Communication: Listening in
Many vertebrate species communicate with sounds. These sounds can range from simple innate calls, such as monkey alarm calls, to complex sequences of sounds that need to be learned, such as bird song and human speech. However, it has been challenging to study these more complex sounds under natural conditions because, for many years, recording technology has not been good enough to record multiple animals at once. Thus, researchers often resorted to using a combination of a single microphone and constant visual observation to study one, two or three animals at a time.
Now, in eLife, Lisa Gill, Wolfgang Goymann, Andries Ter Maat and Manfred Gahr of the Max Plank Institute for Ornithology report how backpacks containing miniature wireless microphones can be mounted onto the backs of zebra finches and then used to listen in as these songbirds interact with each other (Gill et al., 2015). A typical zebra finch is the size of a mouse and weighs about 15 grams, so the backpack had to be light and attached in way that did not prevent the bird from flying or copulating. The backpack, which weighed about 1 gram, was attached so that the microphone faced the animal, whereas the antenna that sent the signal to the recording apparatus pointed away from the bird (see image). The Max Planck team also developed software that automatically categorizes vocal interactions between two or more animals.
Prior to this study, the best way to record multiple animals in the same location was to use an array of microphones, followed by sophisticated sorting software to identify which animals were vocalizing, as was recently done with mice (Neunuebel et al., 2015). Last year Alexei Vyssotski and co-workers in Moscow and Zurich used microphones attached to zebra finches for the first time (Anisimov et al., 2014): however, the data were stored on a chip in the backpack, and had to be manually uploaded to a computer later, rather than being transmitted to the computer wirelessly in real time as done by Gill et al.
The subsequent experiment designed by Gill et al. is reminiscent of a reality television show. Imagine having a group of single young men living in a house for an extended period time, and a group of single young women living in a different house. Then select four of the men and four of the women, attach wireless microphones to them, move them to a large house in the tropics with four bedrooms, and record every word they say for 20 days. This is what was done with the birds, except that the house was a large, hot and humid aviary, and the rooms were nest boxes, provided with nest material nearby. No rules were enforced on the animals, so they were free to wander, socialize, mate and fight as they wished.
Gill et al. found that the communications between the birds changed over the course of the 20 days, partly depending on who paired with whom. At first all the birds produced a flurry of long distance calls, and each bird responded a lot to all other birds. After several days, as soon as they found the nest material, individual birds started to form opposite-sex pairs, communicating with each other much more (by a factor of five or six) than with the other birds. The birds in a pair also tended to use ‘cackle and whine’ calls to communicate with each other, but not with other birds. ‘Tet’ calls were produced more often by males during unpaired and non-nesting social contexts, whereas ‘stack’ calls were produced more often by females during unpaired and nest defense contexts.
A remarkable finding was that after 15–20 days, the male–female pairs that used the same call types to communicate with each other were more successful in laying and incubating eggs. Those that communicated more often with different call types were unsuccessful mates. These findings indicate that proper communication is important for forming successful pair bonds and producing offspring, and show that there is more to the chatter of birds than randomly produced calls.
Relatively few animals are capable of vocal learning: those that are include songbirds, parrots, hummingbirds, humans, bats, elephants, dolphins and seals (Petkov and Jarvis, 2012). In zebra finches, as in many other songbirds that live in temperate climates, males have the ability to learn how to produce novel vocalizations, whereas females have lost the this ability (Odom et al., 2014) for both song and calls (Simpson and Vicario, 1990). However, the results of Gill et al. could mean that although female zebra finches cannot imitate new sounds, they still learn when and where to produce their innate sounds through social learning.
The results of the Max Planck team might stimulate further research on other species that vocalize, including non-human primates such as marmosets, macaques and chimpanzees: both the males and females of these species can only produce innate vocalizations but, like female zebra finches, they are capable of learning when and where to produce these sounds through social experience (Petkov and Jarvis, 2012; Takahashi et al., 2015). In the long term this combination of wireless technology and sophisticated software may help us learn more about the rules that govern vocal communication in animals.
