, approximately linear and homogenous elastic cones (Solomon and Hartmann, 2006; Birdwell et al., 2007; Williams and Kramer, 2010; Pammer et al., 2013). As a result of the linear taper, whisker bending stiffness decreases with distance from the face over five orders of magnitude. Behavioral measurements have shown that mice use distance-dependent whisker mechanics as a ruler to estimate object location along the length of the whisker (Pammer et al., 2013).
Here we used theoretical and experimental methods to analyze the interactions of whiskers with objects. We uncover additional decisive advantages of conical whiskers compared to cylindrical whiskers for tactile exploration. Conical whiskers sweep across textures with informative micromotions, whereas cylindrical whiskers get stuck. The steep increase in flexibility from base to tip of conical whiskers allow rodents to maneuver their sensors past objects with relative ease. Conical whisker shape is thus critical for tactile exploration in confined spaces.
Results
Mechanical model of whiskers interacting with an object
We modeled rodent whiskers as truncated cones with a cylindrical cross section, base radius rbase, tip radius rtip, and length LW (Ibrahim and Wright, 1975; Boubenec et al., 2012) (Figure 1A). Whiskers have intrinsic curvature (Quist and Hartmann, 2012) and are further deflected by forces that are caused by interactions with objects (O’Connor et al., 2010a; Bagdasarian et al., 2013; Pammer et al., 2013) (Figure 1B). In our model, contacts occurred either in the ‘concave backward’ (CB) or ‘concave forward’ (CF) directions (Figure 1C) (Quist and Hartmann, 2012). We quantified contact strength using the push angle θp (Quist and Hartmann, 2012), the angle through which the whisker is rotated into the object (Figure 1D). By convention, contacts for the CB configuration correspond to θp > 0, and for the CF configuration to θp < 0; θp = 0 defines the angle of initial touch. In all cases whisker movement and bending were limited to the x-y plane. We computed whisker shape by solving the Euler–Bernoulli beam equation in the quasi-static regime (Euler, 1744; Birdwell et al., 2007; Solomon and Hartmann, 2006; Pammer et al., 2013). The beam equation describing whisker shape was converted to a boundary-value problem formulation (‘Materials and methods’; Press et al., 1992), a set of differential equations with defined boundary conditions at the whisker base and at the point of contact with the object. The object was assumed to be a cylindrical pole perpendicular to the plane of motion, as is typically used in object localization experiments (Knutsen et al., 2006; Mehta et al., 2007; O’Connor et al., 2010a; Pammer et al., 2013) (Figure 1B). The whisker shape at each time was determined by the static solution computed for the time-varying boundary conditions. Using identical methods we also modeled hypothetical cylindrical whiskers.
The boundary-value problem for whisker shape generally has two solutions, one stable and the other unstable (Figure 2A). During object contact, the whisker shape matches the stable solution since small perturbations from it will decay back to the stable solution (Strogatz, 1994). The bending of the stable solution is weaker compared to the unstable solution. As the whisker pushes further into the object it becomes increasingly deflected (Figure 2B). At the same time the whisker slides along the object and the arclength from the whisker base to the point of contact, sobj, increases until the whisker detaches from the object. We note two qualitatively different types of detachment. First, under some conditions detachment occurs suddenly before the end of the whisker has reached the object, sobj < LW; we refer to this type of detachment as ‘slip-off’ (Figure 2B). Second, detachment has to occur when the tip reaches the end of the object, sobj = LW; we refer to this type of detachment as ‘pull-off’ (Figure 2C,D).
Bending of the whisker can be characterized by the angle of the whisker at the point of object contact, θobj (Figure 1B). The whisker first touches the pole at θp = 0 (Figure 3A–D, open circles). As the whisker pushes into the pole (|θp| > 0), θobj changes monotonically (Figure 3B). In the CB configuration, the whisker bends and its shape becomes more ‘concave backwards’. The force F acting on the whisker increases as more elastic energy is stored in the whisker (Figure 3C); sobj, also increases (Figure 3D). At a critical θp, the two solutions (solid lines, dashed lines) coalesce and disappear at a saddle-node bifurcation (SNB) (Strogatz, 1994) (Figure 3B–D, solid circles). No solution exists above this critical θp value, which also corresponds to a critical sobj < LW. The whisker slips suddenly and rapidly past the pole. The saddle-node bifurcation corresponds to slip-off. In the CF configuration, sobj first decreases as |θp| increases from 0, because touch forces straighten the whisker (Figure 3D). With further increases in θp, the whisker bends in the other direction and sobj increases.
The regime in which a stable static solution exists for whisker shape can be visualized by plotting a ‘detachment’ curve in the θp−d plane, where d is the distance of the object from the base of the whisker (Figure 3E). The detachment curve is the set of points in the θp−d plane where detachments occur. It encloses an area where stable contacting solutions exist. When the object is close to the face (small d), the whisker contacts the object near its base and a static solution exists for most practical θp values (peak-to-peak amplitude of whisker movements, 50° [Voigts et al., 2008; Curtis and Kleinfeld, 2009; O’Connor et al., 2013]). For larger d, the θp regime with a stable solution decreases approximately linearly. Detachments correspond to slip-offs. The range of θp with a stable solution is larger for the CF than the CB configuration. This is consistent with experimental observations (Quist and Hartmann, 2012) and intuition: in the CB configuration the intrinsic curvature aids slip-off. When the object touches near the whisker tip (large d), the saddle-node bifurcation ceases to exist and the whisker is ‘pulled off’ the object. (Figure 3E, blue line). The pull-offs are the result of whisker truncation, and would not occur for a perfect cone.
Identical analyses were performed for hypothetical cylindrical whiskers (Figure 3F–J). Although the bifurcation diagrams were superficially similar for conical and cylindrical whiskers (c.f. Figure 3B–E,G–J), cylindrical whiskers exhibit stable solutions at much larger θp. The SNB occurs for θp > 90° (Figure 3G–I, black lines and solid circles; d = 10 mm), which is beyond plausible ranges of whisking since whiskers cannot move into the face. When cylindrical whiskers touch the object close to their end, they are pulled off at moderate θp, because the whisker tip reaches the object (Figure 3I, blue line) sobj = LW. Therefore, cylindrical whiskers do not slip-off the pole. For a homogenous cylinder this effect is independent of the cylinder’s bending stiffness and thus its thickness. Our model thus predicts that conical and cylindrical whiskers interact with objects in a fundamentally different way. For a large range of object distances conical whiskers slip past objects, whereas cylindrical whiskers get stuck. This difference is expected to have profound consequences for object-whisker interactions during haptic sensation.
Whisker-object interactions
We compared our model with measurements made on mouse whiskers (conical) and human hair (cylindrical) (Figure 4). Mouse and human hair have similar Young’s Modulus (Hu et al., 2010; Quist et al., 2011). A C2 mouse whisker was mounted on a galvanometer scanner so that its intrinsic curvature was in the plane of whisker movement (Figure 4A). The whisker was then moved slowly (fgalvo = 0.2 Hz, peak-to-peak amplitude 30°) against a pole. As the whisker rotated into the pole it was deformed until, at a critical θp, it suddenly slipped off the pole (Figure 4B, Video 1). The red line in Figure 4B shows the whisker immediately before detachment. Whisker slip-offs occurred before the tip of the whisker had reached the point of contact. In contrast, for the cylindrical hair, slip-offs did not occur. Detachments always coincided with the whisker tip reaching the point of contact and were thus pull-offs (Figure 4C, red line, Video 2).
We performed the same type of measurement for multiple object locations along the whisker (d, Figure 4A). The regime of stable interactions between whisker and pole, bounded by the detachment curve, can be visualized in the θp−d plane (Figure 4D). The experimental results were in agreement with the model. For conical whiskers, slip-off occurred before the whisker tip reached the object, and the critical θp decreased rapidly with object distance (Figure 4D, black circles). The observed deviations between the idealized conical model and actual whisker are expected because the whisker is not a perfect cone (Ibrahim and Wright, 1975) and because the whisker’s Young’s modulus may vary slightly along its length (Quist et al., 2011). In contrast, the cylindrical hair only pulled off when the whisker tip reached the pole (Figure 4C), with a close fit between experimental and theoretical results (Figure 4D, blue circles).
We next tested if slip-offs occur normally during whisker-dependent behavior (Figure 5). We analyzed data from head-fixed mice trained in a vibrissa-based object location discrimination task (Pammer et al., 2013). Mice reported the presence of a pole at a target position (the ‘Go stimulus’; proximal) or in a distracter position (the ‘No Go stimulus’; distal) (Figure 5A) by either licking (Go response) or withholding licking (No Go response). In each trial, the pole was presented at a single location. Whiskers were trimmed so that mice performed the task with a single whisker (C2). For the trials analyzed here the pole distance from the face was randomly chosen from the range d = 7–13 mm (measured from the follicle; the No Go stimuli). We used high-speed (500 Hz) videography and automated whisker tracking to measure the position and shape of the whisker in two mice (Clack et al., 2012; Pammer et al., 2013) (140 slip events).
In behaving mice, the intrinsic whisker curvature is not parallel to the plane of whisking and imaging (Towal et al., 2011). We corrected for the curvature out of the imaging plane using a simple procedure (‘Materials and methods’). Furthermore, whiskers exhibit torsion during movement, rotating from a partially concave backward orientation, thru concave down to partially concave forward during protraction (Knutsen et al., 2008). We thus define positive and negative θp to denote whisker movement in the protraction and retraction directions respectively, independent of intrinsic curvature.