References
-
Reconstruction of vocal interactions in a group of small songbirdsNature Methods 11:1135–1137.https://doi.org/10.1038/nmeth.3114
-
Female song is widespread and ancestral in songbirdsNature Communications 5:3379.https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms4379
-
Birds, primates, and spoken language origins: behavioral phenotypes and neurobiological substratesFrontiers in Evolutionary Neuroscience 4:12.https://doi.org/10.3389/fnevo.2012.00012
-
Brain pathways for learned and unlearned vocalizations differ in zebra finchesJournal of Neuroscience 10:1541–1556.
Article and author information
Author details
Publication history
Copyright
© 2015, Jarvis
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use and redistribution provided that the original author and source are credited.
Metrics
-
- 961
- views
-
- 86
- downloads
-
- 0
- citations
Views, downloads and citations are aggregated across all versions of this paper published by eLife.
Download links
Downloads (link to download the article as PDF)
Open citations (links to open the citations from this article in various online reference manager services)
Cite this article (links to download the citations from this article in formats compatible with various reference manager tools)
Further reading
-
- Ecology
- Microbiology and Infectious Disease
Interspecies interactions involving direct competition via bacteriocin production play a vital role in shaping ecological dynamics within microbial ecosystems. For instance, the ribosomally produced siderophore bacteriocins, known as class IIb microcins, affect the colonization of host-associated pathogenic Enterobacteriaceae species. Notably, to date, only five of these antimicrobials have been identified, all derived from specific Escherichia coli and Klebsiella pneumoniae strains. We hypothesized that class IIb microcin production extends beyond these specific compounds and organisms. With a customized informatics-driven approach, screening bacterial genomes in public databases with BLAST and manual curation, we have discovered 12 previously unknown class IIb microcins in seven additional Enterobacteriaceae species, encompassing phytopathogens and environmental isolates. We introduce three novel clades of microcins (MccW, MccX, and MccZ), while also identifying eight new variants of the five known class IIb microcins. To validate their antimicrobial potential, we heterologously expressed these microcins in E. coli and demonstrated efficacy against a variety of bacterial isolates, including plant pathogens from the genera Brenneria, Gibbsiella, and Rahnella. Two newly discovered microcins exhibit activity against Gram-negative ESKAPE pathogens, i.e., Acinetobacter baumannii or Pseudomonas aeruginosa, providing the first evidence that class IIb microcins can target bacteria outside of the Enterobacteriaceae family. This study underscores that class IIb microcin genes are more prevalent in the microbial world than previously recognized and that synthetic hybrid microcins can be a viable tool to target clinically relevant drug-resistant pathogens. Our findings hold significant promise for the development of innovative engineered live biotherapeutic products tailored to combat these resilient bacteria.
-
- Ecology
For the first time in any animal, we show that nocturnal bull ants use the exceedingly dim polarisation pattern produced by the moon for overnight navigation. The sun or moon can provide directional information via their position; however, they can often be obstructed by clouds, canopy, or the horizon. Despite being hidden, these bodies can still provide compass information through the polarised light pattern they produce/reflect. Sunlight produces polarised light patterns across the overhead sky as it enters the atmosphere, and solar polarised light is a well-known compass cue for navigating animals. Moonlight produces an analogous pattern, albeit a million times dimmer than sunlight. Here, we show evidence that polarised moonlight forms part of the celestial compass of navigating nocturnal ants. Nocturnal bull ants leave their nest at twilight and rely heavily on the overhead solar polarisation pattern to navigate. Yet many foragers return home overnight when the sun cannot guide them. We demonstrate that these bull ants use polarised moonlight to navigate home during the night, by rotating the overhead polarisation pattern above homing ants, who alter their headings in response. Furthermore, these ants can detect this cue throughout the lunar month, even under crescent moons, when polarised light levels are at their lowest. Finally, we show the long-term incorporation of this moonlight pattern into the ants’ path integration system throughout the night for homing, as polarised sunlight is incorporated throughout the day.