Slip-offs were more likely for more distant object locations (Figure 5B) and occurred at larger θp for smaller object distances (Figure 5C). Overall, slip-offs occurred in approximately 15% of behavioral trials. We again compared model and experiment in the θp−d plane. One of the two whiskers was truncated (Figure 5C, left). For objects touching the whisker near the tip, detachments occurred for small θp (<20°), with the whisker tip reaching the pole (i.e., pull-offs). For smaller object distances slip-off occurred at larger θp (>20°), consistent with a saddle-node bifurcation (i.e., slip-offs). The second whisker was less truncated (Figure 5C, right). For the object distances tested we observed slip-offs only along the whisker. These results are consistent with our model. The critical θp values for slip-off varied significantly across trials even for identical object distances. This variability is likely caused by differences in whisker movement and whisker elevation across trials.
Rodents move their whiskers over objects to explore surfaces. For example, mice can discriminate surface roughness over a few whisking cycles (Chen et al., 2013). Texture is likely inferred from the statistics of whisker micromotions produced by the interactions between whiskers and objects (Diamond, 2010). In particular, as whiskers move over objects whiskers occasionally get stuck, followed by high-velocity slips. The pattern of stick-slip events is highly informative about surface texture (Arabzadeh et al., 2005; Wolfe et al., 2008).
We wondered whether whisker shape determines the nature of the stick-slip events underlying texture exploration. We moved a C2 mouse whisker over extra fine (600 grit) sandpaper using a galvanometer scanner (fgalvo = 0.2 Hz; peak-to-peak amplitude, 30°) while tracking whisker shape in three dimensions using dual view videography (Figure 6A, Video 3). The tips of mouse whiskers moved along the surface in an irregular manner, during protractions and retractions. Whisker tips were transiently trapped (Figure 6B, red) followed by small, high-velocity slips (Figure 6B,C). The pattern of stick-slip events differed for different wall distances (Figure 6C).
In contrast, as cylindrical hairs swept across the surface they were trapped during initial protraction and remained trapped for the remainder of the experiment lasting multiple whisking cycles (Figure 6D–F, Video 4). When the distance between follicle and the site of trapping was shorter than the hair length, the hair buckled out of the plane of movement (Figure 6E, top). The tips of hairs escaped the traps only when the distance to the tip along the path of an undeflected hair exceeded the actual hair arclength (Figure 6E,F, pane 2). The whisker tip was thus pulled out of the trap (Figure 3J, blue line). These measurements show that the conical whisker shape is critical for the sweeping motions of whisker tips across objects and surfaces, which supports feature extraction via stick-slip events. More generally, conical whiskers can move past walls and objects, which may be critical for positioning of whiskers in confined spaces, such as tunnels, during directed tactile exploration.
We investigated whether slip-offs convey specific sensory information to cortex. Silicon probes were inserted into the C2 barrel column (O’Connor et al., 2013) (Figure 7A). We recorded multi-unit activity across cortical layers 2–5 while mice performed an object location discrimination task with the C2 whisker (O’Connor et al., 2013). Mice touched the pole multiple times during a trial (Figure 7B). The first touch within a trial caused a large peak in activity with a rapid rise (Figure 7B,C), consistent with previous work (Simons, 1978; Armstrong-James et al., 1992; de Kock et al., 2007; O’Connor et al., 2010b; O’Connor et al., 2013). Later touches within a series, during which slip-offs were more commonly seen, produced smaller responses (Figure 7D) (Ahissar et al., 2001). When slip-off did not occur, the detach-related signals were almost undetectable. In contrast, when slip-off did occur, the detach-related signals were large, comparable to the first touch (Figure 7F,G).
Discussion
We have developed a mathematical framework for whisker deflection in the context of dynamical systems theory (Figures 1–3) to explore the functional consequences of whisker taper. Recent findings have shown that whisker taper is used as a ruler by mice to gauge the distance to objects with a single whisker (Pammer et al., 2013). Tapered whiskers have resonance frequencies that are robust to wear and truncation damage to their tips (Williams and Kramer, 2010). Tapered whiskers also detach from objects at shallower push angles than cylindrical whisker substitutes.
Here we go beyond prior observations and uncover fundamental differences in how tapered and untapered hairs interact with objects; tapered whiskers slip-off when the contact point is along the whisker body (sobj < Lw), whereas cylindrical hairs require the tip to be pulled off (sobj = Lw) for biologically plausible push angles (Figure 3E,J, Figure 4). Thus, tapered whiskers have greater freedom of movement past obstacles compared to cylindrical hairs. This mobility is seen in reduced preparations (Figure 4) and also during active behavior, as whisker slip-off occurs in a variety of object location discrimination tasks (Figures 5, 7). The intrinsic difference in detachment also produces qualitatively different interaction patterns during palpation of textured surfaces (Figure 6). Tapered whiskers sweep past with stick-slip micromotions, whereas cylindrical whiskers become immobilized on surface imperfections.
Theoretical treatment of whisker mechanics is a necessary foundation for understanding how sensory input shapes neural representations of the tactile world. Previous work has computed whisker deflections based on the quasi-static solution of the Euler-Bernoulli equation (Birdwell et al., 2007; Williams and Kramer, 2010; Quist and Hartmann, 2012; Pammer et al., 2013). Aspects of whisker vibrations have also been treated, including resonant frequencies (Hartmann et al., 2003; Neimark et al., 2003) and wave propagation following contact-induced impulses (Boubenec et al., 2012).
We framed whisker-object interactions in the language of boundary-value problems. This allowed us to carry out bifurcation analysis and distinguish stable from unstable shapes. We demonstrate that for conical whiskers there are only two possible solutions for whisker shape for a given object distance and push angle, one stable, one unstable. We identify how the saddle-node bifurcations separating the two branches of solutions vary as a function of parameters, such as θp (Figure 3). This is not possible using previously developed numerical approaches (Birdwell et al., 2007; Quist and Hartmann, 2012). Slip-offs occur suddenly at a critical push angle, θp, corresponding to the angle where these saddle-node bifurcations occur. Curves of saddle-node bifurcations in a two-parameter plane define the regime in which stable solutions can be obtained (Figure 3E,J). For conical whiskers the critical push angles are within the normal range of whisking (Figure 4). For cylindrical hairs the critical angles fall outside the range of whisking. Thus our theory predicts that conical and cylindrical whiskers will interact with objects in a fundamentally different manner.
Our theory addresses the effects of whisker truncations (Figures 3–5). Truncations of conical whiskers make whisker behavior more ‘cylindrical-like’ (Figure 5). The intuition obtained from our analysis led us to distinguish between the dynamics of conical whiskers and cylindrical hairs during sweeping across textures (Figure 6).
We compared theoretical predictions with videos of whiskers and cylindrical hair rotated into a steel pole. In situations where the whisker curvature was contained within a plane, the agreement between theory and experiment was very good (Figure 4), despite our model ignoring frictional forces. The small remnant differences between theory and experiment are due to deviations of whisker geometry from perfect conical shape (AH, KS, DG, unpublished) and possible inhomogeneities in the Young’s modulus (Quist et al., 2011) (but also see Carl et al., 2012). In behaving mice the whisker droops out of the plane of whisking (Towal et al., 2011). Rigorous treatment of whisker deflection by an object would thus require a three-dimensional model. We developed a phenomenological model to predict slip-offs even for behaving mice (‘Materials and methods’), which produced qualitative agreement with experiments (Figure 5).
Our results suggest several functions for which the conical shape of rodent whiskers is evolutionarily adaptive. Within their natural habitat, many rodents, including house mice (Berry, 1968) and African pouched mice (Ellison, 1993), live in group nests consisting of chambers connected by long, body-width tunnels. During running, whiskers point forward to touch unanticipated objects. When a new object is encountered rodents foveate their whiskers on the object for fine-scale exploration (Grant et al., 2009). Within these dark, radially constrained tunnels, navigation (Vincent, 1912; Dehnhardt et al., 2001), localization of objects (Hutson and Masterton, 1986; Krupa et al., 2001), social touch (Wolfe et al., 2011), and determination of friend or foe (Anjum et al., 2006) demands freedom of whisker motion. Whiskers have to be moved past the rough walls of the tunnel. Without the flexibility provided by whisker taper, the whiskers could be trapped in a far protracted or retracted orientation, causing a tactile ‘blind-spot’.
Whisker taper is also desirable outside of constrained spaces. A major sensory avenue for the localization and identification of objects and their properties is via directed sweeping of whiskers across object surfaces (Carvell and Simons, 1990; Ritt et al., 2008; von Heimendahl et al., 2007; Wolfe et al., 2008). During artificial periodic palpation of fine-grained textures, conical whiskers traversed the surface with complex micromotions, whereas cylindrical hair became trapped against the surface (Figure 6). Although a precise understanding of the interaction between a tapered whisker and textured surface during a stick-slip event has not been treated mathematically, it is likely that forces at the tip build up until they bend the whisker tip sufficiently to free it from traps. In the cylindrical case, the constant bending stiffness of the body and tip render the whisker incapable of transmitting sufficient lateral force to buckle the much stiffer tip and release it from the surface.
Beyond mechanical maneuverability, do slips contribute to the neural representation of tactile sensation? During active whisking, stick-slip micromotions on textured surfaces drive sparse, precisely timed spikes in barrel cortex that provide a sensory cue for surface texture (Jadhav et al., 2009). Neural responses in barrel cortex to repeated contacts between whiskers and objects show strong adaptation during active touch (Ahissar et al., 2001; Crochet et al., 2011) and object location discrimination (Figure 7C,D,G). Despite occurring when the circuit is adapted to touch, slip-offs produce strong volleys of cortical activity, of comparable magnitude to pre-adapted touch (Figure 7F). Thus, slip-related excitation can overcome cortical touch adaptation and likely contributes to sensation and perception in a variety of tactile behaviors (Arabzadeh et al., 2005).
Materials and methods
Model of a whisker deflected by a cylindrical pole
Request a detailed protocolWe model whiskers as truncated cones with length LW, base radius rbase, and tip radius rtip (Figure 1A). The conical shape is virtually extended to a perfect cone of length L. The whisker is located in the x-y plane. The arclength along the whisker, s, is s = 0 at the base, s = sobj at the point of object contact, s = LW at the tip, and s = L at the virtual tip (Figure 1B). The whisker base is located at point (x0, y0), and the positions of a point along the whisker is (x(s), y(s)), 0≤s≤LW. The running angle between the whisker and the x-axis is θ(s), and θ(0) = θ0. The whisker radius is rw = (L−s) rbase/L and the area moment of inertia is . The Young’s modulus is E = 3 GPa (Birdwell et al., 2007; Quist et al., 2011; Pammer et al., 2013). Similar calculations were carried out for cylindrical hair with rw = rbase.
The bending stiffness of the whisker is the product EI(s). In the absence of contact with an object, the intrinsic curvature of the whisker is κi(s). The object is a cylindrical pole oriented perpendicular to the x-y plane with radius rpole, centered at (xcen, ycen). Upon contact, the whisker touches the object at (xobj, yobj) with angle θobj (Figure 1B), where
The Euclidian distance between the whisker base and the contact point is d. The object applies force on the whisker:
At steady state, the shape of the whisker is determined by the solution of the static Euler-Bernoulli equation (Landau and Lifshitz, 1986; Birdwell et al., 2007; Williams and Kramer, 2010; Pammer et al., 2013)
where Mz is the component of the bending moment perpendicular to the x-y plane and , together with the equations
Substituting Equations 1,2 in Equation 3, we obtain
We seek a solution for Equations 4–6 given the boundary conditions at the base (x0, y0 and θ0), and that the whisker contacts the pole at an (initially unknown) arclength sobj.
Given the shape of an undeflected whisker as a function of the running arclength s, namely (x,y)=(g(s),h(s)), the intrinsic curvature is
where d/ds is denoted by ‘. The shape of the undeflected whisker is considered to be parabolic, y = Ax2 (Quist and Hartmann, 2012).
To compute the whisker shape Equations 4–7 are transformed to a form of a boundary-value problem (BVP) by introducing a variable σ = s/sobj
The differential Equations 8–13 are solved on the interval 0 ≤ σ ≤ 1 together with the equations
The boundary-value conditions for σ = 0 are: x(0) = x0, y(0) = y0, θ(0) = θ0. The conditions for σ = 1 are: x(1) = xcen − rpole sin θobj, y(1) = ycen + rpole cos θobj, θ(1) = θobj. Solutions to Equations 8–14 have physical meaning if sobj < LW. If the whisker tip reaches the contact point (sobj = LW) the whisker detaches because it is pulled off the pole.
We solved six differential Equations 8–13 together with their boundary conditions to find six unknown variables (x, y, θ, F, θobj, sobj) as functions σ on the interval 0 ≤ σ ≤ 1. The variable θobj is treated as a separate variable from θ, but the boundary condition θ(1) = θobj guarantees that the solution is self-consistent. The equations were solved numerically using the iterative shooting method (Press et al., 1992). We start with guessed initial values for the unknown variables for σ = 0 and integrate the differential equation until σ = 1. The initial conditions are then varied to reduce the difference between the given boundary conditions and those that are obtained by the most recent integration. The method converges if the initial conditions are sufficiently close to the solution. We begin by solving the boundary-value problem with θobj corresponding to θp = 0 (i.e., the whisker is barely touching the pole). We then vary θobj slightly, compute the whisker shape, and repeat the process until the desired θobj is reached. We used the boundary-value problem solver software package XPPAUT (Ermentrout, 2002). The software package AUTO (Doedel, 1981), which is incorporated into XPPAUT, was used to compute bifurcation diagrams (Figure 3), by following the solutions of the boundary-value problem as parameters, such as θobj, vary.
The static solution of Equations 8–14 is a fixed point of a spatiotemporal dynamical system representing whisker movement. The full dynamical system can be formulated as a partial differential equation only for small θp and straight beams (Timoshenko, 1961; Boubenec et al., 2012). Since the full dynamical system for all θp and beams with intrinsic curvature is not known we cannot linearize a dynamic equation. However, the static solution for small θp must be stable. In addition, bifurcation theory implies that if we increase θp the solution will coalesce with an unstable solution and they both disappear, via a saddle-node bifurcation (Strogatz, 1994). In principle, a branch of stable solutions can lose stability via a Hopf bifurcation before the saddle-node bifurcation. Slip-off will occur at θp values smaller than predicted by our quasi-static theory. The good correspondence between the computed saddle-node bifurcation and the experimentally measured value for θp at slip-off shows that the static solution disappears via a saddle-node bifurcation (Figure 4).
Undeflected whiskers can be modeled as parabolas within a plane (Towal et al., 2011). In the work reported here the whisker is contained entirely within a plane perpendicular to the pole. For a whisker with intrinsic curvature, contact occurs in either the ‘concave backward’ (CB) or ‘concave forward’ (CF) directions (Figure 1C) (Quist and Hartmann, 2012). To quantify contact strength, we use the push angle θp (Figure 1D) (Quist and Hartmann, 2012). Suppose a whisker originates at (x0, y0, θ0) and touches a pole at (xobj, yobj). We plot an undeflected whisker with the same (x0, y0, θ0), and find a point along the whisker with the same Euclidian distance d from (x0, y0) as (xobj, yobj), defined as (xvirtual, yvirtual) The angle between the two rays starting at (x0, y0) towards (xobj, yobj) and (xvirtual, yvirtual) is defined as θp. By convention, we define the sign of θp to be positive for CB and negative for CF.
Exact treatment of whisker deflection in behaving rodents demands a three-dimensional model that is outside the scope of this work. Instead, we developed a phenomenological two-dimensional model (Figure 5). First we assume that the whisker touches the object in a concave-down configuration. Second, the deflection of the whisker is described by the two-dimensional model (Equation 8–14) when the projection of the whisker on that plane is treated as a two-dimensional whisker. The area moment of inertia (I) is computed by estimating the arclength s along the real whisker from the whisker projection and using this value in Equation 14. This correction in s was on the order of 3%. We measured the length of the isolated whisker. Estimating the whisker base is inaccurate because of the fur on the face. We therefore determined the effective whisker length from the estimated whisker base to the tip based on the video recordings. If the whisker slips off at its tip, we find the maximal projected length during events of slip-off at the tip. If there are no such events, we compute the projected length that yields the theoretically-obtained slip-off at the largest d for which slip-off is obtained. For all cases, this estimated value is less than 1 mm smaller than the length measured directly.
Whisker measurements
Request a detailed protocolFor galvo experiments (Figures 4, 6), we used plucked, full-grown mouse C2 whiskers. The shapes of these whiskers were measured under a light microscope at high magnification (Pammer et al., 2013). The follicle ends of the whiskers were embedded in the barrel of a cut 21 gauge needle filled with Kwik-Cast silicon sealant (World Precision Instruments, Sarasota, FL). Needles were mounted on the top edge of a galvo scan mirror (6800HP; Cambridge Technology, Bedford, MA). Whiskers were then rotated into a cylindrical object (steel Wiretrol II plunger; Drummond Scientific, Broomall, PA) at 0.2 Hz, 30° peak-to-peak amplitude (Figure 4). Dual-perspective imaging confirmed that whiskers remained in the concave forward or concave backward orientation during the interaction with the pole (data not shown). The same whiskers were used for imaging whisker motion across textured surfaces (Figure 6). The surface was fine sandpaper (600 grit) rigidly mounted on a glass slide and positioned perpendicular to both planes of imaging. A variety of human hair was characterized. Hair from an Asian female closely matched the whisker diameter close to the base and was used as a cylindrical hair. The hair dimensions were: base diameter, 60 μm; tip diameter, 53 μm; length, 15.0 mm.
High-speed videography was used to measure the position and shape of mouse whiskers during galvo experiments (Figures 4, 6) (X-PRI camera, 32 fps, 0.6 ms exposure, 8-bit depth, AOS Technologies, Switzerland) and behavior (Figures 5, 7) (1000 fps, 0.2 ms exposure, 8-bit depth, Basler 504 k, Germany). Pixel size was 0.07 mm (Figure 5), or 0.031 mm (Figure 7), or 0.032 mm (Figures 4, 6). Illumination was with a 940 nm infrared LED delivered through a diffuser and condenser lens and projected directly into the camera. A silver mirror (PFSQ10-03-P01, Thor Labs, Newton, NJ) reflected an orthogonal side view projection onto the same camera (Knutsen et al., 2008). Videos were split and cropped prior to whisker tracking.
Whiskers were tracked with the Janelia Whisker Tracker (Clack et al., 2012) (https://openwiki.janelia.org/wiki/display/MyersLab/Whisker+Tracking). The whisker medial axis is stored as an array of points (xi,yi), i = 1,…,N, where N is on the order of several hundreds. To remove artifacts associated with tracking variation at the base when calculating θ0, the angle of the whisker base was determined by a linear fit of the fifth through tenth points closest to the base. Forces acting on the whisker (Figure 7B) were calculated using published methods (O’Connor et al., 2010a; Clack et al., 2012; Pammer et al., 2013).
The behavioral task and apparatus have been described in detail elsewhere (O’Connor et al., 2010a; O’Connor et al., 2013; Pammer et al., 2013). Briefly, head-fixed mice judged the distance to a metal pole that was presented at a range of positions along the whisker in the radial dimension (Figure 5) or horizontal dimension (Figure 7). For radial discrimination, a proximal position (5 mm radially from follicle) was defined as the Go position, distal positions (7–13 mm) were defined as No Go positions. For horizontal discrimination, Go and No go positions were separated by 4.5 mm along a parallel to the anteroposterior axis of the mouse at a radial distance of 7–11 mm. Mice performed object location discrimination with a single C2 whisker. Within two days of the behavioral experiments we plucked whiskers and measured their shape and material properties using a macroscope and microgram balance (Mx5; Mettler Toledo, Columbus, OH) (Pammer et al., 2013).
Electrophysiology
Request a detailed protocolParts of the electrophysiology dataset is a reanalysis of previously acquired data (see O’Connor et al., 2013 for detailed methods). During a head-fixed pole location discrimination task, a 32 channel, four shank silicon probe (Buz32, Neuronexus, Ann Arbor, MI) was lowered into barrel cortex, with an estimated tip depth of 375–720 μm from the pia. Prior to insertion, probes were painted with DiI. Following recordings, mouse brains were fixed, stained for cytochrome oxidase and tangentially sectioned to determine the location of the shanks within the barrel field. Shanks within 300 μm of the center of C2 were included for analysis for slip-off responses (two animals, three behavioral sessions, six shanks). Following common signal subtraction, bandpass filtering between 300 and 6,000 Hz, spike extraction of 4 s.d. threshold crossing, and spike merging, multiunit responses were aligned to whisker behavioral events. Spikes with peaks <307.5 μs jitter on the same shank were considered a single spike. Each multiunit was the sum of activity on all eight electrodes on a single shank (six total multiunit recordings). Slip-off events were rare (17 total in three sessions) compared to detach without slip-off. Significance was calculated as unpaired two-tailed t-tests on the difference between the number of spikes in the period 10–30 ms post event and the 50 ms prior to event normalized to the respective period lengths followed by Bonferroni–Holm correction for multiple comparisons.
References
-
Temporal frequency of whisker movement. II. Laminar organization of cortical representationsJ Neurophysiol 86:354–367.
-
Tactile guidance of prey capture in Etruscan shrewsProc Natl Acad Sci USA 103:16544–16549.https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0605573103
-
Flow of excitation within rat barrel cortex on striking a single vibrissaJ Neurosci 68:1345–1354.
-
Biometric analysis of vibrissal tactile discrimination in the ratJ Neurosci 10:2638–2648.
-
Automated tracking of whiskers in videos of head fixed rodentsPLOS Comput Biol 8:e1002591.https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1002591
-
Texture sensation through the fingertips and the whiskersCurr Opin Neurobiol 20:319–327.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2010.03.004
-
AUTO: a program for the automatic bifurcation analysis of autonomous systemsCongressus Numerantium 30:265–284.
-
Simulating, analyzing, and animating dynamical systems: a guide to XPPAUT1st ed., Society for Industrial and Applied Mathematics, 10.1137/1.9780898718195.
-
BookEneström number 65: methodus inveniendi lineas curvas maximi minimive proprietate gaudentes, sive solutio problematis isoperimetrici lattissimo sensu accepti. Marcum-Michaelem bousquet and sociosGeneva: Switzerland.
-
Mechanical characteristics of rat vibrissae: resonant frequencies and damping in isolated whiskers and in the awake behaving animalJ Neurosci 23:6510–6519.
-
Measurement of Young’s modulus and Poisson’s ratio of human hair using optical techniquesProc. SPIE 7522, Fourth International Conference on Experimental Mechanics, 75222Q, 10.1117/12.851415.
-
The sensory contribution of a single vibrissa’s cortical barrelJ Neurophysiol 56:1196–1223.
-
The growth of rats and mice vibrissae under normal and some abnormal conditionsJ Embryol Exp Morphol 33:831–844.
-
Behavioral properties of the trigeminal somatosensory system in rats performing whisker-dependent tactile discriminationsJ Neurosci 21:5752–5763.
-
Vibrissa resonance as a transduction mechanism for tactile encodingJ Neurosci 23:6499–6509.
-
Response properties of vibrissa units in rat SI somatosensory neocortexJ Neurophysiol 41:798–820.
-
BookNonlinear dynamics and chaos: with applications to physics, biology, chemistry and engineeringReading, MA: Adisson-Wessley.
-
The function of vibrissae in the behavior of the white ratBehavior Monographs 1:1–82.
-
Unsupervised whisker tracking in unrestrained behaving animalsJ Neurophysiol 100:504–515.https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00012.2008
Article and author information
Author details
Funding
Howard Hughes Medical Institute
- Samuel Andrew Hires
- Lorenz Pammer
- Karel Svoboda
- David Golomb
Israel Science Foundation (88/13)
- David Golomb
The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.
Acknowledgements
The authors thank Mitra Hartmann, Brian Quist, Lucie Huet and Tansu Celikel for useful discussions, Diego Gutnisky, Nick Sofroniew and Daniel O’Connor for comments on our manuscript.
Ethics
Animal experimentation: This study was performed in strict accordance with the recommendations in the Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals of the National Institutes of Health. All procedures were in accordance with protocol 08–42 approved by the Janelia Farm Research Campus Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee. All surgical procedures were performed under isoflurane anesthesia, and every effort was made to minimize suffering including administration of buprenorphine, ketoprofen and marcaine during and after surgery.
Copyright
© 2013, Hires et al.
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
-
- 1,645
- views
-
- 248
- downloads
-
- 60
- citations
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.
Download links
Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)
Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)
Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)
Further reading
-
- Developmental Biology
- Neuroscience
Williams syndrome (WS; OMIM#194050) is a rare disorder, which is caused by the microdeletion of one copy of 25–27 genes, and WS patients display diverse neuronal deficits. Although remarkable progresses have been achieved, the mechanisms for these distinct deficits are still largely unknown. Here, we have shown that neural progenitor cells (NPCs) in WS forebrain organoids display abnormal proliferation and differentiation capabilities, and synapse formation. Genes with altered expression are related to neuronal development and neurogenesis. Single cell RNA-seq (scRNA-seq) data analysis revealed 13 clusters in healthy control and WS organoids. WS organoids show an aberrant generation of excitatory neurons. Mechanistically, the expression of transthyretin (TTR) are remarkably decreased in WS forebrain organoids. We have found that GTF2IRD1 encoded by one WS associated gene GTF2IRD1 binds to TTR promoter regions and regulates the expression of TTR. In addition, exogenous TTR can activate ERK signaling and rescue neurogenic deficits of WS forebrain organoids. Gtf2ird1-deficient mice display similar neurodevelopmental deficits as observed in WS organoids. Collectively, our study reveals critical function of GTF2IRD1 in regulating neurodevelopment of WS forebrain organoids and mice through regulating TTR-ERK pathway.
-
- Cell Biology
- Neuroscience
The assembly and maintenance of neural circuits is crucial for proper brain function. Although the assembly of brain circuits has been extensively studied, much less is understood about the mechanisms controlling their maintenance as animals mature. In the olfactory system, the axons of olfactory sensory neurons (OSNs) expressing the same odor receptor converge into discrete synaptic structures of the olfactory bulb (OB) called glomeruli, forming a stereotypic odor map. The OB projection neurons, called mitral and tufted cells (M/Ts), have a single dendrite that branches into a single glomerulus, where they make synapses with OSNs. We used a genetic method to progressively eliminate the vast majority of M/T cells in early postnatal mice, and observed that the assembly of the OB bulb circuits proceeded normally. However, as the animals became adults the apical dendrite of remaining M/Ts grew multiple branches that innervated several glomeruli, and OSNs expressing single odor receptors projected their axons into multiple glomeruli, disrupting the olfactory sensory map. Moreover, ablating the M/Ts in adult animals also resulted in similar structural changes in the projections of remaining M/Ts and axons from OSNs. Interestingly, the ability of these mice to detect odors was relatively preserved despite only having 1–5% of projection neurons transmitting odorant information to the brain, and having highly disrupted circuits in the OB. These results indicate that a reduced number of projection neurons does not affect the normal assembly of the olfactory circuit, but induces structural instability of the olfactory circuitry of adult animals.